Main

During sleep, the regulation of respiration undergoes important changes in both adults and infants. Sleep is associated with hypotonia of various respiratory muscles such as the genioglossus, masticatory, and deep neck muscles (1), and this can lead to increased upper airway resistance (2) and reduced functional residual capacity (3). Minute ventilation is also decreased during sleep (by approximately 8%–15% in adolescents and adults) when compared with wakefulness (4). With a loss of the wakefulness drive to breathing and the variability of respiratory control between different sleep states, responses to respiratory stimuli are altered during sleep (5); thus, physiologic challenges such as hypoxia, hypercapnia, and airway obstruction can pose a greater risk when asleep than when awake.

The HVR is a key protective mechanism aimed at maintaining adequate blood oxygen levels. In adults, the response consists of a prompt increase in ventilation, which peaks within 3–5 min of exposure to hypoxia, followed by a period of sustained hyperventilation for 15–30 min (6). In contrast, studies in infants have reported an increase in ventilation during the first 1–2 min of hypoxic exposure, followed by a reduction to or below normoxic levels within 5 min, suggesting an immature HVR that is biphasic in nature. This biphasic response has been demonstrated in both term (79) and preterm infants (1015).

Previous studies in infants have primarily been conducted in QS; however, as infants spend the majority of their sleep time in AS, it is important to understand the HVR in this state too. Furthermore, in the majority of studies, only a single HVR test in each infant was performed; only one study made repeated measurements in the same infant (7). Thus, the consistency of the HVR in term infants has not yet been established. Studies in adults have shown a significant variability in individual responses to hypoxia in both rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and non-REM (NREM) sleep (16). In addition, studies in adults have demonstrated that the oxygen saturation at which arousal occurs varies between subjects (16). Arousal is a vital protective mechanism in the event of an inadequate HVR; however, despite the importance of arousal, studies in infants have usually not accounted for arousal responses when investigating the infant HVR.

Our present aim was to investigate the maturation and consistency of the initial phase of the HVR by studying healthy term infants longitudinally over the first 6 mo of life; infants have been repeatedly challenged in both sleep states, and arousal responses have been taken into consideration.

METHODS

Ethical approval for this project was obtained from the Southern Health and Monash University Human Research Ethics Committees. Written informed parental consent was obtained before each investigation. Participation in the study was entirely voluntary with no monetary incentive provided.

Subjects.

Data were collected from a group of 15 healthy term infants recruited from Jessie McPherson Private Hospital and the maternity wards at Monash Medical Centre, Melbourne, Australia. Infants were born at 38–41 wk of gestation (mean 40 ± 0.3 wk, mean ± SEM) with normal birth weights of 3110–4725 g (mean 3549 ± 132 g). Apgar scores were median 9 at 1 min (range 3–9) and median 9 at 5 min (range 6–10). Infants were studied at 2–5 wk (mean 22 ± 0.8 d, range 17–26 d), 2–3 mo (mean 72 ± 1 d, range 68–84 d), and 5–6 mo (mean 164 ± 2 d, range 157–175 d) after birth. None of the mothers reported smoking during pregnancy or after delivery, and this was confirmed with infant urinary cotinine analysis (17).

Sleep diaries were completed by the parent(s) for 2 d preceding each study to ensure that sleep observed in the laboratory was comparable with typical sleep patterns in the home.

Polysomnographic recording variables.

A 16-channel polygraph (Model 78A, Grass Instrument Co., Quincy, MA) was used to record an electroencephalogram, electro-oculogram, submentalis electromyogram, electrocardiogram, and instantaneous heart rate. Thoracic and abdominal breathing movements were recorded using Resp-ez Piezoelectric sensors (EPM Systems, Midlothian, VA); oxygen saturation (Spo2) was recorded from a pulse oximeter attached to the ear (BIOX 3700e, Ohmeda, Louisville, CO); transcutaneous carbon dioxide was recorded using a carbon dioxide electrode (Model E5280, Radiometer, Copenhagen, Denmark). Abdominal skin temperature was also recorded (YSI 400 Series Thermistor, Mallinckrodt, Melbourne, VIC, Australia).

Nasal airflow was measured using a small purpose-built pneumotachograph attached to a specially designed silicone rubber nose mask, as described previously (17,18). Briefly, a continuous flow of medical grade gas was passed across the pneumotachograph at a constant rate of 5 L/min, producing a pressure of 0.25 cm H2O within the mask. Expired carbon dioxide (Petco2) was monitored at a sampling rate of 100 mL/min. A differential transducer attached to the pneumotachograph was electronically balanced to account for the bias flow and capnograph gas withdrawal.

Study protocol.

Polysomnography was performed between 1000 and 1600 h. Environmental temperature was maintained at 21–24°C throughout the studies, while noise and light levels were kept to a minimum. Following electrode attachment, infants were placed supine in a bassinet to sleep. Sleep state was determined as AS, QS, or indeterminate sleep according to standard criteria (19); data from indeterminate sleep were excluded from analyses.

At the beginning of each sleep period, after application of the mask, a 2-min control period ensured the infant was settled into stable sleep and provided baseline cardiorespiratory values. A further 1-min control period was obtained before each hypoxic test and provided baseline cardiorespiratory and respiratory gas values. Infants were challenged with hypoxia by using a custom-drilled three-way tap (Swagelock, OH) to change the inspired gas supply from air (21% O2) to 15% O2 (balance N2). Tests were terminated under three conditions: (1) when the infant aroused, (2) after 5 min with no arousal, or (3) if Spo2 fell below 85%. Replicate tests (n ≥ 3) were obtained in each sleep state. Between successive challenges, recovery periods of at least 2 min allowed the infant to reestablish normal respiration.

Tests were not performed during periodic breathing; tests that induced periodic breathing were continued until one of the termination criteria was met, but were not included in analyses. Data from tests which were terminated when Spo2 fell to ≤85% were also excluded from further analyses.

Arousal criteria.

Subcortical activation from sleep was determined according to four criteria, with the simultaneous presence of at least three required to designate an arousal response (2022). This classification of arousal is consistent with the recent definition by the International Paediatric Work Group on Arousals (23). Based on appropriate responses to avoid or overcome the stimulus, these criteria were (1) a change in both amplitude and frequency of the ventilatory pattern for more than two breaths, (2) an observed behavioral response (usually turning the head away from the stimulus), (3) a heart rate acceleration of >10% above baseline, and (4) an increase in submental electromyographic activity. The 10 s of recording immediately preceding hypoxia-induced arousals provided baseline data to assess the degree of change in each physiologic variable.

Data analysis.

HVR tests lasting at least 30 s were analyzed breath by breath, and mean values for Spo2 and V˙I/kg were calculated in 15-s epochs for the initial 30 s of each hypoxia test and expressed as the percentage of changes relative to baseline values. This initial 30-s period was chosen for analysis to maximize the data in AS tests, which were invariably terminated by arousal. To assess the variability in V˙I/kg between infants, data from tests repeated in each infant were averaged for each sleep state, with arousing and nonarousing tests analyzed separately and standard deviations (SDs) of each group were compared using F tests to investigate the effects of sleep state, the arousal response, and postnatal age. To assess the response variability observed within an infant, the difference between values of V˙I/kg from repeated measurements (n ≥ 2) was calculated. The effects of sleep state and postnatal age were examined with a t test and one-way analysis of variance, respectively.

RESULTS

A total of 254 hypoxic challenges was performed (115 in AS and 139 in QS). Of all tests, 54 in AS and 86 in QS provided suitable data for analyses of initial ventilatory responses. In response to hypoxia, infants consistently aroused from AS; however, in QS, infants both aroused and failed to arouse (17). In QS, the data have been grouped into arousal and nonarousal responses. Total sleep time recorded in the laboratory was not different from that recorded at home (when matched for time of day), except at 5–6 mo of age, when infants slept longer in the laboratory (91 ± 6 versus 66 ± 8 min, p < 0.05) (17,18).

Ventilatory Responses to Hypoxia

At each age, mild hypoxia induced a significant decrease in Spo2 in both sleep states, regardless of whether the infant aroused. In individual infants who aroused from sleep, Spo2 fell at a similar rate over the first 30 s of hypoxia in both AS and QS, as illustrated for the 2–5 wk study (Fig. 1A and C). The V˙I/kg response, however was less consistent, with some infants exhibiting an increase, a decrease, or no change in V˙I/kg in response to hypoxia; furthermore, this variation appeared to be increased in AS compared with QS (Fig. 1B and D).

Figure 1
figure 1

Individual infant responses to 15% O2 at 2–5 wk postnatal age during AS (n = 10) and QS (n = 11). The percentage of change in Spo 2 after 15 s and 30 s of hypoxia in AS (A) and QS (B) sleep. The percentage of change in V˙I/kg after 15 s and 30 s of hypoxia in AS (C) and QS (D). Note the variability of V˙I/kg between infants, particularly in AS.

Variability of V˙I/kg Responses Between Infants

Effects of sleep state.

The variability of ventilation (SD of V˙I/kg) during the first 15 s of hypoxia was significantly increased in AS compared with QS when infants aroused at all three ages studied (p < 0.05). Similarly, after 30 s of hypoxia, V˙I/kg variability also tended to be greater in AS; however, this only reached statistical significance at 2–5 wk postnatal age (p < 0.05) (Fig 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Effects of sleep state and arousal on the variability between infants of the percentage of change in V˙I/kg following 15 s (solid columns) and 30 s (cross-hatched columns) of hypoxia during AS (black columns), and QS when infants aroused (open columns) or failed to arouse (shaded columns). *p < 0.05.

When arousal and nonarousal responses were compared after 15 s of hypoxia in QS V˙I/kg, variability was greater when infants aroused at 2–3 mo (p < 0.05) and less at 5–6 mo (p < 0.05), and at 2–5 wk, there was no difference. After 30 s of hypoxia, there was no difference between the variability of V˙I/kg responses associated with arousal or nonarousal responses at any age.

Effects of postnatal age.

At both 2–5 wk (p < 0.05) and 2–3 mo (p < 0.05), the variability of V˙I/kg after 15 s of hypoxia was significantly greater than at 5–6 mo in AS. When arousal occurred during QS, the variation of V˙I/kg between infants also decreased with postnatal age, reaching significance at 5–6 mo (p < 0.05). In contrast, when hypoxia failed to induce arousal in QS, the ventilatory responses after 15 s were significantly less variable at 2–3 mo when compared with the other two ages studied (p < 0.05) (Fig. 3).

Figure 3
figure 3

Effects of postnatal age on the variability between infants of the percentage of change in V˙I/kg following 15 s (solid columns) and 30 s (cross-hatched columns) of hypoxia at 2–5 wk (solid columns), 2–3 mo (open columns) and 5–6 mo (shaded columns) of postnatal age. *p < 0.05.

There were no effects of postnatal age on the variation in the HVR between infants after 30 s of hypoxia.

Reproducibility of the Ventilatory Response in Individual Infants

Effects of sleep state.

For this analysis, only infants with two or more tests in each sleep state at a given age were included. As few infants failed to arouse from QS more than twice at a given age (2–5 wk, n = 0; 2–3 mo, n = 2; 5–6 mo, n = 3), the QS arousal and nonarousal data have been combined. There were no significant differences in the variability of the HVR between data obtained in QS associated with arousal alone or with data from QS when arousal/nonarousal data were combined (Fig. 4).

Figure 4
figure 4

Effects of sleep state on within-infant variability of the percentage of change in V˙I/kg following repeated hypoxic test after 15 s (solid columns) and 30 s (cross-hatched columns) of hypoxia in AS (solid columns) and QS (open columns). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01. Note that arousal and nonarousal data in QS were combined for this analysis.

At 2–5 wk postnatal age, the variability between replicate V˙I/kg measurements was significantly greater in AS compared with QS at both 15 s (p < 0.05) and 30 s (p < 0.01). Although this trend was also observed at 2–3 mo and 5–6 mo, the differences did not reach statistical significance.

Effects of postnatal age.

I/kg differences between replicate tests did not differ significantly with postnatal age in either sleep state.

Habituation of Ventilatory and Arousal Responses

There was no evidence of habituation to repeated hypoxic tests in either sleep state. An example of test-to-test variation for one infant at 2–5 wk is presented in Figure 5; arousal and ventilatory responses of each infant were not progressive and appeared to be random; that is, neither arousal latency nor V˙I/kg increased or decreased progressively with the number of repeated tests.

Figure 5
figure 5

Effects of repeated hypoxic tests on arousal and ventilatory responses during AS and QS in infants (n = 5) at 2–5 wk postnatal age. Arousal latency in AS (A) and QS (B). The percentage of change in V˙I/kg in AS (C) and QS (D). Note that there is no consistent change in responsiveness as measured by either arousal latency or percentage of change in V˙I/kg.

DISCUSSION

This is the first study to investigate the variability of the initial phase of the HVR between and within term infants over the first 6 mo of life. The testing of infants in both AS and QS and the separate analysis of responses in QS that either induced arousal or in which arousal failed to occur are also novel aspects of this study. Our study has demonstrated that the initial phase of the HVR varies between term infants during both AS and QS, with responses being markedly more variable during AS, and becoming more consistent with increasing postnatal age. Ventilatory and arousal responses also vary in an individual infant, but do not show any habituation to repeated tests. We suggest that by performing only one test or by failing to account for arousal responses, previous studies may not have detected the natural variation of the infant HVR.

Our study reveals considerable variation both between and within infants in both hypoxic ventilatory and arousal responses at all three ages studied. In the only other study to perform repeated hypoxic tests in term infants, Cohen et al. (7) also demonstrated a significant variability in the reproducibility of tests. They reported that less than half of the duplicate tests in QS produced similar ventilatory responses and suggested that QS was not a homogeneous state. Our study has expanded on these findings by studying infants in both AS and QS and also longitudinally over the first 6 mo of life. Our data are supported by those for adults in which repeated hypoxic tests in both NREM and REM sleep have also demonstrated significant variability in both ventilatory and arousal responses (16).

Our finding that HVRs were more variable in AS compared with QS may be another reflection of the physiologic variability of this sleep state. In infants, cardiorespiratory disturbances occur predominantly in AS. Therefore, given that AS is the dominant sleep state in infancy, infants may be at greater risk of cardiorespiratory instability in this state (25).

Previous studies by other groups of the HVR in term infants have been conducted only in QS. Recently we showed that, in infants challenged by mild hypoxia, arousal is always elicited in AS, whereas in QS, the same infant both aroused and failed to arouse to tests in QS (17); we also found that the arousal latency is also longer in QS. These findings are consistent with other studies using different respiratory stimuli (26,27) and somatosensory stimuli (21,22,28,29). Our finding that infants frequently fail to arouse in QS has previously been reported by us (30) and others (3138), and the novel finding that infants always aroused in AS highlights the importance of arousal in this state. Arousal from sleep is accompanied by an increase in heart rate, arterial pressure, and ventilation (39), and, importantly, a behavioral response is evoked, allowing body movements aimed at avoiding the life-threatening challenge (40). Arousal from sleep has been described as a vital protective mechanism against cardiorespiratory failure (40), and depressed arousal responses have been implicated in sudden infant death sysndrome (SIDS) (41).

Our study did not reveal any habituation to repeated tests of either ventilatory or arousal responses; this is consistent with previous findings from our group using nasal air-jet stimulation (28). In contrast, there is significant habituation in infants to repeated tactile stimuli, and this occurred more rapidly in REM sleep (42). Habituation to a nonthreatening stimulus such as tactile stimulation of the foot, as used in this study, may be advantageous to preserve sleep continuity; however, habituation to a life-threatening stimulus such as hypoxia could have serious consequences for the individual and would certainly be maladaptive. It has been suggested by some authors that SIDS may result from a depression of ventilatory or arousal responses to repeated episodes of hypoxia (43); however, our study has shown that infants do not appear to habituate to small numbers (fewer than five) of hypoxic challenges.

In summary, we have shown that repeated hypoxic tests in both AS and QS do not result in habituation and provide a more accurate measure of individual responses to hypoxia that are variable.