Main

There is considerable evidence from animal experiments that early exposure to stress, either in the antenatal or in the immediate postnatal periods, can have long term effects on both the function of the HPA axis, and on behavior (1–5). In general, the HPA axis of the offspring becomes hyper-responsive to a novel stressor. These effects are mediated, at least in part, by activation of the maternal HPA axis and the effect of fetal cortisol or corticosterone (in rodents) on the developing brain. These effects can, in some paradigms, be mimicked by synthetic glucocorticoids (6–8). Until recently, this has been little studied in humans. It does appear that cortisol from the mother crosses the placenta in sufficient amounts to affect the fetus (9,10), although there is so far no direct evidence that maternal cortisol alters infant development. However, there is increasing evidence from epidemiologic studies that maternal stress and anxiety during pregnancy has an adverse effect on the development of the fetus and on the later behavior and cognitive development of the child (11–16).

Preterm babies are exposed both to a stressful environment, with concomitant endogenous glucocorticoid release, and may also be exposed, antenatally and postnatally, to exogenous glucocorticoids used therapeutically. If the animal models are relevant, one would predict that these infants would show long-term effects, both in HPA axis response and in behavior. Babies born extremely preterm are at risk of later behavioral problems, most noticeably with attention deficit disorders (17). Recent evidence suggests that antenatal glucocorticoid exposure has long-term effects on brain development (18). Davis et al. (19) have shown preterm babies exposed to antenatal betamethasone had a lower saliva cortisol response to a heel stick than matched controls at 3-6 d after delivery. Ng et al. (20) have also shown that one or two doses of antenatal corticosteroids were associated with a suppressed cortisol response to a CRH challenge at d 7 but not d 14. However, the longer-term effects of exogenous glucocorticoids and that of endogenously generated cortisol in the preterm infant have not been studied. The aim of this study was to determine the plasma level of cortisol in a cohort of preterm infants, and to determine whether the level of the endogenous and exogenous glucocorticoids during the neonatal period predicted subsequent cortisol responses to a standardized stressor.

To test the possible mechanisms underlying the association between endogenous and exogenous glucocorticoids in the perinatal period and later stress responses a path model was developed. This considered antenatal and postnatal steroids as separate possible influences on later stress responses, and also as possible influences on mean cortisol levels in the postnatal period. The sex of the child, gestational age, and birth weight were also considered as independent predictors of stress responses either directly or via their impact on postnatal glucocorticoids.

METHODS

Singleton infants born below 32 wk gestation without major immediate life-threatening congenital abnormalities, receiving care in the neonatal intensive care units at Hammersmith and Queen Charlotte's Hospitals, London, were eligible for recruitment into a trial examining the effects of mother-infant skin-to-skin contact on a range of infant and maternal outcomes. These hospitals were situated 5 miles apart in inner London and serve a multicultural urban population. They are under the management of a single National Health Service Healthcare Trust. The maternity and neonatal service is a referral center for high-risk patients. The neonatal service is provided by a single pediatric department and medical and nursing protocols for the two neonatal units are identical. The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Imperial College Faculty of Medicine and Hammersmith Hospitals Trust and written maternal consent was obtained. The trial showed that there was no effect of the skin-to-skin intervention on plasma cortisol or later salivary cortisol baseline or response levels, or any other outcome measure and so data from all the infants were combined. The results of this trial are to be reported elsewhere.

Here we report data on circulating cortisol levels in the first 4 postnatal weeks, and salivary cortisol responses to the standardised stress of 4 and 12 mo immunizations (n = 45). All subjects participated in these evaluations.

Illness severity.

Preterm babies in intensive care have a variety of influences on their wellbeing. We used the number of days the infant received level 1 (full) intensive care according to predefined national criteria (21) as an index of illness severity.

Antenatal and postnatal steroids.

Most of the mothers were given either betamethasone (n = 31; range, 1–16 doses) or dexamethasone (n = 5; range, 1–4 doses) or both (n = 5) antenatally. Nine of the babies were given dexamethasone postnatally (range, 7–39 doses).

Blood samples.

Basal plasma cortisol level during wk 1–4 post birth was measured in a once weekly 0.5-mL blood sample and in residual plasma volumes left over from samples taken at routine clinical samples. These samples could have been obtained on any day of the specified week. If more than one sample was obtained during a particular week, as was generally the case, the results were meaned. Blood samples were collected in heparinized vacutainers, and spun at 3000 g for 15 min. The plasma aliquot was removed and frozen at –80°C in polypropylene storage vials until assay.

Saliva samples.

Infants were recalled to hospital to receive routine 4- and 12-mo immunizations in accordance with the UK national schedule. Immunizations were administered at standard times (between 1200 and 1600 h at 4 mo; between 1000 and 1200 h at 12 mo) to control for possible diurnal variation. Saliva samples were obtained immediately before and 20 min after injection (the time taken for cortisol to rise after acute stress). Fruit juices, sweet drinks and milk feeds were avoided for 30 min before immunization. Saliva was collected into salivettes and stored frozen until assay

Cortisol assays.

These were carried out using Coat-A-Count DPC RIA (Diagnostic Products, Los Angeles, CA).

Data analyses.

Each variable was tested for the normality of its distribution. Mean plasma cortisol wk 1–4 was log transformed to achieve a normal distribution for statistical analyses. (Values in the text are given in geometric means to show actual concentrations.) Univariate and multivariate correlations were initially sought between the level of endogenous cortisol during the neonatal period and the pre minus post immunization salivary cortisol responses (Δcortisol) to 4- and 12-mo immunization. Path analysis (22) was used to test a model of the putative causal relationships between factors influencing Δcortisol at 4 mo. The final model reported is that which retains only significant paths. Its fit to the data (the measured covariances between the variables) is estimated using χ2, comparative fit index (CFI), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). A model shows good fit if χ2 is not significant, the CFI is >0.90 and the RMSEA is <0.08 (22).

RESULTS

Infant ages for the first immunization assessment were chronological age 21.7 ± 4.0 wk (mean ± SD), corrected age 9.4 ± 4.4 wk; and for the second assessment, chronological age 55.3 ± 5.6, corrected age 42.8 ± 4.1 wk.

Figure 1 shows the individual infant plasma cortisol levels during wk 1–4. The geometric means for plasma cortisol during the first 4 wk were 358, 314, 231, and 195 nmol/L, respectively.

Figure 1
figure 1

Mean plasma cortisol for each baby in the study (n = 45) for each of the first 4 wk.

Cortisol responses to inoculation at 4 mo were available on 45 infants. Infant characteristics are shown in Table 1. Although there was a clear saliva cortisol response to inoculation at 4 mo (p < 0.001 paired t test), the mean response was much reduced by 12 mo (p = 0.006 paired t test for deltas at the two time points).

Table 1 Characteristics of babies in the study and their saliva cortisol values (n = 45)

Most of these babies spent some of the first 4 wk in full level 1 intensive care (Table 1). Although there was no correlation between the time spent and mean plasma cortisol weeks 1–4 in the whole group, there was a significant difference between those who spent less than a week (n = 10), geometric mean, 243 nmol/L, and the rest, 335 nmol/L (p = 0.014).

An initial correlation analysis (Table 2) showed that the saliva cortisol response to inoculation at 4 mo was significantly correlated with both the mean plasma cortisol in the first 4 wk and to birthweight SDS (Child Growth Foundation, London, UK). There was no correlation between baseline saliva cortisol at 4 mo and mean plasma cortisol wk 1–4. Also, there was no significant correlation between baseline saliva cortisols at 4 and 12 mo or between the Δcortisols at the two time points. There was a significant positive association between antenatal steroids and baseline cortisol at 4 mo. The correlation patterns were similar whether total maternal antenatal steroid exposure or dexamethasone or betamethasone were used separately. There was no correlation between antenatal and postnatal exogenous steroid exposure.

Table 2 Univariate correlation coefficients between predictors and Δcortisol and baseline cortisol

A multivariate analysis (Table 3) confirmed the independent contribution of birth weight SDS and mean plasma cortisol wk 1–4 to Δcortisol at 4 mo, but also showed an additional negative association with therapeutic antenatal maternal steroids. This analysis showed that nearly half the variance in Δcortisol can be predicted by these three factors (R 2 = 0.46). Males tended to have a larger response (see Table 3).

Table 3 Simultaneous entry of predictors of Δcortisol at 4 mo (R 2 = 45%)

A path model was developed where the paths between variables were based on their relationships over time (Fig. 2). The paths retained are all significant and there are no additional significance factors. This model fits the data well (χ2 = 16.34, df = 14, NS; CFI = 0.934; RMSEA = 0.062 (CI.90 0.000–0.163). The model shows that the postnatal steroids have a dual contribution to Δcortisol, a direct positive predictive effect together with a negative effect on mean cortisol wk 1–4. This might explain why their effect was missed in the previous analyses. The path analysis confirms that antenatal steroids, postnatal steroids, plasma cortisol wk 1–4, birth weight SDS, and gestational age all independently predicted Δcortisol at 4 mo.

Figure 2
figure 2

Path analysis showing significant determinants of the Δcortisol response to inoculation at 4 mo. Birth weight indicates birth weight SDS.

DISCUSSION

This study confirms that preterm babies are exposed to high levels of endogenous cortisol (23). The levels found here were four to seven times higher than that documented in fetuses at equivalent gestational ages (9,10). Given the extensive evidence from animal experiments (5,6), it is a plausible hypothesis that this may have long-term effects on the human HPA axis.

It is well recognized that some preterm babies manifest a hypoadrenal response (24). However, none of the infants in this study showed any overt manifestation suggestive of this, such as intractable hypotension responsive only to glucocorticoid therapy. We did find a wide range of basal cortisol values (from 110 to 1282 nmol/L in wk 1, and from 35 to 683 nmol/L in wk 4). There was also a wide range of cortisol responses to inoculation at 4 mo (from –11.1 to 27.9 nmol/L).

We used both blood and saliva for cortisol assays. Other studies have shown good correlation between plasma and saliva cortisol levels (25). Although the blood samples were collected at varying times of day, preterm infants of this postnatal age, unlike adults, do not exhibit diurnal variation in cortisol production (26). The nature of the method for blood collection (needle or indwelling catheter) would not be expected to have any effect on cortisol levels as the cortisol response to an invasive procedure takes at least 10–20 min to develop.

The results presented here show that there is a positive association between the endogenous cortisol response during the first 4 wk and the infant saliva cortisol response to a stressor 4 mo later. The effect of postnatal steroids was similar to that of the postnatal endogenous cortisol. However, the effects shown in the path analysis are complex; antenatal steroids had a negative correlation with the infants' saliva cortisol response at 4 mo. The negative relationship between antenatal steroids and the saliva cortisol response of preterm babies is similar to that reported in other studies with infants studied in the first week (19,20). Antenatal steroids were also associated with a raised baseline cortisol level at 4 mo, which was in turn link with a reduced response to stress (Table 2). Lower birth weight SDS was also associated with a reduced response to immunization at 4 mo. Low birth weight for gestational age can be indicative of fetal stress and also be associated with higher endogenous fetal glucocorticoid levels (27). The effect of antenatal therapeutic steroids thus has some similarities to high antenatal endogenous levels (as indicated by low birth weight for gestation), and postnatal therapeutic steroids appear similar in effect to postnatal endogenous cortisol. Why responses to antenatal and postnatal exposure should differ is not clear and needs further confirmation and exploration. It is unlikely to be due solely to maturational stage, given the overlap in gestational age at which antenatal and postnatal exposure occurred. It presumably reflects other differences in the intrauterine and postnatal environmental milieu.

Grunau et al. (28) have shown that basal saliva cortisol at 8 mo corrected age, was higher in babies born at <29 wk compared with those born at 29–32 wk or full term. In the current study no association was found between plasma cortisol levels in the first 4 wk and basal cortisol levels at 4 mo or 12 mo. However, there was a nonsignificant trend for those born before 29 wk to have higher basal cortisol levels at 12 mo than those born later.

This cohort of infants had, in general, a substantially diminished saliva cortisol response to immunization by 12 mo (Table 1). In this they resembled full-term babies, in whom this phenomenon of a reduction in saliva cortisol response by one year is well established (29). The reason for this change in responsiveness of the HPA axis in the first year is still not clear. It does not mean that there is no longer-term programming of the HPA axis in humans, but that it may be complex, and will need different methods of investigation from that in younger babies.

It is of interest that preterm infants have a similar spectrum of developmental and behavioral problems to babies whose mothers have been very stressed or anxious antenatally. Both show increased levels of attention deficit and hyperactivity, anxiety and depression (14,17). We have suggested that the long-term effects of preterm stress or anxiety may in part be due, as in the animal models, to exposure of the fetus to increased cortisol (30). Although the most sensitive period to gestational stress in humans is not established, it has been suggested that maternal anxiety at 32 wk has a greater effect than maternal anxiety at 18 wk (14). Thus, exposure in the last trimester of pregnancy may be important, and would coincide with the period of exposure to raised cortisol of preterm infants.

There are obviously multiple potential influences on HPA reactivity in the perinatal period. Our use of path analysis is an attempt to tease out those that are most relevant. The programming hypothesis is strengthened by the effect of postnatal steroids given to the infant in the first 4 wk. These had the expected effect of reducing the plasma cortisol level at the time, but also having a direct independent positive effect on the later response.

Several caveats must be applied to this study. The numbers were small. Preterm babies are a heterogeneous group with many underlying co-morbidities that may affect outcomes. Both antenatal and postnatal exogenous steroids are given for specific reasons, and these could have independent effects on the development of HPA axis responses. Other antenatal stressors to the mother or postnatal to the infant could have independent effects. Variable levels of placental 11β hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase may alter fetal exposure to glucocorticoids. It was not possible to resolve these issues in this study

It is possible that the more reactive infants showed a higher response both early and later. However, given the different degrees of stressful procedures that different infants are exposed to, it seems unlikely that the main determinant of the early plasma cortisol level is genetic. The findings with the effects of the postnatal exogenous glucocorticoids also support a programming hypothesis.

In conclusion, the results presented here suggest that the exposure to high levels of endogenous and exogenous cortisol, experienced by many preterm infants, may have an effect on the later function of their HPA axis. These questions need to be explored further in larger cohorts. Even if these effects only last for the time demonstrated here, up to 4 mo, they could be important for behavioral and other development, including the immune response (31).