Key Points
-
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most prevalent and controversial conditions in child psychiatry. Both clinical progress and basic research will benefit from the identification of endophenotypes — intermediate, quantifiable traits that predict an individual's risk of disease and can be linked to the underlying aetiology of the condition. Such endophenotypes should also be anchored in neuroscience.
-
One of the clearest symptoms of ADHD is locomotor hyperactivity. There is evidence for dopaminergic dysfunction in humans with ADHD and, in animals, hyperactivity can be seen in both hypodopaminergic and hyperdopaminergic models. Striatal abnormalities and alterations in dopamine transporter density have been measured in subjects with ADHD, and psychostimulants (which increase the levels of catecholamines at synapses) are effective in treating the symptoms of the disorder.
-
Subjects with ADHD appear to have a deficit in response inhibition. They also manifest delay aversion, including a tendency to choose a small, immediate reward over a larger, delayed one. This can be modelled as a shortened delay gradient (a decline in the effectiveness of reinforcement as the delay between behaviour and reward increases). At least three brain abnormalities that have been observed in ADHD could potentially lead to this endophenotype: striatal abnormalities, alterations in dopamine transporter expression and reduced volume of the cerebellar vermis.
-
Many data lead us to believe that subjects with ADHD show high response variability and inconsistency in performance. They also show deficits in the perception and reproduction of time intervals. Brain abnormalities that could lead to such a deficit include cerebellar dysfunction, striatal lesions and changes in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) activity in the prefrontal cortex.
-
The last proposed endophenotype is an impairment in working memory, which could be linked to delay aversion, executive dysfunction, inattention and phonemic-awareness deficits in ADHD subjects. Potential brain abnormalities that could affect working memory include striatal lesions, alterations in COMT activity and changes in the electroencephalogram (EEG).
-
The endophenotype approach allows researchers and clinicians to avoid some of the problems associated with the use of symptom scales, and should facilitate large-scale collaborative projects that are aimed at clarifying the complex causes of ADHD.
Abstract
Research on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), a highly prevalent and controversial condition, has, for the most part, been descriptive and atheoretical. The imperative to discover the genetic and environmental risk factors for ADHD is motivating the search for quantifiable intermediate constructs, termed endophenotypes. In this selective review, we conclude that such endophenotypes should be solidly grounded in the neurosciences. We propose that three such endophenotypes — a specific abnormality in reward-related circuitry that leads to shortened delay gradients, deficits in temporal processing that result in high intrasubject intertrial variability, and deficits in working memory — are most amenable to integrative collaborative approaches that aim to uncover the causes of ADHD.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 print issues and online access
$189.00 per year
only $15.75 per issue
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Goldman, L. S., Genel, M., Bezman, R. J. & Slanetz, P. J. Diagnosis and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Council on Scientific Affairs, American Medical Association. JAMA 279, 1100–1107 (1998).
Barbaresi, W. J. et al. How common is attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Incidence in a population-based birth cohort in Rochester, Minn. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 156, 217–224 (2002).
LeFever, G. B., Dawson, K. V. & Morrow, A. L. The extent of drug therapy for attention deficit–hyperactivity disorder among children in public schools. Am. J. Public Health 89, 1359–1364 (1999).
Moll, G. H., Hause, S., Ruther, E., Rothenberger, A. & Huether, G. Early methylphenidate administration to young rats causes a persistent reduction in the density of striatal dopamine transporters. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 11, 15–24 (2001).One of the few studies to have examined the effects of stimulants in developing animals. The authors found enduring changes in striatal DATs only when chronic low-dose exposure to methylphenidate was started in young rats (day 25).
Zito, J. M. et al. Trends in the prescribing of psychotropic medications to preschoolers. JAMA 283, 1025–1030 (2000).
Morton, J. & Frith, U. in Developmental Psychopathology (eds Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J.) 357–390 (John Wiley, New York, 1995).
Almasy, L. & Blangero, J. Endophenotypes as quantitative risk factors for psychiatric disease: rationale and study design. Am. J. Med. Genet. 105, 42–44 (2001).
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, Washington DC, 1994).
Swanson, J. M. et al. Seminar: attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and hyperkinetic disorder. Lancet 351, 429–433 (1998).
Landgren, M., Kjellman, B. & Gillberg, C. Deficits in attention, motor control and perception (DAMP): a simplified school entry examination. Acta Paediatr. 89, 302–309 (2000).
Sonuga-Barke, E. J. Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD — a dual pathway model of behaviour and cognition. Behav. Brain Res. 130, 29–36 (2002).
Barkley, R. A. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol. Bull. 121, 65–94 (1997).
Sergeant, J., Oosterlaan, J. & Van der Meere, J. in Handbook of Disruptive Behavior Disorders (eds Quay, H. C. & Hogan, A. E.) 75–104 (Plenum, New York, 1999).
Nigg, J. T. Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? Psychol. Bull. 127, 571–598 (2001).
Solanto, M. V., Arnsten, A. F. T. & Castellanos, F. X. (eds) Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2001).
Rutter, M. & Silberg, J. Gene–environment interplay in relation to emotional and behavioral disturbance. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 463–490 (2002).
Smalley, S. L. Genetic influences in childhood-onset psychiatric disorders: autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 60, 1276–1282 (1997).
Tannock, R. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: advances in cognitive, neurobiological, and genetic research. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 39, 65–99 (1998).
Faraone, S. V. & Doyle, A. E. Genetic influences on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2, 143–146 (2000).
Swanson, J. M. et al. Dopamine genes and ADHD. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 21–25 (2000).
Faraone, S. V. Report from the third international meeting of the Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Molecular Genetics Network. Am. J. Med. Genet. 114, 272–276 (2002).
Fisher, S. E. et al. A genomewide scan for loci involved in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 70, 1183–1196 (2002).This first scan in ADHD, using a sample of 126 affected sibling pairs, did not find any genome-wide statistically significant peaks, although several suggestive peaks were identified and are being explored in greater detail with an enlarged sample.
International Molecular Genetic Study of Autism Consortium (IMGSAC). A genomewide screen for autism: strong evidence for linkage to chromosomes 2q, 7q, and 16p. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 69, 570–581 (2001).
Herskovits, E. H. et al. Is the spatial distribution of brain lesions associated with closed-head injury predictive of subsequent development of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Analysis with brain-image database. Radiology 213, 389–394 (1999).
Max, J. E. et al. Putamen lesions and the development of attention-deficit/hyperactivity symptomatology. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 41, 563–571 (2002).
Kreppner, J. M., O'Connor, T. G. & Rutter, M. Can inattention/overactivity be an institutional deprivation syndrome? J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 29, 513–528 (2001).This study compared 165 children that were adopted in the United Kingdom after severe deprivation with 52 within-UK adoptees who had not suffered deprivation. Parent and teacher ratings of enduring inattention/overactivity were found in relationship to the extent of early severe deprivation.
Biederman, J. et al. Family-environment risk factors for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. A test of Rutter's indicators of adversity. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 52, 464–470 (1995).
Mick, E., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Sayer, J. & Kleinman, S. Case–control study of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and maternal smoking, alcohol use, and drug use during pregnancy. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 41, 378–385 (2002).
Hellstrom-Lindahl, E., Gorbounova, O., Seiger, A., Mousavi, M. & Nordberg, A. Regional distribution of nicotinic receptors during prenatal development of human brain and spinal cord. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 108, 147–160 (1998).
Roy, T. S., Seidler, F. J. & Slotkin, T. A. Prenatal nicotine exposure evokes alterations of cell structure in hippocampus and somatosensory cortex. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 300, 124–133 (2002).
Lim, D. K. & Kim, H. S. Changes in the glutamate release and uptake of cerebellar cells in perinatally nicotine-exposed rat pups. Neurochem. Res. 26, 1119–1125 (2001).
Oliff, H. S. & Gallardo, K. A. The effect of nicotine on developing brain catecholamine systems. Front. Biosci. 4, D883–D897 (1999).
Fried, P. A. & Watkinson, B. Differential effects on facets of attention in adolescents prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 23, 421–430 (2001).
Cornblatt, B. A. & Malhotra, A. K. Impaired attention as an endophenotype for molecular genetic studies of schizophrenia. Am. J. Med. Genet. 105, 11–15 (2001).
Quay, H. C. in Handbook of Disruptive Behavior Disorders (eds Quay, H. C. & Hogan, A. E.) 3–21 (Plenum, New York, 1999).
Teicher, M. H., Ito, Y., Glod, C. A. & Barber, N. I. Objective measurement of hyperactivity and attentional problems in ADHD. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 35, 334–342 (1996).
Teicher, M. H. et al. Functional deficits in basal ganglia of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder shown with functional magnetic resonance imaging relaxometry. Nature Med. 6, 470–473 (2000).
Anderson, C. M., Polcari, A. M., Lowen, S. B., Renshaw, P. F. & Teicher, M. H. Effects of methylphenidate on functional magnetic resonance relaxometry of the cerebellar vermis in children with ADHD. Am. J. Psychiatry (in the press).
Porrino, L. J. et al. A naturalistic assessment of the motor activity of hyperactive boys. I. Comparison with normal controls. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 40, 681–687 (1983).
Antrop, I., Roeyers, H., Van Oost, P. & Buysse, A. Stimulation seeking and hyperactivity in children with ADHD. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 41, 225–231 (2000).
Corkum, P., Tannock, R., Moldofsky, H., Hogg-Johnson, S. & Humphries, T. Actigraphy and parental ratings of sleep in children with attention-deficit–hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Sleep 24, 303–312 (2001).
Ferguson, S. A. in Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience (eds Solanto, M. V., Arnsten, A. F. T. & Castellanos, F. X.) 209–220 (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2001).
Shaywitz, B. A., Yager, R. D. & Klopper, J. H. Selective brain dopamine depletion in developing rats: an experimental model of minimal brain dysfunction. Science 191, 305–308 (1976).
Cardinal, R. N., Pennicott, D. R., Sugathapala, C. L., Robbins, T. W. & Everitt, B. J. Impulsive choice induced in rats by lesions of the nucleus accumbens core. Science 292, 2499–2501 (2001).
Giros, B., Jaber, M., Jones, S. R., Wightman, R. M. & Caron, M. G. Hyperlocomotion and indifference to cocaine and amphetamine in mice lacking the dopamine transporter. Nature 379, 606–612 (1996).
Viggiano, D., Grammatikopoulos, G. & Sadile, A. G. A morphometric evidence for a hyperfunctioning mesolimbic system in an animal model of ADHD. Behav. Brain Res. 130, 181–189 (2002).
Solanto, M. V., Arnsten, A. F. T. & Castellanos, F. X. in Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience (eds Solanto, M. V., Arnsten, A. F. T. & Castellanos, F. X.) 355–379 (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2001).
Todd, R. D. & Botteron, K. N. Is attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder an energy deficiency syndrome? Biol. Psychiatry 50, 151–158 (2001).
Castellanos, F. X. in Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience (eds Solanto, M. V., Arnsten, A. F. T. & Castellanos, F. X.) 243–258 (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2001).
Castellanos, F. X. et al. Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 58, 289–295 (2001).
Dougherty, D. D. et al. Dopamine transporter density is elevated in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet 354, 2132–2133 (1999).The first report of elevated striatal DAT density in adults with ADHD.
Krause, K. H., Dresel, S. H., Krause, J., Kung, H. F. & Tatsch, K. Increased striatal dopamine transporter in adult patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: effects of methylphenidate as measured by single photon emission computed tomography. Neurosci. Lett. 285, 107–110 (2000).
van Dyck, C. H. et al. Unaltered dopamine transporter availability in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 159, 309–312 (2002).
Castellanos, F. X. Toward a pathophysiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin Pediatr (Phila) 36, 381–393 (1997).
Ernst, M., Zametkin, A. J., Matochik, J. A., Jons, P. H. & Cohen, R. M. DOPA decarboxylase activity in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder adults. A [fluorine-18]fluorodopa positron emission tomographic study. J. Neurosci. 18, 5901–5907 (1998).
Ernst, M. et al. High midbrain 18F-DOPA accumulation in children with ADHD. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 1209–1215 (1999).
Pennington, B. F. & Ozonoff, S. Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 37, 51–87 (1996).
Nigg, J. T. On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychol. Bull. 126, 220–246 (2000).
Oosterlaan, J., Logan, G. D. & Sergeant, J. A. Response inhibition in AD/HD, CD, comorbid AD/HD + CD, anxious, and control children: a meta-analysis of studies with the stop task. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 39, 411–425 (1998).
Solanto, M. V. et al. The ecological validity of delay aversion and response inhibition as measures of impulsivity in AD/HD: a supplement to the NIMH multimodal treatment study of AD/HD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 29, 215–228 (2001).A head-to-head collaborative comparison of experimental measures linked to two competing theories of ADHD pathogenesis.
Logan, G. D., Cowan, W. B. & Davis, K. A. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 10, 276–291 (1984).
Crosbie, J. & Schachar, R. Deficient inhibition as a marker for familial ADHD. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 1884–1890 (2001).A preliminary demonstration of the usefulness of a laboratory measure of response inhibition (stop task) as an endophenotype. ADHD was significantly more prevalent in first-degree relatives of children with ADHD who showed poor inhibition (48% versus 19%).
Chhabildas, N., Pennington, B. F. & Willcutt, E. G. A comparison of the neuropsychological profiles of the DSM-IV subtypes of ADHD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 29, 529–540 (2001).
Tripp, G. & Alsop, B. Sensitivity to reward delay in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 691–698 (2001).
Schultz, W. Reward signaling by dopamine neurons. Neuroscientist 7, 293–302 (2001).
Johansen, E. B., Aase, H., Meyer, A. & Sagvolden, T. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) behaviour explained by dysfunctioning reinforcement and extinction processes. Behav. Brain Res. 130, 37–45 (2002).
Barkley, R. A., Edwards, G., Laneri, M., Fletcher, K. & Metevia, L. Executive functioning, temporal discounting, and sense of time in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD). J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 29, 541–556 (2001).
Madras, B. K., Miller, G. M. & Fischman, A. J. The dopamine transporter: relevance to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Behav. Brain Res. 130, 57–63 (2002).
Knutson, B., Adams, C. M., Fong, G. W. & Hommer, D. Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci. 21, RC159 (2001).This was the first human functional study to show selective activation of the nucleus accumbens during reward anticipation but not reward attainment.
Berquin, P. C. et al. The cerebellum in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a morphometric study. Neurology 50, 1087–1093 (1998).
Mostofsky, S. H., Reiss, A. L., Lockhart, P. & Denckla, M. B. Evaluation of cerebellar size in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Child Neurol. 13, 434–439 (1998).
Melchitzky, D. S. & Lewis, D. A. Tyrosine hydroxylase- and dopamine transporter-immunoreactive axons in the primate cerebellum. Evidence for a lobular- and laminar-specific dopamine innervation. Neuropsychopharmacology 22, 466–472 (2000).
Snider, R. S., Maiti, A. & Snider, S. R. Cerebellar pathways to ventral midbrain and nigra. Exp. Neurol. 53, 714–728 (1976).
Dempesy, C. W. et al. Stimulation of the paleocerebellar cortex of the cat: increased rate of synthesis and release of catecholamines at limbic sites. Biol. Psychiatry 18, 127–132 (1983).
Ernst, M. et al. Intravenous dextroamphetamine and brain glucose metabolism. Neuropsychopharmacology 17, 391–401 (1997).
Volkow, N. D. et al. Effects of methylphenidate on regional brain glucose metabolism in humans: relationship to dopamine D2 receptors. Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 50–55 (1997).
Corkum, P. V. & Siegel, L. S. Is the continuous performance test a valuable research tool for use with children with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 34, 1217–1239 (1993).
Koelega, H. S. Is the continuous performance task useful in research with ADHD children? Comments on a review. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 36, 1477–1485 (1995).
Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Sarason, I., Bransome, E. D. & Beck, L. H. A continuous performance test of brain damage. J. Consult. Psychol. 20, 343–350 (1956).
Douglas, V. I. in Handbook of Disruptive Behavior Disorders (eds Quay, H. C. & Hogan, A. E.) 105–138 (Plenum, New York, 1999).
Kuntsi, J. & Stevenson, J. Psychological mechanisms in hyperactivity. II. The role of genetic factors. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 211–219 (2001).
Kuntsi, J., Oosterlaan, J. & Stevenson, J. Psychological mechanisms in hyperactivity. I. Response inhibition deficit, working memory impairment, delay aversion, or something else? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 199–210 (2001).
Barkley, R. A., Koplowitz, S., Anderson, T. & McMurray, M. B. Sense of time in children with ADHD: effects of duration, distraction, and stimulant medication. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 3, 359–369 (1997).
Barkley, R. A., Murphy, K. R. & Bush, T. Time perception and reproduction in young adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology 15, 351–360 (2001).
Meck, W. H. Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 3, 227–242 (1996).
Ivry, R. Cerebellar timing systems. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 41, 555–573 (1997).
Ivry, R. B. The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6, 851–857 (1996).
Smith, A., Taylor, E., Rogers, J. W., Newman, S. & Rubia, K. Evidence for a pure time perception deficit in children with ADHD. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 43, 529–542 (2002).
Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J. & Dean, P. Developmental dyslexia: the cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends Neurosci. 24, 508–511 (2001).
Castellanos, F. X. et al. Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 53, 607–616 (1996).
Castellanos, F. X. et al. Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA (in the press).This, the first study to compare medication-naive with previously treated ADHD patients and controls, found that brain volume is globally decreased in ADHD, but that cerebellar volumes are even more diminished than cerebral cortical regions.
Ivry, R. Exploring the role of the cerebellum in sensory anticipation and timing: commentary on Tesche and Karhu. Hum. Brain Mapp. 9, 115–118 (2000).
Egan, M. F. et al. Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 6917–6922 (2001).
Ruskin, D. N., Bergstrom, D. A. & Walters, J. R. Multisecond oscillations in firing rate in the globus pallidus: synergistic modulation by D1 and D2 dopamine receptors. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 290, 1493–1501 (1999).
Ruskin, D. N. et al. Drugs used in the treatment of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder affect postsynaptic firing rate and oscillation without preferential autoreceptor action. Biol. Psychiatry 49, 340–350 (2001).A study that addressed the hypothesis that stimulants might function through inhibitory striatal autoreceptors. This study also showed that stimulants in the therapeutic dose range have prominent effects on multisecond oscillations/periodicities (frequency <0.1 Hz) in basal ganglia single-unit recordings.
Ehlers, C. L. & Foote, S. L. Ultradian periodicities in EEG and behavior in the squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus). Am. J. Primatol. 7, 381–389 (1984).
Borger, N. & Van der Meere, J. Motor control and state regulation in children with ADHD: a cardiac response study. Biol. Psychol. 51, 247–267 (2000).
Miyake, A. & Shah, P. in Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Control (eds Miyake, A. & Shah, P.) 442–481 (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, UK, 1999).
Conway, A. R., Cowan, N. & Bunting, M. F. The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: the importance of working memory capacity. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 8, 331–335 (2001).
de Fockert, J. W., Rees, G., Frith, C. D. & Lavie, N. The role of working memory in visual selective attention. Science 291, 1803–1806 (2001).
Braver, T. S. et al. A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. Neuroimage 5, 49–62 (1997).
Carlson, S. et al. Distribution of cortical activation during visuospatial n-back tasks as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb. Cortex 8, 743–752 (1998).
Arnsten, A. F. T. in Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience (eds Solanto, M. V., Arnsten, A. F. T. & Castellanos, F. X.) 185–208 (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2001).
Ellis, K. A. & Nathan, P. J. The pharmacology of human working memory. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 4, 299–313 (2001).
Denney, C. B. & Rapport, M. D. in Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience (eds Solanto, M. V., Arnsten, A. F. T. & Castellanos, F. X.) 283–302 (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2001).
Welsh, M. C. in Developmental Variations in Learning: Applications to Social, Executive Function, Language, and Reading Skills (eds Molfese, D. L. & Molfese, V. J.) 139–185 (Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, New Jersey, 2002).
Barnett, R. et al. Abnormal executive function in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: the effect of stimulant medication and age on spatial working memory. Psychol. Med. 31, 1107–1115 (2001).
Kempton, S. et al. Executive function and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: stimulant medication and better executive function performance in children. Psychol. Med. 29, 527–538 (1999).
Nigg, J. T., Blaskey, L. G., Huang-Pollock, C. L. & Rappley, M. D. Neuropsychological executive functions and DSM-IV ADHD subtypes. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 41, 59–66 (2002).
Schweitzer, J. B. et al. Alterations in the functional anatomy of working memory in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 278–280 (2000).
Sawaguchi, T. The effects of dopamine and its antagonists on directional delay-period activity of prefrontal neurons in monkeys during an oculomotor delayed-response task. Neurosci. Res. 41, 115–128 (2001).
Mattay, V. S. et al. Effects of dextroamphetamine on cognitive performance and cortical activation. Neuroimage 12, 268–275 (2000).
Eisenberg, J. et al. Haplotype relative risk study of catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): association of the high-enzyme activity val allele with ADHD impulsive–hyperactive phenotype. Am. J. Med. Genet. 88, 497–502 (1999).
Barr, C. L. et al. Linkage study of catechol-O-methyltransferase and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Med. Genet. 88, 710–713 (1999).
Hawi, Z., Millar, N., Daly, G., Fitzgerald, M. & Gill, M. No association between catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene polymorphism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in an Irish sample. Am. J. Med. Genet. 96, 282–284 (2000).
Tahir, E. et al. No association between low- and high-activity catecholamine-methyl-transferase (COMT) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in a sample of Turkish children. Am. J. Med. Genet. 96, 285–288 (2000).
Castellanos, F. X. & Swanson, J. in Hyperactivity and Attention Disorders in Childhood (ed. Sandberg, S.) 336–366 (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, UK, 2002).
Monastra, V. J., Lubar, J. F. & Linden, M. The development of a QEEG scanning process for ADHD: reliability and validity studies. Neuropsychology 15, 136–144 (2001).
Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R. & Selikowitz, M. Electroencephalogram differences in two subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychophysiology 38, 212–221 (2001).
Lazzaro, I. et al. Simultaneous EEG and EDA measures in adolescent attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 34, 123–134 (1999).
Raghavachari, S. et al. Gating of human theta oscillations by a working memory task. J. Neurosci. 21, 3175–3183 (2001).
Tesche, C. D. & Karhu, J. Theta oscillations index human hippocampal activation during a working memory task. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 919–924 (2000).
Offord, D. R. et al. Integrating assessment data from multiple informants. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 35, 1078–1085 (1996).
Gould, T. D., Bastain, T. M., Israel, M. E., Hommer, D. W. & Castellanos, F. X. Altered performance on an ocular fixation task in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 50, 633–635 (2001).
Moll, G. H., Heinrich, H., Trott, G., Wirth, S. & Rothenberger, A. Deficient intracortical inhibition in drug-naive children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder is enhanced by methylphenidate. Neurosci. Lett. 284, 121–125 (2000).The first study using transcranial magnetic stimulation to probe the motor system in ADHD. This paper reported isolated deficits in intracortical inhibition.
Heinrich, H., Dickhaus, H., Rothenberger, A., Heinrich, V. & Moll, G. H. Single-sweep analysis of event-related potentials by wavelet networks — methodological basis and clinical application. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 46, 867–879 (1999).
Cowan, R. L. et al. Sex differences in response to red and blue light in human primary visual cortex: a BOLD fMRI study. Psychiatry Res. 100, 129–138 (2000).Although focused on sex differences in normal adults, this report provides further evidence that the visual-system response to blue light might serve as a marker for central dopamine 'tone' that can be studied non-invasively through fMRI.
Todd, R. D. et al. Familiality and heritability of subtypes of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in a population sample of adolescent female twins. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 1891–1898 (2001).
Thapar, A., Hervas, A. & McGuffin, P. Childhood hyperactivity scores are highly heritable and show sibling competition effects: twin study evidence. Behav. Genet. 25, 537–544 (1995).
Simonoff, E. et al. Genetic influences on childhood hyperactivity: contrast effects imply parental rating bias, not sibling interaction. Psychol. Med. 28, 825–837 (1998).
Conners, C. K. Clinical use of rating scales in diagnosis and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatr. Clin. North Am. 46, 857–870 (1999).
Frick, P. J. et al. DSM-IV field trials for the disruptive behavior disorders: symptom utility estimates. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 33, 529–539 (1994).
Angold, A. & Costello, E. J. The child and adolescent psychiatric assessment (CAPA). J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39, 39–48 (2000).
Mota, V. L. & Schachar, R. J. Reformulating attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder according to signal detection theory. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39, 1144–1151 (2000).
Cook, E. H. Jr et al. Association of attention deficit disorder and the dopamine transporter gene. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 56, 993–998 (1995).The first replicated report of a gene locus associated with ADHD.
Faraone, S. V., Doyle, A. E., Mick, E. & Biederman, J. Meta-analysis of the association between the 7-repeat allele of the dopamine D4 receptor gene and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 1052–1057 (2001).
Scheres, A., Oosterlaan, J. & Sergeant, J. A. Response execution and inhibition in children with AD/HD and other disruptive disorders: the role of behavioural activation. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 347–357 (2001).
Tannock, R. in Diagnosis and Treatment of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: an Evidence-Based Approach (eds Jensen, P. & Cooper, J.) (American Medical Association Press, New York, in the press).
Casey, B. J. et al. Implication of right frontostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36, 374–383 (1997).
Rubia, K. et al. Hypofrontality in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: a study with functional MRI. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 891–896 (1999).
Saunders, C. et al. Amphetamine-induced loss of human dopamine transporter activity: an internalization-dependent and cocaine-sensitive mechanism. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 6850–6855 (2000).
Swanson, J. & Volkow, N. D. in Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience (eds Solanto, M. V., Arnsten, A. F. T. & Castellanos, F. X.) 259–282 (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2001).
Arnold, L. E. Responders and nonresponders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 35, 1569–1570 (1996).
Volkow, N. D. et al. Is methylphenidate like cocaine? Studies on their pharmacokinetics and distribution in human brain. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 52, 456–463 (1995).A provocatively titled study that highlights the importance of pharmacokinetic differences for abuse potential and subjective effects of stimulants in humans.
Andersen, S. L., Arvanitogiannis, A., Pliakas, A. M., LeBlanc, C. & Carlezon, W. A. Jr. Altered responsiveness to cocaine in rats exposed to methylphenidate during development. Nature Neurosci. 5, 13–14 (2002).A study showing that conditioned place preference for cocaine is decreased in rats that are exposed to methylphenidate as juveniles, but not as adults.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Related links
Related links
DATABASES
LocusLink
Medscape DrugInfo
OMIM
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
FURTHER INFORMATION
ADHD.net
Encyclopedia of Life Sciences
attention deficit–hyperactivity disorder
MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive Sciences
Glossary
- WORKING MEMORY
-
The representation of items held in consciousness during experiences or after retrieval of memories. Working memory is short-lasting and associated with the active rehearsal or manipulation of information.
- BASAL GANGLIA
-
A group of interconnected subcortical nuclei in the forebrain and midbrain that includes the striatum, globus pallidus, subthalamic nucleus, ventral tegmental area and substantia nigra.
- EFFECT SIZE
-
A measure of effect that is adopted when different scales are used to measure an outcome. It is usually defined as the difference in means between the experimental and control groups, divided by the standard deviation of the control or both groups. As effect size is a standardized measure, it allows us to compare and/or combine the effects found in different studies of the same phenomenon.
- STRIATUM
-
Part of the subpallidum and one of the components of the striatopallidal complex. It comprises deep (caudate nucleus, putamen and nucleus accumbens) and superficial (olfactory tubercle) parts.
- SINGLE-PHOTON EMISSION COMPUTED TOMOGRAPHY
-
A method in which images are generated by using radionuclides that emit single photons of a given energy. Images are captured at multiple positions by rotating the sensor around the subject; the three-dimensional distribution of radionuclides is then used to reconstruct the images. SPECT can be used to observe biochemical and physiological processes, as well as the size and volume of structures. Unlike positron emission tomography, SPECT uses many fewer detectors, resulting in the loss of many available photons and the degradation of the image.
- EXECUTIVE FUNCTION
-
A cluster of high-order capacities, which include selective attention, behavioural planning and response inhibition, and the manipulation of information in problem-solving tasks.
- ANTISACCADE TASKS
-
Tasks in which subjects are required to suppress the automatic response of making a saccade towards a target and, instead, produce an eye movement in the opposite direction.
- GO/NO-GO TASK
-
A task in which the subject must produce a motor response for one class of stimulus while ignoring others.
- POLYMORPHISM
-
The simultaneous existence in the same population of two or more genotypes in frequencies that cannot be explained by recurrent mutations.
- VENTRAL TEGMENTAL AREA
-
A nucleus of the midbrain. The main supplier of dopamine to the cortex.
- LOCUS COERULEUS
-
A nucleus of the brainstem. The main supplier of noradrenaline to the brain.
- P300 COMPONENT
-
A positive-going waveform in the electroencephalogram that occurs approximately 300 ms after the onset of a stimulus, and is related to the attentional and working memory demands of a task.
- THETA/BETA POWER RATIO
-
A ratio that compares the power output in the theta (4–8 Hz) versus the beta (13–21 Hz) frequency bands of the electroencephalogram.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Castellanos, F., Tannock, R. Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the search for endophenotypes. Nat Rev Neurosci 3, 617–628 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn896
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn896
This article is cited by
-
Trait Impulsivity as a Mediator Between Early Cigarette Smoking Initiation and Addiction Severity in Patients with Methamphetamine Use Disorder
International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction (2024)
-
Preference for Immediate Rewards in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Substance Use Disorder: A Shared Intermediate Phenotype?
Current Addiction Reports (2024)
-
Shared genetic etiology between ADHD, task-related behavioral measures and brain activation during response inhibition in a youth ADHD case–control study
European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience (2024)
-
The mesocortical dopaminergic system cannot explain hyperactivity in an animal model of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)- Spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR)
Laboratory Animal Research (2023)
-
Bayesian dynamical system analysis of the effects of methylphenidate in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a randomized trial
Neuropsychopharmacology (2023)