In my recent Opinion article (Sensory theories of developmental dyslexia: three challenges for research. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 16, 43–54 (2015))1, I described the challenges in establishing causality for each of the main sensory-deficit theories of developmental dyslexia. In their comment (Visual attention deficits in developmental dyslexia cannot be ascribed solely to poor reading experience. Nature Rev. Neurosci. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3836-c1 (2015))2, Lobier and Valdois bring together 12 of their studies to argue that a visual attention (VA) span deficit meets the criteria that are required to demonstrate causality in developmental dyslexia3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14. Unfortunately, all of the studies that were discussed used letters to assess VA span; thus, the demonstrated deficit only affects letters and is causally ambiguous1. Lobier and Valdois do not address my key point — that a VA span deficit disappears if non-alphanumeric stimuli are used15,16 (Fig. 1). Furthermore, Lobier and Valdois rely on studies with adults and chronological age controls. Such research designs are ambiguous with respect to causality1, as the individuals with dyslexia and control individuals that were studied differed dramatically in their experience of recoding letters to sound.

Figure 1: The visual attention span task.
figure 1

Variations on the visual attention span task using alphanumeric stimuli (parts a and b) and dots (part c) are shown. Compared with 10-year-old control children (control), French-speaking 10-year-old children with dyslexia (DYS) showed a reliable deficit in the accurate reporting of letters and digits but not in reporting the colour of the dots (part d). Adapted with permission from Ref. 16, © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

PowerPoint slide

Here, I focus on the reading level (RL) match and training data presented by Lobier and Valdois (see Table 1 in my Opinion article1), and a chronological-age match study using unfamiliar visual stimuli. Lobier and Valdois report an RL match study with 10-year-old children with dyslexia and 7-year-old typically developing children7. The VA span deficit (in letter report) was significant, but the younger children were markedly poorer at identifying single letters, which were the stimuli used in the VA span task. Consequently, this difference in identification accuracy could have caused the VA span difference7. Furthermore, the individuals with VA span dyslexia showed markedly worse pseudoword reading than those with 'phonological' dyslexia, indicating that they have very severely reduced experience in phonological recoding. This reduced practice in phonological recoding could explain the reduced VA span1. Lobier and Valdois also cite an RL match study that compared 11-year-old children with dyslexia (RL = 7 years) to 8-year-old typically developing children8. Unfortunately, the VA span task again used letters, but letter identification skills were not measured. Hence, the group difference in VA span could again arise from poorer letter identification skills in younger children.

In addition, Lobier and Valdois cite a training study that followed one 8-year-old child for 17 months9. The child received VA span training in months 3–6 and gained 4 months in reading age. Unfortunately, no control children who received a different type of training were tested, so the gain is impossible to interpret. Indeed, this child was not impaired in VA span when compared with RL match controls. Finally, a visual categorization study involving 10-year-old children was discussed11. Children were shown unfamiliar visual stimuli (Fig. 2), with strings of five stimuli from two categories displayed on any one trial. Children had to decide whether one, two or three stimuli were present from one of the two categories, which was visually cued before and after each trial. This task and stimuli are very similar to those used in a series of studies of visual processing in dyslexia by Vellutino17. Vellutino comprehensively established that, with unfamiliar visual stimuli, recognition and retention is affected by verbal labelling. Hence, a labelling (that is, phonological) difficulty could explain poorer categorization in individuals with dyslexia. Note that this was not a standard VA span study and that an RL match comparison was not included.

Figure 2: Visual attention span task using non-verbal stimuli.
figure 2

The unfamiliar (that is, non-verbal) visual stimuli that are used to assess visual categorization skills in dyslexia are shown. The categories include abstract shapes, pseudoletters and Japanese Hiragana characters. Children were asked to remember which category was presented visually in a pre-trial cue. They then saw a string of five stimuli for 200 ms (top panel) and had to decide how many were in the cued category. They were then cued again with the category (middle panel) and had to respond. Figure reprinted from Cortex, 48, Lobier, M., Zoubrinetzky, R. & Valdois, S., The visual attention span deficit in dyslexia is visual and not verbal, 768–773, Copyright (2012), with permission from Elsevier.

PowerPoint slide

Clearly, the evidence presented does not critically touch on the challenges for sensory theories of dyslexia that are posed in my Opinion article1. Currently missing are studies measuring VA span before reading tuition commences, using both alphanumeric and non-alphanumeric stimuli, showing a reduced span in at-risk children. Studies of illiterate adults could also be informative, as no VA span difference should be found for letters versus symbols15. When verbal labels are overlearned, as for coloured dots, the VA span of individuals with dyslexia is equivalent to chronological age controls (Fig. 1). This fatally undermines the argument that the VA span deficit is a visually driven and independent cause of dyslexia11.