Key Points
-
The N400 response is one of the best-studied ERP components associated with language processing. Manipulations such as semantic priming ('coffee' → 'tea') and contextual fit ('I like my coffee with cream and sugar/socks') modulate the amplitude of the N400 response to words; this is referred to as the 'N400 effect'.
-
Two contrasting hypotheses address the functional interpretation of the N400 effect. The effect may reflect differences in the amount of effort needed to semantically integrate the incoming word into the previous sentential context; alternatively, it may reflect facilitated lexical access due to pre-activation by supportive contexts.
-
Previous imaging and patient data implicate the posterior middle temporal cortex in storing and activating information associated with lexical representations. Some evidence suggests that anterior temporal and inferior parietal cortices are involved in constructing new inputs and/or integrating them into temporary representations of the larger syntactic and semantic structure, and that the inferior frontal cortex subserves control processes of retrieval and selection of lexical representations that make the input available to integrative processes.
-
The functional anatomic model outlined leads to predictions about the locus of the N400 effect: if the effect reflects integration difficulty, it should localize to inferior frontal, anterior temporal or inferior parietal regions; if the effect reflects facilitated lexical access, it should localize to the posterior middle temporal cortex.
-
Functional MRI studies show semantic priming effects in the inferior frontal cortex only when there is a long interval between primes and targets. Priming effects are observed in the posterior middle temporal gyrus across all prime–target intervals, as is the N400 effect. Magnetoencephalography and event-related optical signal (EROS) studies of sentence context show semantic-priming and semantic-anomaly effects in the posterior temporal cortex at the same latency as the N400 effect.
-
The data from across these techniques provide strong evidence that the posterior middle temporal cortex is involved in generating the N400 effect, which supports the claim that the N400 effect is at least to some degree due to facilitated lexical access, and argues against an account in which the effect is purely integrative.
Abstract
Measuring event-related potentials (ERPs) has been fundamental to our understanding of how language is encoded in the brain. One particular ERP response, the N400 response, has been especially influential as an index of lexical and semantic processing. However, there remains a lack of consensus on the interpretation of this component. Resolving this issue has important consequences for neural models of language comprehension. Here we show that evidence bearing on where the N400 response is generated provides key insights into what it reflects. A neuroanatomical model of semantic processing is used as a guide to interpret the pattern of activated regions in functional MRI, magnetoencephalography and intracranial recordings that are associated with contextual semantic manipulations that lead to N400 effects.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Relevant articles
Open Access articles citing this article.
-
Single-trial neurodynamics reveal N400 and P600 coupling in language comprehension
Cognitive Neurodynamics Open Access 20 June 2023
-
Electrical analysis of logical complexity: an exploratory eeg study of logically valid/invalid deducive inference
Brain Informatics Open Access 07 June 2023
-
Stronger functional connectivity during reading contextually predictable words in slow readers
Scientific Reports Open Access 12 April 2023
Access options
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 print issues and online access
$189.00 per year
only $15.75 per issue
Rent or buy this article
Prices vary by article type
from$1.95
to$39.95
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout





References
Kutas, M. & Hillyard, S. A. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science 207, 203–205 (1980). This seminal study was the first to describe the N400 response, which the authors elicited by using semantically anomalous sentence endings.
Kutas, M. & Federmeier, K. D. Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 463–470 (2000). An influential review which argues that the N400 effect reflects facilitated activation of long-term memory representations rather than semantic-integration difficulty.
Kutas, M., Van Petten, C. & Kluender, R. in The Handbook of psycholinguistics (eds Traxler, M. J. & Gernsbacher, M. A.) 659–724 (Elsevier, San Diego, 2006).
Friederici, A. D. Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 78–84 (2002).
Kotz, S. A. & Friederici, A. D. Electrophysiology of normal and pathological language processing. J. Neurolinguistics 16, 45–58 (2003).
Kuperberg, G. R. Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: challenges to syntax. Brain Res. 1146, 23–49 (2007).
Hagoort, P. The fractionation of spoken language understanding by measuring electrical and magnetic brain signals. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 1055–1069 (2008).
Grainger, J. & Holcomb, P. J. in The Neural Basis of Reading (eds Cornelissen, P. et al.) (Oxford Univ. Press, in the press).
Van Petten, C. & Luka, B. J. Neural localization of semantic context effects in electromagnetic and hemodynamic studies. Brain Lang. 97, 279–293 (2006). This review provides a thorough summary of the N400 effect and its localization.
Bentin, S., McCarthy, G. & Wood, C. C. Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 60, 343–355 (1985).
Rugg, M. D. The effects of semantic priming and word repetition on event-related potentials. Psychophysiology 22, 642–647 (1985).
Rugg, M. D. & Nagy, M. E. Lexical contribution to nonword-repetition effects: evidence from event-related potentials. Mem. Cognit. 15, 473–481 (1987).
Barrett, S. E., Rugg, M. D. & Perrett, D. I. Event-related potentials and the matching of familiar and unfamiliar faces. Neuropsychologia 26, 105–117 (1988).
Barrett, S. E. & Rugg, M. D. Event-related potentials and the semantic matching of faces. Neuropsychologia 27, 913–922 (1989).
Barrett, S. E. & Rugg, M. D. Event-related potentials and the semantic matching of pictures. Brain Cogn. 14, 201–212 (1990).
Holcomb, P. J. & McPherson, W. B. Event-related brain potentials reflect semantic priming in an object decision task. Brain Cogn. 24, 259–276 (1994).
Ganis, G., Kutas, M. & Sereno, M. I. The search for “common sense”: an electrophysiological study of the comprehension of words and pictures in reading. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 89–106 (1996).
Kutas, M. In the company of other words: electrophysiological evidence for single-word and sentence context effects. Lang. Cogn. Process. 8, 533–572 (1993).
Kutas, M. & Hillyard, S. A. Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature 307, 161–163 (1984). In showing that N400 amplitude for sentence endings could be manipulated by expectancy or 'Cloze probability', this paper clearly demonstrated that the N400 response is an index of normal word processing and not only a response to violations.
Federmeier, K. D. & Kutas, M. A rose by any other name: long-term memory structure and sentence processing. J. Mem. Lang. 41, 469–495 (1999).
Osterhout, L. & Holcomb, P. J. Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. J. Mem. Lang. 31, 785–806 (1992).
Brown, C. & Hagoort, P. The processing nature of the N400: evidence from masked priming. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 34–44 (1993). This ERP study used a masked-priming paradigm to provide one of the clearest arguments that the N400 effect reflects integration rather than lexical access mechanisms.
Sereno, S. C., Rayner, K. & Posner, M. I. Establishing a time-line of word recognition: evidence from eye movements and event-related potentials. Neuroreport 9, 2195–2200 (1998).
Hauk, O. & Pulvermuller, F. Effects of word length and frequency on the human event-related potential. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 1090–1103 (2004).
Hauk, O., Davis, M. H., Ford, M., Pulvermuller, F. & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. The time course of visual word recognition as revealed by linear regression analysis of ERP data. Neuroimage 30, 1383–1400 (2006).
Federmeier, K. D. Thinking ahead: the role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology 44, 491–505 (2007).
van Berkum, J. J., Hagoort, P. & Brown, C. M. Semantic integration in sentences and discourse: evidence from the N400. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 657–671 (1999).
St George, M., Mannes, S. & Hoffman, J. E. Global semantic expectancy and language comprehension. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 70–83 (1994).
van Berkum, J. J., Zwitserlood, P., Hagoort, P. & Brown, C. M. When and how do listeners relate a sentence to the wider discourse? Evidence from the N400 effect. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 17, 701–718 (2003). An influential ERP study that demonstrated that the N400 effect can be driven by discourse context beyond the local sentence.
Chwilla, D. J. & Kolk, H. H. J. Accessing world knowledge: evidence from N400 and reaction time priming. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 589–606 (2005).
Camblin, C. C., Gordon, P. C. & Swaab, T. Y. The interplay of discourse congruence and lexical association during sentence processing: evidence from ERPs and eye tracking. J. Mem. Lang. 56, 103–128 (2007).
Van Petten, C. & Kutas, M. Interactions between sentence context and word frequency in event-related brain potentials. Mem. Cognit. 18, 380–393 (1990).
Allen, M., Badecker, W. & Osterhout, L. Morphological analysis in sentence processing: an ERP study. Lang. Cogn. Process. 18, 405–430 (2003).
Holcomb, P. J. & Neville, H. J. Auditory and visual semantic priming in lexical decision: a comparison using event-related brain potentials. Lang. Cogn. Process. 5, 281–312 (1990).
Holcomb, P. J. Semantic priming and stimulus degradation: implications for the role of the N400 in language processing. Psychophysiology 30, 47–61 (1993).
Bentin, S., Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., Giard, M. H., Echallier, J. F. & Pernier, J. ERP manifestations of processing printed words at different psycholinguistic levels: time course and scalp distribution. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 235–260 (1999).
Anderson, J. E. & Holcomb, P. J. Auditory and visual semantic priming using different stimulus onset asynchronies: an event-related brain potential study. Psychophysiology 32, 177–190 (1995).
Deacon, D. et al. The lifetime of automatic semantic priming effects may exceed two seconds. Cogn. Brain Res. 7, 465–472 (1999).
Hill, H., Strube, M., Roesch-Ely, D. & Weisbrod, M. Automatic vs. controlled processes in semantic priming—differentiation by event-related potentials. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 44, 197–218 (2002).
Franklin, M. S., Dien, J., Neely, J. H., Huber, E. & Waterson, L. D. Semantic priming modulates the N400, N300, and N400RP. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 1053–1068 (2007).
Kiefer, M. The N400 is modulated by unconsciously perceived masked words: further evidence for an automatic spreading activation account of N400 priming effects. Cogn. Brain Res. 13, 27–39 (2002).
Grossi, G. Relatedness proportion effects on masked associative priming: an ERP study. Psychophysiology 43, 21–30 (2006).
Damasio, H. in Acquired Aphasia Vol. 2 (ed. Sarno, M. T.) 45–71 (Academic Press, New York, 1991).
Indefrey, P., Hagoort, P., Herzog, H., Seitz, R. J. & Brown, C. M. Syntactic processing in left prefrontal cortex is independent of lexical meaning. Neuroimage 14, 546–555 (2001).
Hickok, G. & Poeppel, D. Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition 92, 67–99 (2004).
Hickok, G. & Poeppel, D. The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 8, 393–402 (2007).
Martin, A. The representation of object concepts in the brain. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 25–45 (2007).
Gagnepain, P. et al. Spoken word memory traces within the human auditory cortex revealed by repetition priming and functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 28, 5281–5289 (2008).
Gold, B. T., Balota, D. A., Kirchhoff, B. A. & Buckner, R. L. Common and dissociable activation patterns associated with controlled semantic and phonological processing: evidence from fMRI adaptation. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1438–1450 (2005).
Price, C. J. et al. Brain activity during reading: the effects of exposure duration and task. Brain 117, 1255–1269 (1994).
Pugh, K. R. et al. Cerebral organization of component processes in reading. Brain 119, 1221–1238 (1996).
Cappa, S. F., Perani, D., Schnur, T., Tettamanti, M. & Fazio, F. The effects of semantic category and knowledge type on lexical-semantic access: a PET study. Neuroimage 8, 350–359 (1998).
Gitelman, D. R., Nobre, A. C., Sonty, S., Parrish, T. B. & Mesulam, M. M. Language network specializations: an analysis with parallel task designs and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 26, 975–985 (2005).
Gold, B. T. et al. Dissociation of automatic and strategic lexical-semantics: functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for differing roles of multiple frontotemporal regions. J. Neurosci. 26, 6523–6532 (2006). This fMRI study provided a comprehensive investigation of a classic psychological manipulation, short- versus long-SOA in semantic priming. The paper demonstrated that priming effects modulate MTG activation across all SOAs, but that priming affects the IFG only for long SOAs.
Davis, M. H. & Johnsrude, I. S. Hierarchical processing in spoken language comprehension. J. Neurosci. 23, 3423–3431 (2003).
Giraud, A. L. et al. Contributions of sensory input, auditory search and verbal comprehension to cortical activity during speech processing. Cereb. Cortex 14, 247–255 (2004).
Badre, D., Poldrack, R. A., Pare-Blagoev, E. J., Insler, R. Z. & Wagner, A. D. Dissociable controlled retrieval and generalized selection mechanisms in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron 47, 907–918 (2005). This paper provided fMRI evidence demonstrating the differential contributions of the anterior and posterior IT to the mechanisms of retrieval and selection.
Hart, J. Jr & Gordon, B. Delineation of single-word semantic comprehension deficits in aphasia, with anatomical correlation. Ann. Neurol. 27, 226–231 (1990).
Kertesz, A. Aphasia and Associated Disorders: Taxonomy, Localization, and Recovery (Grune & Stratton, New York, 1979).
Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D. P., Van Valin, R. D., Redfern, B. B. & Jaeger, J. J. Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition 92, 145–177 (2004). This paper presented neuropsychological evidence from a large aphasic patient group. The analysis distinguished distinct operations mediated by the anterior temporal lobe and the MTG.
Indefrey, P. & Levelt, W. J. The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition 92, 101–144 (2004).
Vandenberghe, R., Price, C., Wise, R., Josephs, O. & Frackowiak, R. S. Functional anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures. Nature 383, 254–256 (1996).
Reddy, L. & Kanwisher, N. Coding of visual objects in the ventral stream. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 408–414 (2006).
Binder, J. R. et al. Human temporal lobe activation by speech and nonspeech sounds. Cereb. Cortex 10, 512–528 (2000).
Wernicke, C. The Aphasic Symptom Complex: a Psychological Study on a Neurological Basis (Kohn and Weigert, Breslau, 1874). English translation available in Cohen, R. S. & Wartofsky, M. W. Boston Studies in the Philosophy of science Vol. 4 (Riedel, Dondrecht, 1969).
Wise, R. J. et al. Separate neural subsystems within 'Wernicke's area'. Brain 124, 83–95 (2001).
Zekveld, A. A., Heslenfeld, D. J., Festen, J. M. & Schoonhoven, R. Top–down and bottom–up processes in speech comprehension. Neuroimage 32, 1826–1836 (2006).
Scott, S. K. Auditory processing—speech, space and auditory objects. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 197–201 (2005).
Davis, M. H. & Johnsrude, I. S. Hearing speech sounds: top-down influences on the interface between audition and speech perception. Hear. Res. 229, 132–147 (2007).
Bavelier, D. et al. Sentence reading: a functional MRI study at 4 Tesla. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 664–686 (1997).
Scott, S. K., Blank, C. C., Rosen, S. & Wise, R. J. Identification of a pathway for intelligible speech in the left temporal lobe. Brain 123, 2400–2406 (2000).
Crinion, J. T., Lambon-Ralph, M. A., Warburton, E. A., Howard, D. & Wise, R. J. Temporal lobe regions engaged during normal speech comprehension. Brain 126, 1193–1201 (2003).
Noppeney, U. & Price, C. J. An fMRI study of syntactic adaptation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 702–713 (2004).
Spitsyna, G., Warren, J. E., Scott, S. K., Turkheimer, F. E. & Wise, R. J. Converging language streams in the human temporal lobe. J. Neurosci. 26, 7328–7336 (2006).
Mazoyer, B. M. et al. The cortical representation of speech. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 4, 467–479 (1993).
Stowe, L. A. et al. Localizing components of a complex task: sentence processing and working memory. Neuroreport 9, 2995–2999 (1998).
Friederici, A. D., Meyer, P. & von Cramon, D. Y. Auditory language comprehension: an event-related fMRI study on the processing of syntactic and lexical information. Brain Lang. 74, 289–300 (2000).
Vandenberghe, R., Nobre, A. C. & Price, C. J. The response of left temporal cortex to sentences. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 550–560 (2002).
Humphries, C., Love, T., Swinney, D. A. & Hickok, G. Response of anterior temporal cortex to prosodic and syntactic manipulations during sentence processing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 26, 128–138 (2005).
Humphries, C., Binder, J. R., Medler, D. A. & Liebenthal, E. Syntactic and semantic modulation of neural activity during auditory sentence comprehension. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 665–679 (2006).
Rogalsky, C. & Hickok, G. Selective attention to semantic and syntactic features modulates sentence processing networks in anterior temporal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 31 July 2008 (doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn126).
Humphries, C., Willard, K., Buchsbaum, B. & Hickok, G. Role of anterior temporal cortex in auditory sentence comprehension: an fMRI study. Neuroreport 12, 1749–1752 (2001).
Gorno-Tempini, M. L. et al. Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Ann. Neurol. 55, 335–346 (2004).
Mummery, C. J. et al. A voxel-based morphometry study of semantic dementia: relationship between temporal lobe atrophy and semantic memory. Ann. Neurol. 47, 36–45 (2000).
Williams, G. B., Nestor, P. J. & Hodges, J. R. Neural correlates of semantic and behavioural deficits in frontotemporal dementia. Neuroimage 24, 1042–1051 (2005).
Noppeney, U. et al. Temporal lobe lesions and semantic impairment: a comparison of herpes simplex virus encephalitis and semantic dementia. Brain 130, 1138–1147 (2007).
Mummery, C. J. et al. Disrupted temporal lobe connections in semantic dementia. Brain 122, 61–73 (1999).
Humphries, C., Binder, J. R., Medler, D. A. & Liebenthal, E. Time course of semantic processes during sentence comprehension: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 36, 924–932 (2007). This fMRI study provided compelling evidence for the role of the angular gyrus in processing combinatorial semantic structure, showing that this region is selectively activated by congruent sentences.
Binder, J. R. et al. Neural correlates of lexical access during visual word recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 372–393 (2003).
Binder, J. R., Medler, D. A., Desai, R., Conant, L. L. & Liebenthal, E. Some neurophysiological constraints on models of word naming. Neuroimage 27, 677–693 (2005).
Ischebeck, A. et al. Reading in a regular orthography: an fMRI study investigating the role of visual familiarity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 727–741 (2004).
Rissman, J., Eliassen, J. C. & Blumstein, S. E. An event-related fMRI investigation of implicit semantic priming. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 1160–1175 (2003).
Mechelli, A., Gorno-Tempini, M. L. & Price, C. J. Neuroimaging studies of word and pseudoword reading: consistencies, inconsistencies, and limitations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 260–271 (2003).
Noppeney, U., Josephs, O., Hocking, J., Price, C. J. & Friston, K. J. The effect of prior visual information on recognition of speech and sounds. Cereb. Cortex 18, 598–609 (2008).
Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M. & Raichle, M. E. Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing. Nature 331, 585–589 (1988).
McDermott, K. B., Petersen, S. E., Watson, J. M. & Ojemann, J. G. A procedure for identifying regions preferentially activated by attention to semantic and phonological relations using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia 41, 293–303 (2003).
Buckner, R. L., Koutstaal, W., Schater, D. L. & Rosen, B. R. Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming. Brain 123, 620–640 (2000).
Gabrieli, J. D., Poldrack, R. A. & Desmond, J. E. The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 906–913 (1998).
Mechelli, A., Josephs, O., Lambon-Ralph, M. A., McClelland, J. L. & Price, C. J. Dissociating stimulus-driven semantic and phonological effect during reading and naming. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 205–217 (2007).
Bookheimer, S. Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 151–188 (2002).
Poldrack, R. A. et al. Functional specialization for semantic and phonological processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 10, 15–35 (1999).
Dapretto, M. & Bookheimer, S. Y. Form and content: dissociating syntax and semantics in sentence comprehension. Neuron 24, 427–432 (1999).
Gough, P. M., Nobre, A. C. & Devlin, J. T. Dissociating linguistic processes in the left inferior frontal cortex with transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Neurosci. 25, 8010–8016 (2005).
Badre, D. & Wagner, A. D. Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory. Neuropsychologia 45, 2883–2901 (2007).
Thompson-Schill, S. L., D'Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K. & Farah, M. J. Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 94, 14792–14797 (1997).
Thompson-Schill, S. L., D'Esposito, M. & Kan, I. P. Effects of repetition and competition on activity in left prefrontal cortex during word generation. Neuron 23, 513–522 (1999).
Rodd, J. M., Davis, M. H. & Johnsrude, I. S. The neural mechanisms of speech comprehension: fMRI studies of semantic ambiguity. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1261–1269 (2005). An elegant, psycholinguistically motivated fMRI investigation that underscored the importance of the posterior temporal cortex for lexical processing and the posterior IFG for lexical selection.
Hagoort, P., Hald, L., Bastiaansen, M. & Petersson, K. M. Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science 304, 438–441 (2004). This study demonstrated that extra-linguistic world knowledge can affect the N400 semantic anomaly effect with evidence from both fMRI and ERP.
Hagoort, P. On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 416–423 (2005).
Kutas, M., Van Petten, C. & Besson, M. Event-related potential asymmetries during the reading of sentences. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 69, 218–233 (1988).
Kutas, M., Hillyard, S. A. & Gazzaniga, M. S. Processing of semantic anomaly by right and left hemispheres of commissurotomy patients. Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain 111, 553–576 (1988).
Van Petten, C. & Rheinfelder, H. Conceptual relationships between spoken words and environmental sounds: event-related brain potential measures. Neuropsychologia 33, 485–508 (1995).
Curran, T., Tucker, D. M., Kutas, M. & Posner, M. I. Topography of the N400: brain electrical activity reflecting semantic expectancy. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 88, 188–209 (1993).
Johnson, B. W. & Hamm, J. P. High-density mapping in an N400 paradigm: evidence for bilateral temporal lobe generators. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 532–545 (2000).
Frishkoff, G. A., Tucker, D. M., Davey, C. & Scherg, M. Frontal and posterior sources of event-related potentials in semantic comprehension. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 20, 329–354 (2004).
Iragui, V., Kutas, M. & Salmon, D. P. Event-related brain potentials during semantic categorization in normal aging and senile dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 100, 392–406 (1996).
Olichney, J. M. et al. Abnormal verbal event related potentials in mild cognitive impairment and incipient Alzheimer's disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 73, 377–384 (2002).
Olichney, J. M. et al. Word repetition in amnesia. Electrophysiological measures of impaired and spared memory. Brain 123, 1948–1963 (2000).
Hagoort, P., Brown, C. M. & Swaab, T. Y. Lexical-semantic event-related potential effects in patients with left hemisphere lesions and aphasia, and patients with right hemisphere lesions without aphasia. Brain 119, 627–649 (1996).
Swaab, T. Y., Brown, C. & Hagoort, P. Spoken sentence comprehension in aphasia: event-related potential evidence for a lexical integration deficit. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 39–66 (1997).
Kojima, T. & Kaga, K. Auditory lexical-semantic processing impairments in aphasic patients reflected in event-related potentials (N400). Auris Nasus Larynx 30, 369–378 (2003).
Friederici, A. D., von Cramon, D. Y. & Kotz, S. A. Language related brain potentials in patients with cortical and subcortical left hemisphere lesions. Brain 122, 1033–1047 (1999).
Friederici, A. D., Hahne, A. & von Cramon, D. Y. First-pass versus second-pass parsing processes in a Wernicke's and a Broca's aphasic: electrophysiological evidence for a double dissociation. Brain Lang. 62, 311–341 (1998).
Olichney, J. M. et al. Reduced sensitivity of the N400 and late positive component to semantic congruity and word repetition in left temporal lobe epilepsy. Clin. Electroencephalogr. 33, 111–118 (2002).
Posner, M. I. & Snyder, C. R. R. Information Processing and Cognition: the Loyola symposium (ed. Solso, R. L.) 55–85 (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1975).
Neely, J. H. Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 106, 226–254 (1977).
Neely, J. H., Keefe, D. E. & Ross, K. L. Semantic priming in the lexical decision task: roles of prospective prime-generated expectancies and retrospective semantic matching. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 15, 1003–1019 (1989).
Rossell, S. L., Price, C. J. & Nobre, A. C. The anatomy and time course of semantic priming investigated by fMRI and ERPs. Neuropsychologia 41, 550–564 (2003).
Nakao, M. & Miyatani, M. in Communicating Skills of Intention (ed. Sakamoto, T. ) 21–31 (Hitsuji Shobo, Tokyo, 2007).
Holcomb, P. J. Automatic and attentional processing: an event-related brain potential analysis of semantic priming. Brain Lang. 35, 66–85 (1988).
Matsumoto, A., Iidaka, T., Haneda, K., Okada, T. & Sadato, N. Linking semantic priming effect in functional MRI and event-related potentials. Neuroimage 24, 624–634 (2005).
Federmeier, K. D., Wlotko, E. W., de Ochoa-Dewald, E. & Kutas, M. Multiple effects of sentential constraint on word processing. Brain Res. 1146, 75–84 (2007).
Van Petten, C., Coulson, S., Rubin, S., Plante, E. & Parks, M. Time course of word identification and semantic integration in spoken language. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 25, 394–417 (1999).
van den Brink, D., Brown, C. M. & Hagoort, P. Electrophysiological evidence for early contextual influences during spoken-word recognition: N200 versus N400 effects. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 967–985 (2001).
Diaz, M. T. & Swaab, T. Y. Electrophysiological differentiation of phonological and semantic integration in word and sentence contexts. Brain Res. 1146, 85–100 (2007).
Connolly, J. F. & Phillips, N. A. Event-related potential components reflect phonological and semantic processing of the terminal words of spoken sentences. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 256–266 (1994).
Vissers, C. T., Chwilla, D. J. & Kolk, H. H. Monitoring in language perception: the effect of misspellings of words in highly constrained sentences. Brain Res. 1106, 150–163 (2006).
Baumgaertner, A., Weiller, C. & Büchel, C. Event-related fMRI reveals cortical sites involved in contextual sentence integration. Neuroimage 16, 736–745 (2002).
Dien, J., Franklin, M. S., Michelson, C. A. & Lemen, L. C. fMRI characterization of the language formulation area. Brain Res. 1229, 179–192 (2008).
Kuperberg, G. R., Holcomb, P. J., Sitnikova, T. & Greve, D. Distinct patterns of neural modulation during the processing of conceptual and syntactic anomalies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 272–293 (2003). This study carried out a detailed comparison of the ERP and fMRI responses to semantic anomaly, using the same material in both techniques.
Devlin, J. T. et al. Susceptibility-induced loss of signal: comparing PET and fMRI on a semantic task. Neuroimage 11, 589–600 (2000).
Uusvuori, J., Parviainen, T., Inkinen, M. & Salmelin, R. Spatiotemporal interaction between sound form and meaning during spoken word perception. Cereb. Cortex 18, 456–466 (2008).
Helenius, P. et al. Cortical activation during spoken-word segmentation in nonreading-impaired and dyslexic adults. J. Neurosci. 22, 2936–2944 (2002).
Simos, P. G., Basile, L. F. & Papanicolaou, A. C. Source localization of the N400 response in a sentence-reading paradigm using evoked magnetic fields and magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Res. 762, 29–39 (1997).
Helenius, P., Salmelin, R., Service, E. & Connolly, J. F. Distinct time courses of word and context comprehension in the left temporal cortex. Brain 121, 1133–1142 (1998). In this study, the first whole-head MEG study to examine the N400 response, the dipole-modelling results showed left-lateralized sources in the posterior temporal cortex.
Helenius, P., Salmelin, R., Service, E. & Connolly, J. F. Semantic cortical activation in dyslexic readers. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 535–550 (1999).
Halgren, E. et al. N400-like magnetoencephalography responses modulated by semantic context, word frequency, and lexical class in sentences. Neuroimage 17, 1101–1116 (2002). This was the first MEG study of the semantic anomaly effect to use a distributed-source model to localize the N400 response. The study showed that early activity in the MTG later spread to more anterior regions.
Pylkkanen, L. & McElree, B. An MEG study of silent meaning. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1905–1921 (2007).
Service, E., Helenius, P., Maury, S. & Salmelin, R. Localization of syntactic and semantic brain responses using magnetoencephalography. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1193–1205 (2007).
Dale, A. M. et al. Dynamical statistical parametric mapping: combining fMRI and MEG for high-resolution imaging of cortical activity. Neuron 26, 55–67 (2000).
Maess, B., Herrmann, C. S., Hahne, A., Nakamura, A. & Friederici, A. D. Localizing the distributed language network responsible for the N400 measured by MEG during auditory sentence processing. Brain Res. 1096, 163–172 (2006).
Tse, C. Y. et al. Imaging cortical dynamics of language processing with the event-related optical signal. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 17157–17162 (2007).
Smith, M. E., Stapleton, J. M. & Halgren, E. Human medial temporal lobe potentials evoked in memory and language tasks. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 63, 145–159 (1986).
Halgren, E. et al. Spatio-temporal stages in face and word processing. I. Depth-recorded potentials in the human occipital, temporal and parietal lobes [corrected]. J. Physiol. (Paris) 88, 1–50 (1994).
Halgren, E. et al. Spatio-temporal stages in face and word processing. 2. Depth-recorded potentials in the human frontal and Rolandic cortices. J. Physiol. (Paris) 88, 51–80 (1994).
Nobre, A. C. & McCarthy, G. Language-related field potentials in the anterior-medial temporal lobe: II. Effects of word type and semantic priming. J. Neurosci. 15, 1090–1098 (1995).
Elger, C. E. et al. Human temporal lobe potentials in verbal learning and memory processes. Neuropsychologia 35, 657–667 (1997).
McCarthy, G., Nobre, A. C., Bentin, S. & Spencer, D. D. Language-related field potentials in the anterior-medial temporal lobe: I. Intracranial distribution and neural generators. J. Neurosci. 15, 1080–1089 (1995). The authors show that recordings from intracranial electrodes in the anterior medial temporal lobe display a semantic-anomaly response that is very similar to the scalp-recorded N400 response.
Pylkkanen, L. & Marantz, A. Tracking the time course of word recognition with MEG. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 187–189 (2003).
Ganis, G. & Kutas, M. An electrophysiological study of scene effects on object identification. Cogn. Brain Res. 16, 123–144 (2003).
Sitnikova, T., Kuperberg, G. & Holcomb, P. J. Semantic integration in videos of real-world events: an electrophysiological investigation. Psychophysiology 40, 160–164 (2003).
Sitnikova, T., Holcomb, P. J., Kiyonaga, K. A. & Kuperberg, G. R. Two neurocognitive mechanisms of semantic integration during the comprehension of visual real-world events. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 2037–2057 (2008).
Plante, E., Petten, C. V. & Senkfor, A. J. Electrophysiological dissociation between verbal and nonverbal semantic processing in learning disabled adults. Neuropsychologia 38, 1669–1684 (2000).
Orgs, G., Lange, K., Dombrowski, J. H. & Heil, M. N400-effects to task-irrelevant environmental sounds: further evidence for obligatory conceptual processing. Neurosci. Lett. 436, 133–137 (2008).
Kintsch, W. The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension construction-integration model. Psychol. Rev. 95, 163–182 (1988).
Crain, S. & Steedman, M. in Natural Language Parsing (eds Dowty, D. R., Karttunen, L. & Zwicky, A. M.) 320–358 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1985).
Zwaan, R. A. & Radvansky, G. A. Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychol. Bull. 123, 162–185 (1998).
Lee, T. S. & Mumford, D. Hierarchical Bayesian inference in the visual cortex. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 20, 1434–1448 (2003).
Rao, R. P. N. & Ballard, D. H. Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neurosci. 2, 79–87 (1999).
Friston, K. A theory of cortical responses. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360, 815–836 (2005).
DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P. & Kutas, M. Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature Neurosci. 8, 1117–1121 (2005).
Van Berkum, J. J. A., Brown, C. M., Zwitserlood, P., Kooijman, V. & Hagoort, P. Anticipating upcoming words in discourse: evidence from ERPs and reading times. Learn. Mem. 31, 443–467 (2005).
van Wassenhove, V., Grant, K. W. & Poeppel, D. Visual speech speeds up the neural processing of auditory speech. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 1181–1186 (2005).
Lau, E., Stroud, C., Plesch, S. & Phillips, C. The role of structural prediction in rapid syntactic analysis. Brain Lang. 98, 74–88 (2006).
Otten, M., Nieuwland, M. S. & Van Berkum, J. J. A. Great expectations: specific lexical anticipation influences the processing of spoken language. BMC Neurosci. 8, 89 (2007).
Vigneau, M. et al. Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. Neuroimage 30, 1414–1432 (2006).
Devlin, J. T., Jamison, H. L., Matthews, P. M. & Gonnerman, L. M. Morphology and the internal structure of words. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 14984–14988 (2004).
Wheatley, T., Weisberg, J., Beauchamp, M. S. & Martin, A. Automatic priming of semantically related words reduces activity in the fusiform gyrus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 1871–1885 (2005).
Kotz, S. A., Cappa, S. F., von Cramon, D. Y. & Friederici, A. D. Modulation of the lexical-semantic network by auditory semantic priming: an event-related functional MRI study. Neuroimage 17, 1761–1772 (2002).
Wible, C. G. et al. Connectivity among semantic associates: an fMRI study of semantic priming. Brain Lang. 97, 294–305 (2006).
Giesbrecht, B., Camblin, C. C. & Swaab, T. Y. Separable effects of semantic priming and imageability on word processing in human cortex. Cereb. Cortex 14, 521–529 (2004).
Ni, W. An event-related neuroimaging study distinguishing form and content in sentence processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 120–133 (2000).
Friederici, A. D., Rüschemeyer, S. A., Hahne, A. & Fiebach, C. J. The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cereb. Cortex 13, 170–177 (2003).
Kiehl, K. A., Laurens, K. R. & Liddle, P. F. Reading anomalous sentences: an event-related fMRI study of semantic processing. Neuroimage 17, 842–850 (2002).
Kuperberg, G. R., Sitnikova, T. & Lakshmanan, B. M. Neuroanatomical distinctions within the semantic system during sentence comprehension: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 40, 367–388 (2008).
Ferstl, E. C., Rinck, M. & von Cramon, D. Y. Emotional and temporal aspects of situation model processing during text comprehension: an event-related fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 724–739 (2005).
Rüschemeyer, S. A., Fiebach, C. J., Kempe, V. & Friederici, A. D. Processing lexical semantic and syntactic information in first and second language: fMRI evidence from German and Russian. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 266–286 (2005).
Rüschemeyer, S. A., Zysset, S. & Friederici, A. D. Native and non-native reading of sentences: an fMRI experiment. Neuroimage 31, 354–365 (2006).
Stringaris, A. K., Medford, N. C., Giampietro, V., Brammer, M. J. & David, A. S. Deriving meaning: distinct neural mechanisms for metaphoric, literal, and non-meaningful sentences. Brain Lang. 100, 150–162 (2007).
Newman, A. J., Pancheva, R., Ozawa, K., Neville, H. J. & Ullman, M. T. An event-related fMRI study of syntactic and semantic violations. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 30, 339–364 (2001).
Cardillo, E. R., Aydelott, J., Matthews, P. M. & Devlin, J. T. Left inferior prefrontal cortex activity reflects inhibitory rather than facilitatory priming. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1552–1561 (2004).
Kang, A. M., Constable, R. T., Gore, J. C. & Avrutin, S. An event-related fMRI study of implicit phrase-level syntactic and semantic processing. Neuroimage 10, 555–561 (1999).
Kuperberg, G. R., Lakshmanan, B. M., Greve, D. N. & West, W. C. Task and semantic relationship influence both the polarity and localization of hemodynamic modulation during lexico-semantic processing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 544–561 (2008).
Acknowledgements
We thank H. Kolk for extensive feedback on the manuscript. Preparation of this manuscript was supported by a National Science Foundation (NSF) Graduate Research Fellowship to E.F.L., 2R01DC05660 to D.P. and NSF DGE-0801465 to the University of Maryland.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Related links
Glossary
- Event-related potentials
-
(ERPs). Electrical potentials that are generated in the brain as a consequence of the synchronized activation of neuronal networks by external stimuli. These evoked potentials are recorded at the scalp and consist of precisely timed sequences of waves or 'components'.
- Morpheme
-
The smallest linguistic unit that has a specific meaning or syntactic function. For example, the word 'outplayed' consists of three morphemes: 'out', 'play' and 'ed'.
- Semantic structure
-
The mental representation of how the meanings of individual words or concepts are combined to yield the meaning of entire phrases or sentences.
- Syntactic structure
-
The mental representation of how the words and phrases within a sentence are organized to form a hierarchically nested structure.
- Discourse structure
-
The mental representation of the information in a conversation or speech act, specifying which information is common knowledge between the speakers, which events or individuals are the current topic of discussion and what are the speakers' communicative goals.
- Pseudowords
-
Auditory or visual strings that sound or look like words but are not actually words (for example, 'blick' but not 'lbikc').
- Aphasia
-
A language impairment that is acquired as a result of stroke or other brain injury.
- Syntactic priming
-
The tendency in language production to formulate sentences with recently used syntactic constructions. For example, when one speaker in a conversation uses a passive construction, other speakers become more likely to use passives.
- Phoneme
-
Individual units of speech sound used to store the sounds of words in memory.
- Split brain
-
A brain in which the two hemispheres have been separated by severing the commissures that connected them.
- Phase coherence
-
The degree to which two oscillating signals (for example, as measured with electrodes from the scalp) are synchronized.
- Stimulus-onset asynchrony
-
(SOA). The time between the onsets of sequentially presented stimuli.
- Equivalent current dipole (ECD) models
-
Estimates of the focal source of neuronal activation derived from multi-sensor data obtained by electroencephalography or magnetoencephalography. The activation is modelled as a point source with a position, direction and intensity.
- Distributed-source models
-
Estimates of the neuronal sources that underlie the brain activity that can be represented with magnetoencephalography or electroencephalography multi-channel data. Activity is not assumed to be focal, permitting a spatially distributed and perhaps more realistic visualization.
- Mental models
-
Internal psychological representations of actual or hypothetical situations that can guide perception, action and reasoning.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Lau, E., Phillips, C. & Poeppel, D. A cortical network for semantics: (de)constructing the N400. Nat Rev Neurosci 9, 920–933 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2532
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2532
This article is cited by
-
Electrical analysis of logical complexity: an exploratory eeg study of logically valid/invalid deducive inference
Brain Informatics (2023)
-
Brain structure, phenotypic and genetic correlates of reading performance
Nature Human Behaviour (2023)
-
Stronger functional connectivity during reading contextually predictable words in slow readers
Scientific Reports (2023)
-
Single-trial neurodynamics reveal N400 and P600 coupling in language comprehension
Cognitive Neurodynamics (2023)
-
Multiple functions of the angular gyrus at high temporal resolution
Brain Structure and Function (2023)