Key Points
-
A voluntary action is a motor act, the occurrence, form and timing of which are generated internally rather than by an immediate external stimulus.
-
Voluntary actions involve a characteristic network of brain motor areas, including the basal ganglia, the pre-supplementary motor area and the parietal lobes.
-
The computations that are performed by this network can be divided into three types of decisions: whether to act, what action to perform and when to perform it.
-
Neural preparation of voluntary action is accompanied by a particular subjective experience that is best described as 'conscious intention'. Conscious intention provides a predictive experience of current actions and contributes to the sense of controlling our actions and, through them, the world around us.
-
Clearer cognitive models of the information processing that is involved in voluntary actions, and new neural data about the brain areas that perform these processes, are making voluntary action amenable to scientific study for the first time.
-
Advances in understanding voluntary action provide the starting point for a neuroscientific approach to one of the fundamental aspects of being human. This will in turn allow better understanding of failures of volition in both neurological and psychiatric illnesses.
Abstract
The capacity for voluntary action is seen as essential to human nature. Yet neuroscience and behaviourist psychology have traditionally dismissed the topic as unscientific, perhaps because the mechanisms that cause actions have long been unclear. However, new research has identified networks of brain areas, including the pre-supplementary motor area, the anterior prefrontal cortex and the parietal cortex, that underlie voluntary action. These areas generate information for forthcoming actions, and also cause the distinctive conscious experience of intending to act and then controlling one's own actions. Volition consists of a series of decisions regarding whether to act, what action to perform and when to perform it. Neuroscientific accounts of voluntary action may inform debates about the nature of individual responsibility.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 print issues and online access
$189.00 per year
only $15.75 per issue
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Blakemore, S., Wolpert, D. & Frith, C. Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 237–242 (2002).
Ryle, G. The Concept of Mind (Univ. Chicago Press, 2000).
Shadlen, M. N. & Gold, J. I. in The Cognitive Neurosciences 3rd edn (ed. Gazzaniga, M. S.) 1229–1241 (MIT Press, 2004).
Libet, B. et al. Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain 106, 623–642 (1983). This classic paper showed that neural preparation for action precedes the conscious feeling of being about to act. Brain activity therefore causes conscious intention rather than the other way around: there is no 'ghost in the machine'.
Frith, C. D. et al. Willed action and the prefrontal cortex in man: a study with PET. Proc. Biol. Sci. 244, 241–246 (1991).
Haggard, P. & Eimer, M. On the relation between brain potentials and the awareness of voluntary movements. Exp. Brain Res. 126, 128–133 (1999).
Brass, M. & Haggard, P. To do or not to do: the neural signature of self-control. J. Neurosci. 27, 9141–9145 (2007). Participants in this study were asked to make a simple manual action on some trials, whereas on other trials they prepared the action but cancelled it at the last moment. Participants' estimates of when they experienced conscious intentions to make actions that were subsequently cancelled allowed the voluntary inhibition of voluntary action to be localized to the anterior frontomedian cortex.
Libet, B., Wright, E. W. & Gleason, C. A. Preparation- or intention-to-act, in relation to pre-event potentials recorded at the vertex. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 56, 367–372 (1983).
Frith, C. Making Up the Mind: How the Brain Creates Our Mental World (Blackwell, 2007).
Jahanshahi, M. & Dirnberger, G. The left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and random generation of responses: studies with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuropsychologia 37, 181–190 (1999).
Haggard, P. Conscious intention and motor cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 290–295 (2005).
Libet, B., Wright, E. W. & Gleason, C. A. Readiness-potentials preceding unrestricted 'spontaneous' vs. pre-planned voluntary acts. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 54, 322–335 (1982).
Sherrington, C. S. The Integrative Action of the Nervous System (Charles Scribner's Sons, New York, 1906).
Dum, R. P. & Strick, P. L. Motor areas in the frontal lobe of the primate. Physiol. Behav. 77, 677–682 (2002).
Picard, N. & Strick, P. L. Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. Cereb. Cortex 6, 342–353 (1996).
Jenkins, I. H. et al. Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. II. The effect of movement predictability on regional cerebral blood flow. Brain 123, 1216–1228 (2000).
Deiber, M. P. et al. Mesial motor areas in self-initiated versus externally triggered movements examined with fMRI: effect of movement type and rate. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 3065–3077 (1999).
Dum, R. P. & Strick, P. L. Frontal lobe inputs to the digit representations of the motor areas on the lateral surface of the hemisphere. J. Neurosci. 25, 1375–1386 (2005).
Kornhuber, H. H. & Deecke, L. Hirnpotentialänderungen bei willkürbewegungen und passiven bewegungen des menschen: bereitschaftspotential und reafferente potentiale. Pflügers Arch. 284, 1–17 (1965).
Shibasaki, H. & Hallett, M. What is the Bereitschaftspotential? Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 2341–2356 (2006).
Lang, W. et al. Three-dimensional localization of SMA activity preceding voluntary movement. A study of electric and magnetic fields in a patient with infarction of the right supplementary motor area. Exp. Brain Res. 87, 688–695 (1991).
Yazawa, S. et al. Human presupplementary motor area is active before voluntary movement: subdural recording of Bereitschaftspotential from medial frontal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 131, 165–177 (2000).
Soon, C. S. et al. Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature Neurosci. 11, 543–545 (2008). Participants in this study chose between making a right or a left hand action while undergoing an MRI scan. Using a novel pattern-classification algorithm, the authors identified areas in the prefrontal cortex that predicted which hand would be used up to 8 seconds before the action was made. This paper suggests how long-range intentions ('prospective memory') may connect to intention-in-action.
Akkal, D., Dum, R. P. & Strick, P. L. Supplementary motor area and presupplementary motor area: targets of basal ganglia and cerebellar output. J. Neurosci. 27, 10659–10673 (2007).
Jahanshahi, M. et al. Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. I. An investigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson's disease subjects. Brain 118, 913–933 (1995).
Loukas, C. & Brown, P. Online prediction of self-paced hand-movements from subthalamic activity using neural networks in Parkinson's disease. J. Neurosci. Methods 137, 193–205 (2004).
Pessiglione, M. et al. Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature 442, 1042–1045 (2006).
Thorndike, E. L. Animal Intelligence: Experimental Studies (The Macmillan Company, 1911).
Rizzolatti, G., Luppino, G. & Matelli, M. The organization of the cortical motor system: new concepts. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 106, 283–296 (1998).
Prabhu, G., Lemon, R. & Haggard, P. On-line control of grasping actions: object-specific motor facilitation requires sustained visual input. J. Neurosci. 27, 12651–12654 (2007).
Shadlen, M. N. & Newsome, W. T. Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 1916–1936 (2001).
Gold, J. I. & Shadlen, M. N. The neural basis of decision making. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 535–574 (2007).
Heekeren, H. R., Marrett, S. & Ungerleider, L. G. The neural systems that mediate human perceptual decision making. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 9, 467–479 (2008).
Shallice, T. From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1988).
Cui, H. & Andersen, R. A. Posterior parietal cortex encodes autonomously selected motor plans. Neuron 56, 552–559 (2007).
Pesaran, B., Nelson, M. J. & Andersen, R. A. Free choice activates a decision circuit between frontal and parietal cortex. Nature 453, 406–409 (2008).
Cohen, J. D., McClure, S. M. & Yu, A. J. Should I stay or should I go? How the human brain manages the trade-off between exploitation and exploration. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 362, 933–942 (2007).
Daw, N. D. et al. Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature 441, 876–879 (2006).
Obhi, S. S. & Haggard, P. Internally generated and externally triggered actions are physically distinct and independently controlled. Exp. Brain Res. 156, 518–523 (2004).
Shallice, T. & Burgess, P. W. Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain 114, 727–741 (1991).
Lhermitte, F. 'Utilization behaviour' and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain 106, 237–255 (1983).
Shallice, T. et al. The origins of utilization behaviour. Brain 112, 1587–1598 (1989).
Boccardi, E. et al. Utilisation behaviour consequent to bilateral SMA softening. Cortex 38, 289–308 (2002).
Della Sala, S., Marchetti, C. & Spinnler, H. Right-sided anarchic (alien) hand: a longitudinal study. Neuropsychologia 29, 1113–1127 (1991).
Kritikos, A., Breen, N. & Mattingley, J. B. Anarchic hand syndrome: bimanual coordination and sensitivity to irrelevant information in unimanual reaches. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 634–647 (2005).
Giovannetti, T. et al. Reduced endogenous control in alien hand syndrome: evidence from naturalistic action. Neuropsychologia 43, 75–88 (2005).
Eimer, M. & Schlaghecken, F. Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Performance 24, 1737–1747 (1998).
Sumner, P. et al. Human medial frontal cortex mediates unconscious inhibition of voluntary action. Neuron 54, 697–711 (2007).
Pacherie, E. The anarchic hand syndrome and utilization behavior: a window onto agentive self-awareness. Funct. Neurol. 22, 211–217 (2007).
Nachev, P. et al. The role of the pre-supplementary motor area in the control of action. Neuroimage 36 (Suppl. 2), T155–T163 (2007).
Nachev, P. et al. Volition and conflict in human medial frontal cortex. Curr. Biol. 15, 122–128 (2005).
Archibald, S. J. et al. Evidence of utilization behavior in children with ADHD. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 11, 367–375 (2005).
Ammon, K. & Gandevia, S. C. Transcranial magnetic stimulation can influence the selection of motor programmes. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 53, 705–707 (1990).
Mueller, V. A. et al. The role of the preSMA and the rostral cingulate zone in internally selected actions. Neuroimage 37, 1354–1361 (2007).
Bracewell, R. M. et al. Motor intention activity in the macaque's lateral intraparietal area. II. Changes of motor plan. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 1457–1464 (1996).
Platt, M. L. & Glimcher, P. W. Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature 400, 233–238 (1999).
Cisek, P. Integrated neural processes for defining potential actions and deciding between them: a computational model. J. Neurosci. 26, 9761–9770 (2006).
Koechlin, E. & Hyafil, A. Anterior prefrontal function and the limits of human decision-making. Science 318, 594–598 (2007).
Wolpert, D. & Miall, R. Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw. 9, 1265–1279 (1996).
Logan, G. D., Cowan, W. B. & Davis, K. A. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 10, 276–291 (1984).
Hallett, M. Volitional control of movement: the physiology of free will. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 1179–1192 (2007).
Cunnington, R. et al. The preparation and execution of self-initiated and externally-triggered movement: a study of event-related fMRI. Neuroimage 15, 373–385 (2002).
Campbell-Meiklejohn, D. K. et al. Knowing when to stop: the brain mechanisms of chasing losses. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 293–300 (2008).
Okuda, J. et al. Differential involvement of regions of rostral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann area 10) in time- and event-based prospective memory. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 64, 233–246 (2007).
Haynes, J. et al. Reading hidden intentions in the human brain. Curr. Biol. 17, 323–328 (2007).
Tipper, S. P., Howard, L. A. & Houghton, G. Action-based mechanisms of attention. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 353, 1385–1393 (1998).
Doyle, M. & Walker, R. Curved saccade trajectories: voluntary and reflexive saccades curve away from irrelevant distractors. Exp. Brain Res. 139, 333–344 (2001).
Cisek, P. & Kalaska, J. F. Neural correlates of reaching decisions in dorsal premotor cortex: specification of multiple direction choices and final selection of action. Neuron 45, 801–814 (2005).
James, W. The Principles of Psychology (Holt and Co., New York, 1890).
Herwig, A., Prinz, W. & Waszak, F. Two modes of sensorimotor integration in intention-based and stimulus-based actions. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 60, 1540–1554 (2007).
Dickinson, A. Contemporary Animal Learning Theory (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1981).
Brown, P. L. & Jenkins, H. M. Auto-shaping of the pigeon's key-peck. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 11, 1–8 (1968).
Haruno, M., Wolpert, D. M. & Kawato, M. Mosaic model for sensorimotor learning and control. Neural Comput. 13, 2201–2220 (2001).
Cunnington, R., Windischberger, C. & Moser, E. Premovement activity of the pre-supplementary motor area and the readiness for action: studies of time-resolved event-related functional MRI. Hum. Move. Sci. 24, 644–656 (2005).
Ursu, S. & Carter, C. S. Outcome representations, counterfactual comparisons and the human orbitofrontal cortex: implications for neuroimaging studies of decision-making. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 23, 51–60 (2005).
Wittgenstein, L. Philosophical Investigations (Blackwell, 1953).
Wegner, D. M. The Illusion of Conscious Will (MIT Press, 2003).
Dennett, D. & Kinsbourne, M. Time and the observer. Behav. Brain Sci. 15, 183–247 (1992).
Kapur, S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 13–23 (2003).
Haggard, P. et al. Awareness of action in schizophrenia. Neuroreport 14, 1081–1085 (2003).
Haggard, P., Clark, S. & Kalogeras, J. Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nature Neurosci. 5, 382–385 (2002).
Moore, J. & Haggard, P. Awareness of action: inference and prediction. Conscious. Cogn. 17, 136–144 (2008).
Klein, T. A. et al. Neural correlates of error awareness. Neuroimage 34, 1774–1781 (2007).
Farrer, C. et al. The angular gyrus computes action awareness representations. Cereb. Cortex 18, 254–261 (2008).
Sirigu, A. et al. Perception of self-generated movement following left parietal lesion. Brain 122, 1867–1874 (1999).
Haggard, P. & Whitford, B. Supplementary motor area provides an efferent signal for sensory suppression. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 19, 52–58 (2004).
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R. & Buckner, R. L. Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 8, 657–661 (2007).
Mulcahy, N. J. & Call, J. Apes save tools for future use. Science 312, 1038–1040 (2006).
Osvath, M. & Osvath, H. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) forethought: self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool use. Anim. Cogn. 11, 661–674 (2008).
Pacherie, E. The phenomenology of action: a conceptual framework. Cognition 107, 179–217 (2008).
Fried, I. et al. Functional organization of human supplementary motor cortex studied by electrical stimulation. J. Neurosci. 11, 3656–3666 (1991). Reported the results of direct stimulation of several frontal sites, including the preSMA, in humans as part of evaluation for neurosurgery. Stimulation at low current elicited an experience of an urge to move a specific body part. More-intense stimulation often produced movement of the same body part.
Leckman, J. F., Walker, D. E. & Cohen, D. J. Premonitory urges in Tourette's syndrome. Am. J. Psychiatry 150, 98–102 (1993).
Sirigu, A. et al. Altered awareness of voluntary action after damage to the parietal cortex. Nature Neurosci. 7, 80–84 (2004).
Velmans, M. How to separate conceptual issues from empirical ones in the study of consciousness. Prog. Brain Res. 168, 1–9 (2008).
Serrien, D. J. et al. Motor inhibition in patients with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome: functional activation patterns as revealed by EEG coherence. Brain 128, 116–125 (2005).
Wundt, W. Grundüge der Physiologischen Psychologie. (Engelmann, Leipzig, 1908).
Lafargue, G. & Duffau, H. Awareness of intending to act following parietal cortex resection. Neuropsychologia 46, 2662–2667 (2008).
Trevena, J. A. & Miller, J. Cortical movement preparation before and after a conscious decision to move. Conscious. Cogn. 11, 162–190 (2002).
Nachev, P., Kennard, C. & Husain, M. Functional role of the supplementary and presupplementary motor areas. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 9, 856–869 (2008).
Penfield, W. & Welch, K. The supplementary motor area of the cerebral cortex. A clinical and experimental study. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 66, 289–317 (1951).
Tanji, J. & Shima, K. Role for supplementary motor area cells in planning several movements ahead. Nature 371, 413–416 (1994).
Kennerley, S. W., Sakai, K. & Rushworth, M. F. S. Organization of action sequences and the role of the pre-SMA. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 978–993 (2004).
Moll, L. & Kuypers, H. G. Premotor cortical ablations in monkeys: contralateral changes in visually guided reaching behavior. Science 198, 317–319 (1977).
Lau, H. C., Rogers, R. D. & Passingham, R. E. Manipulating the experienced onset of intention after action execution. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 81–90 (2007). One of few papers that have tried to manipulate conscious intention, as opposed to merely recording it. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the preSMA just after action significantly advanced the reported time of conscious intention. The authors correctly suggested that the experience of conscious intention reflects a weighted combination of a number of neural signals, including preparation, execution and, perhaps, afferent feedback. TMS adds neural noise to the later components, leading to increased weighting for earlier, preparation-related components in generating conscious experience.
Passingham, R. E. Two cortical systems for directing movement. Ciba Found. Symp. 132, 151–164 (1987). A classical and very clear exposition of the dissociation between systems for internal generation and external guidance of movement, based mainly on ablation of the SMA and premotor areas in monkeys.
Acknowledgements
Writing of this article was supported by generous grants and Fellowships from The Leverhulme Trust, The Royal Society and the Economic and Social Research Council.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Related links
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Haggard, P. Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will. Nat Rev Neurosci 9, 934–946 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2497
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2497
This article is cited by
-
Intact modulation of response vigor in major depressive disorder
Motivation and Emotion (2024)
-
A comparison of the effects of occupation-based interventions with and without responsibility feedback and conventional interventions on participation in people with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial
Trials (2023)
-
Distinguishing intentional from nonintentional actions through eeg and kinematic markers
Scientific Reports (2023)
-
Decision heuristics in contexts integrating action selection and execution
Scientific Reports (2023)
-
Implicit signatures of voluntary action reduce with repeated motor practice
Experimental Brain Research (2023)