Abstract
Our brains and minds are shaped by our experiences, which mainly occur in the context of the culture in which we develop and live. Although psychologists have provided abundant evidence for diversity of human cognition and behaviour across cultures, the question of whether the neural correlates of human cognition are also culture-dependent is often not considered by neuroscientists. However, recent transcultural neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that one's cultural background can influence the neural activity that underlies both high- and low-level cognitive functions. The findings provide a novel approach by which to distinguish culture-sensitive from culture-invariant neural mechanisms of human cognition.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Relevant articles
Open Access articles citing this article.
-
Cortical changes in patients with schizophrenia across two ethnic backgrounds
Scientific Reports Open Access 25 June 2022
-
Cultural values shape the expression of self-evaluative social emotions
Scientific Reports Open Access 23 June 2021
-
Social Robots on a Global Stage: Establishing a Role for Culture During Human–Robot Interaction
International Journal of Social Robotics Open Access 11 November 2020
Access options
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 print issues and online access
$189.00 per year
only $15.75 per issue
Rent or buy this article
Prices vary by article type
from$1.95
to$39.95
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout


References
Markus, H. R., Kitayama, S. & Heiman, R. J. in Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (eds Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W) 857–913 (Guilford, New York, 1996).
Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224–253 (1991).
Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. Culture, self, and the reality of the social. Psychol. Inq. 14, 277–283 (2003).
Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E. & Norenzayan, A. Causal attribution across cultures: variation and universality. Psychol. Bull. 125, 47–63 (1999).
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I. & Norenzayan, A. Culture and systems of thought: holistic vs. analytic cognition. Psychol. Rev. 108, 291–310 (2001).
Nisbett, R. E. & Masuda, T. Culture and point of view. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 11164–11170 (2003).
Nisbett, R. E. The Geography of Thought (Free Press, New York, 2003).
Chiu, C. Y. & Hong, Y. Y. Social Psychology of Culture (Psychology Press, New York, 2006).
Morris, M. & Peng, K. Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 949–971 (1994).
Peng, K. & Nisbett, R. Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. Am. Psychol. 54, 741–754 (1999).
Cacioppo, J. T. Social neuroscience: understanding the pieces fosters understanding the whole and vice versa. Am. Psychol. 57, 819–831 (2002).
Ochsner, K. N. & Lieberman, M. D. The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience. Am. Psychol. 56, 717–734 (2001).
Adolphs, R. Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4, 165–178 (2003).
Blakemore, S., Winston, J. & Frith, U. Social cognitive neuroscience: where are we heading? Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 216–222 (2004).
Lieberman, M. D. Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 58, 259–289 (2007).
Gutchess, A. H., Welsh, R. C., Boduroglu, A. & Park, D. C. Cultural differences in neural function associated with object processing. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 6, 102–109 (2006).
Tang, Y. et al. Arithmetic processing in the brain shaped by cultures. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 10775–10780 (2006).
Zhu, Y., Zhang, Li., Fan, J. & Han, S. Neural basis of cultural influence on self representation. Neuroimage 34, 1310–1317 (2007).
Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R. & Gabrieli, D. E. Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychol. Sci. 19, 12–17 (2008).
Sui, J. & Han, S. Self-construal priming modulates neural substrates of self-awareness. Psychol. Sci. 18, 861–866 (2007).
Lin, Z., Lin., Y. & Han, S. Self-construal priming modulates visual activity underlying global/local perception. Biol. Psychol. 77, 93–97 (2008).
Han, S. et al. Neural consequences of religious belief on self-referential processing. Soc. Neurosci. 3, 1–15 (2008).
Nisbett, R. E. & Miyamoto, Y. The influence of culture: holistic versus analytic perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 467–473 (2005).
Ji, L., Peng, K. & Nisbett, R. E. Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 943–955 (2000).
Kühnen, U. & Oyserman, D. Thinking about the self influences thinking in general: cognitive consequences of salient self-concept. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38, 492–499 (2002).
Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Kawamura, T. & Larsen, J. T. Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures: a cultural look at new look. Psychol. Sci. 14, 201–206 (2003).
Masuda, T. & Nisbett, R. E. Culture and change blindness. Cogn. Sci. 30, 381–399 (2006).
Masuda, T. & Nisbett, R. E. Attending holistically versus analytically: comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81, 922–934 (2001).
Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L., Ungerleider, L. G. & Haxby, J. V. Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature 379, 649–652 (1996).
Goh, J. O. et al. Age and culture modulate object processing and object-scene binding in the ventral visual area. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 44–52 (2007).
Lin, Z. & Han, S. Self-construal priming modulates the scope of visual attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A (in the press).
Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S. & Lee, A. Y. “I” value freedom, but “we” value relationships: self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychol. Sci. 10, 321–326 (1999).
Navon, D. Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353–383 (1977).
Heinze, H. J. et al. Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans. Nature 372, 543–546 (1994).
Di Russo, F., Martinez, A. & Hillyard, S. A. Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention. Cereb. Cortex 13, 486–499 (2003).
Milham, M. P., Banich, M. T., Claus, E. D. & Cohen, N. J. Practice-related effects demonstrate complementary roles of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortices in attentional control. Neuroimage 18, 483–493 (2003).
Bolger, D. J., Perfetti, C. A. & Schneider, W. Cross-cultural effect on the brain revisited: universal structures plus writing system variation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 92–104 (2005).
Tan, L. H., Laird, A., Li, K. & Fox, P. T. Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 83–91 (2005).
Paulesu, E. et al. A cultural effect on brain function. Nature Neurosci. 3, 91–96 (2000).
Shaywitz, S. E. et al. Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 2636–2641 (1998).
Temple, E. et al. Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: evidence from functional MRI. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 2860–2865 (2003).
Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Jin, Z. & Tan, L. H. Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture. Nature 431, 71–76 (2004).
Hoeft, F. et al. Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 4234–4239 (2007).
Siok, W. T., Niu, Z., Jin, Z., Perfetti, C. A. & Tan, L. H. A structural-functional basis for dyslexia in the cortex of Chinese readers. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 5561–5566 (2008).
Morrison, S. J., Demorest, S. M., Aylward, E. H., Cramer, S. C. & Maravilla, K. R. fMRI investigation of cross-cultural music comprehension. Neuroimage 20, 378–384 (2003).
Nan, Y., Knösche, T. R., Zysset, S. & Friederici, A. D. Cross-cultural music phrase processing: an fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 312–328 (2008).
Nan, Y., Knösche, T. R. & Friederici, A. D. The perception of musical phrase structure: a cross-cultural ERP study. Brain Res. 1094, 179–191 (2006).
Ansari, D. Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 9, 278–291 (2008).
Baddeley, A. The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 417–423 (2000).
Markham, R. & Wang, L. Recognition of emotion by Chinese and Australian children. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 27, 616–643 (1996).
Elfenbein, H. A. & Ambady, N. On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 128, 243–249 (2002).
Chiao, J. Y. et al. Cultural specificity in amygdala response to fear faces. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5 May 2008 (doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20151).
Wellman, H. M. The Child's Theory of Mind (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1990).
Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. Interacting minds — a biological basis. Science 286, 1692–1695 (1999).
Frith, C. & Frith, U. Theory of mind. Curr. Biol. 15, R644–R646 (2005).
Fletcher, P. C. et al. Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of 'theory of mind' in story comprehension. Cognition 57, 109–128 (1995).
Abu-Akel, A. A neurobiological mapping of theory of mind. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 43, 29–40 (2003).
Gallagher, H. L. & Frith, C. D. Functional imaging of 'theory of mind'. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 77–83 (2003).
Saxe, R., Carey, S. & Kanwisher, N. Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging Ann. Rev. Psychol. 55, 87–124 (2004).
Frith, U. & Frith, C. D. Development and neurophysiology and mentalizing. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 459–473 (2003).
Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50, 531–534 (2006).
Brunet, E., Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Baylé, M. & Decety, J. A PET investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task. Neuroimage 11, 157–166 (2000).
Gallagher, H. L. et al. Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of 'theory of mind' in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia 38, 11–21 (2000).
Han, S., Jiang, Y., Humphreys, G. W., Zhou, T. & Cai, P. Distinct neural substrates for the perception of real and virtual visual worlds. Neuroimage 24, 928–935 (2005).
Saxe, R. & Kanwisher, N. People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. Neuroimage 19, 1835–1842 (2003).
Saxe, R. & Wexler, A. Making sense of another mind: the role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia 43, 1391–1399 (2005).
Sommer, M. et al. Neural correlates of true and false belief reasoning. Neuroimage 35, 1378–1384 (2007).
Amodio, D. M. & Frith, C. D. Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 7, 268–277 (2006).
Kobayashi, C., Glover, G. H. & Temple, E. Cultural and linguistic influence on neural bases of 'theory of mind': an fMRI study with Japanese bilinguals. Brain Lang. 98, 210–220 (2006).
Allman, J. M., Watson, K. K., Tetreault, N. A. & Hakeem, A. Y. Intuition and autism: a possible role for Von Economo neurons. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 367–373 (2005).
Gorno-Tempini, M. L. et al. Explicit and incidental facial expression processing: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 1, 465–473 (2001).
Moran, M. A., Mufson, E. J. & Mesulam, M. M. Neural inputs into the temporopolar cortex of the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 256, 88–103 (1987).
Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E. & Grafman, J. Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral social judgments. Neuroimage 16, 696–703 (2002).
Kobayashi, C., Glover, G. H. & Temple, E. Cultural and linguistic effects on neural bases of 'theory of mind' in American and Japanese children. Brain Res. 1164, 95–107 (2007).
Decety, J. & Grèzes, J. The power of simulation: imagining one's own and other's behavior. Brain Res. 1079, 4–14 (2006).
Decety, J. & Lamm, C. The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: how low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. Neuroscientist 13, 580–593 (2007).
Ames, D. R. et al. in Intentions and Intentionality (eds Malle, B. F., Moses, L. J. & Baldwin, D. A.) 208–329 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2001).
Perner, J. & Lang, B. Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 337–344 (1999).
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D. & Watson, J. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Dev. 72, 655–684 (2001).
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H. & Norasakkunkit, V. Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72, 1245–1267 (1997).
Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A. & Kirker, W. S. Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 35, 677–688 (1977).
Craik, F. I. M. et al. In search of the self: a positron emission tomography study. Psychol. Sci. 10, 26–34 (1999).
Fossati, P. et al. In search of the emotional self: an fMRI study using positive and negative emotional words. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 1938–1945 (2003).
Heatherton, T. F. et al. Medial prefrontal activity differentiates self from close others. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 1, 18–25 (2006).
Kelley, W. M. et al. Finding the self? An event-related fMRI Study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 785–794 (2002).
Lieberman, M. D., Jarcho, J. M. & Satpute, A. B. Evidence-based and intuition-based self-knowledge: an fMRI study. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 421–435 (2004).
Macrae, C. N., Moran, J. M., Heatherton, T. F., Banfield, J. F. & Kelley, W. M. Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. Cereb. Cortex 14, 647–654 (2004).
Moran, J. M., Macrae, C. N., Heatherton, T. F., Wyland, C. L. & Kelley, W. M. Neuroanatomical evidence for distinct cognitive and affective components of self. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1586–1594 (2006).
D'argembeau, A. et al. Self-referential reflective activity and its relationship with rest: a PET study. Neuroimage 25, 616–624 (2005).
Northoff, G. & Bermpohl, F. Cortical midline structures and processing of the self. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 102–107 (2004).
Northoff, G., DeGreck, M., Bermpohl, F. & Panksepp, J. Self-referential processing in our brain – a metaanalysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 31, 440–457 (2006).
Zhang, L. et al. In search of the Chinese self: an fMRI study. Sci. China C Life Sci. 49, 89–96 (2006).
Burns, C. “Soul-less” Christianity and the Buddhist empirical self: Buddhist-Christian convergence? Budd.-Christ. Stud. 23, 87–100 (2003).
Ching, J. Paradigms of the self in Buddhism and Christianity. Budd.-Christ. Stud. 4, 31–50 (1984).
Lin, H. in Dialogue Between Budhism and Christianity (eds Wu, Y., Lai, P. & Wang, W.) 317–338 (Zhong Hua Book Company, Beijing, 2005).
Platek, S. M., Keenan, J. P., Gallup, G. G. & Mohamed, F. B. Where am I? The neurological correlates of self and other. Cogn. Brain Res. 19, 114–122 (2004).
Platek, S. M. et al. Neural substrates for functionally discriminating self-face from personally familiar faces. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 91–98 (2006).
Uddin, L. Q., Kaplan, J. T., Molnar-Szakaca, I., Zaidel, E. & Iacoboni, M. Self-face recognition activates a frontoparietal “mirror” network in the right hemisphere: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 25, 926–935 (2005).
Sugiura, M. et al. Passive and active recognition of one's own face. Neuroimage 11, 36–48 (2000).
Sugiura, M. et al. Cortical mechanisms of visual self-recognition. Neuroimage 24, 143–49 (2005).
Northoff, G. Philosophy of the Brain. The Brain Problem (Benjamin, Amsterdam, 2004)
Pi, E. H. Transcultural psychopharmacology: present and future. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 52, S185–S187 (1998).
Munoz, C. & Hilgenberg, C. Ethnopharmacology. Am. J. Nurs. 105, 40–48 (2005).
Nisbett, R. E. & Norenzayan, A. in Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology: Cognition Vol. 2 (eds Pashler, H. & Medin, D. L.) 561–597 (Wiley, New York, 2002).
Wu, S. & Keysar, B. The effect of culture on perspective taking. Psychol. Sci. 18, 600–606 (2007).
Wang, Q. & Conway, M. A. The stories we keep: autobiographical memory in American and Chinese middle-aged adults. J. Pers. 72, 911–938 (2004).
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J. & Burton, H. A. Two self-reference effects: the importance of distinguishing between self-descriptiveness judgments and autobiographical retrieval in self-referent encoding. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 56, 853–865 (1989).
Zhu, Y. & Zhang, L. An experimental study on the self-reference effect. Sci. China C Life Sci. 45, 120–128 (2002).
Choi, I. & Nisbett, R. E. Situational salience and cultural differences in the correspondence bias and actor-observer bias. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 24, 949–960 (1998).
Norenzayan, A., Choi, I. & Nisbett, R. E. Cultural similarities and differences in social inference: evidence from behavioral predictions and lay theories of behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 109–120 (2002).
Peng, K. & Knowles, E. Culture, education, and the attribution of physical causality. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 1272–1284 (2003).
Ji, L., Zhang, Z. & Nisbett, R. E. Is it culture or is it language? Examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on categorization. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 57–65 (2004).
Singer, T. et al. Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature 439, 466–469 (2006).
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D. & Williams, K. D. Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science 302, 290–292 (2003).
Park, D. C. & Gutchess, A. H. Aging, cognition, and culture: a neuroscientific perspective. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 859–867 (2002).
Rushton, J. P. & Ankney, C. D. Brain size and cognitive ability: correlations with age, sex, social class, and race. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 3, 21–36 (1996).
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Project 30630025) (S.H.), the Salus Foundation (G.N.) and the Hope of Depression Research Foundation (G.N.).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Related links
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Han, S., Northoff, G. Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: a transcultural neuroimaging approach. Nat Rev Neurosci 9, 646–654 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2456
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2456
This article is cited by
-
Increasing diversity in connectomics with the Chinese Human Connectome Project
Nature Neuroscience (2023)
-
Cortical changes in patients with schizophrenia across two ethnic backgrounds
Scientific Reports (2022)
-
Culture points the moral compass: Shared basis of culture and morality
Culture and Brain (2022)
-
Cognitive and neural bases of decision-making causing civilian casualties during intergroup conflict
Nature Human Behaviour (2021)
-
Cultural values shape the expression of self-evaluative social emotions
Scientific Reports (2021)