Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Review Article
  • Published:

Subcortical face processing

Key Points

  • Recent functional imaging, neuropsychological and electrophysiological studies on adults have provided evidence for a fast, low-spatial-frequency (LSF), subcortical face-detection pathway that might also modulate the responses of certain cortical areas to faces and other social stimuli. This route involves the superior colliculus, the pulvinar and the amygdala.

  • Evidence from human depth electrode studies, and from event-related potential and magnetoencephalographic studies, supports a fast pathway for face detection that can produce a face-selective response before early visual cortical areas are activated.

  • Functional MRI studies indicate that the subcortical route processes LSF information about faces, in contrast to the mid- and high-spatial-frequencies that are processed by the cortical route.

  • In many face perception tasks, activity in the two routes is correlated, and functional connections between the subcortical route and cortical regions increases. Together with the shorter-latency activity in the subcortical route, this raises the hypothesis that the subcortical route can modulate activity in face-sensitive cortical regions before the arrival of visual information through the cortical route.

  • Although the amygdala route has commonly been associated with fear detection, various lines of evidence indicate that it has a broader function. Alternative proposals include that the pathway is maximally sensitive to LSF aspects of faces (and that this selectively differentiates expressions such as fear), and that the route is most responsive to the eyes of a stimulus face.

  • Evidence has accrued from many studies that human newborns are biased to attend towards face-relevant stimuli. Although there is a continuing debate about the mechanisms underlying this bias, it is generally agreed that it is sufficient for newborns to attend to real faces in the natural environment.

  • Converging evidence leads to the hypothesis that the subcortical route described in adults supports the face bias in newborns. This suggests an important role for the subcortical route in establishing the specialization of cortical regions involved in face processing during development.

  • The proposed role for the subcortical route in typical development leads to the postulation that disturbance of this route could account for patterns of deficit in some developmental disorders, particularly autism and developmental prosopagnosia.

Abstract

Recent functional imaging, neuropsychological and electrophysiological studies on adults have provided evidence for a fast, low-spatial-frequency, subcortical face-detection pathway that modulates the responses of certain cortical areas to faces and other social stimuli. These findings shed light on an older literature on the face-detection abilities of newborn infants, and the hypothesis that these newborn looking preferences are generated by a subcortical route. Converging lines of evidence indicate that the subcortical face route provides a developmental foundation for what later becomes the adult cortical 'social brain' network, and that disturbances to this pathway might contribute to certain developmental disorders.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: The dual route model of face processing in adults.
Figure 2: Both schematic and realistic stimuli have been used to test newborns' preferences for face-related stimuli.
Figure 3: Schematic illustration of the stimuli that might be optimal for eliciting a face-related preference in newborns.
Figure 4: How newborns see faces.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Desmond, J. E. & Fiez, J. A. Neuroimaging studies of the cerebellum: language, learning and memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 355–362 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Marien, P., Engelborghs, S., Fabbro, F. & De Deyn, P. The lateralized linguistic cerebellum: a review and new hypothesis. Brain Lang. 79, 580–600 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Vuilleumier, P. Faces call for attention: evidence from patients with visual extinction. Neuropsychologia 38, 693–700 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Vuilleumier, P. & Sagiv, N. Two eyes make a pair: facial organization and perceptual learning reduce visual extinction. Neuropsychologia 39, 1144–1149 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Morris, J. S., de Gelder, B., Weiskrantz, L. & Dolan, R. J. Differential extrageniculostriate and amygdala responses to presentation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field. Brain 124, 1241–1252 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. de Gelder, B., Frissen, I., Barton, J. & Hadjikhani, N. A modulatory role for facial expressions in prosopagnosia. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 13105–13110 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Eimer, M. & Holmes, A. An ERP study on the time course of emotional face processing. Neuroreport 13, 427–431 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Streit, M. et al. Time course of regional brain activations during facial emotion recognition in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 342, 101–104 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Braeutigam, S., Bailey, A. J. & Swithenby, S. J. Task-dependent early latency (30–60 ms) visual processing of human faces and other objects. Neuroreport 12, 1531–1536 (2001). One of several studies showing clear evidence for an early fast-track for face processing.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Pourtois, G., Thut, G., de Peralta, R. G., Michel, C. & Vuilleumier, P. Two electrophysiological stages of spatial orienting towards fearful faces: early temporal-parietal activation preceding gain control in the extrastriate visual cortex. Neuroimage 26, 149–163 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Bar, M. A cortical mechanism for triggering top-down facilitation in visual object recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 600–609 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Bailey, A. J., Braeutigam, S., Jousmaki, V. & Swithenby, S. S. Abnormal activation of face processing systems at early and intermediate latency in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: a magnetoencephalographic study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 2575–2585 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Krolak-Salmon, P., Henaff, M. A., Vighetto, A., Bertrand, O. & Mauguiere, F. Early amygdala reaction to fear spreading in occipital, temporal, and frontal cortex: a depth electrode ERP study in human. Neuron 42, 665–676 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Livingstone, M. & Hubel, D. Segregation of form, color, movement and depth: anatomy, physiology and perception. Science 240, 740–749 (1988).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Merigan, W. & Maunsell, J. How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 369–402 (1993).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Schiller, P. H., Malpeli, J. G. & Schein, S. J. Composition of geniculo-striate input to superior colliculus of the rhesus monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 42, 1124–1133 (1979).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J. & Dolan, R. J. Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nature Neurosci. 6, 624–631 (2003). Establishes that HSF and LSF visual information about faces is processed by distinct neural pathways — the cortical and subcortical routes, respectively.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Winston, J. S., Vuilleumier, P. & Dolan, R. J. Effects of low spatial frequency components of fearful faces on fusiform cortex activity. Curr. Biol. 13, 1824–1829 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Zald, D. H. The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 41, 88–123 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Morris, J. S. et al. A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions. Brain 121, 47–57 (1998).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Keightley, M. L. et al. An fMRI study investigating cognitive modulation of brain regions associated with emotional processing of visual stimuli. Neuropsychologia 41, 585–596 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Iidaka, T. et al. Neural interaction of the amygdala with the prefrontal and temporal cortices in the processing of facial expressions as revealed by fMRI. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 1035–1047 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. George, N., Driver, J. & Dolan, R. J. Seen gaze-direction modulates fusiform activity and its coupling with other brain areas during face processing. Neuroimage 13, 1102–1112 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Kim, H. et al. Contextual modulation of amygdala responsivity to surprised faces. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1730–1745 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. LeDoux, J. E. The Emotional Brain (Simon & Schuster, New York, 1996).

    Google Scholar 

  26. Adolphs, R. & Tranel, D. Amygdala damage impairs emotion recognition from scenes only when they contain facial expressions. Neuropsychologia 41, 1281–1289 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Hariri, A. R., Tessitore, A., Mattay, V. S., Fera, F. & Weinberger, D. R. The amygdala response to emotional stimuli: a comparison of faces and scenes. Neuroimage 17, 317–323 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hadjikhani, N. & de Gelder, B. Seeing fearful body expressions activates the fusiform cortex and amygdala. Curr. Biol. 13, 1–20 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Kesler-West, M. L. et al. Neural substrates of facial emotion processing using fMRI. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 11, 213–226 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Adolphs, R. et al. A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature 433, 22–23 (2005). Establishes that a patient with amygdala damage can successfully recognize fearful expressions if their attention is directed towards the eyes. Results suggest that amygdala damage in humans does not result in a selective fear perception deficit, but in a problem with directing attention towards the eyes of others.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Adams, R. B. J. et al. Effects of gaze on amygdala sensitivity to anger and fear faces. Science 300, 1536 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Kawashima, R. et al. The human amygdala plays an important role in gaze monitoring. Brain 122, 779–783 (1999).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Morris, J. S., deBonis, M. & Dolan, R. J. Human amygdala responses to fearful eyes. Neuroimage 17, 214–222 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Whalen, P. J. et al. Human amygdala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites. Science 306, 2061 (2004). Establishes that, at least with faces close to the viewer, the amygdala responds to the proportion of white around the pupil.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Johnson, M. H. & Morton, J. Biology and Cognitive Development: The Case of Face Recognition (Blackwell, Oxford, 1991).

    Google Scholar 

  36. Grieve, K. L., Acuna, C. & Cudeiro, J. The primate pulvinar nuclei: vision and action. Trends Neurosci. 23, 35–39 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Benevento, L. A. & Standage, G. P. The organization of projections of the retino-recipient and non-retino-recipient nuclei of the pretectal complex and layers of the superior colliculus to the lateral pulvinar and medial pulvinar in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 217, 307–336 (1983).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Robinson, D. L. & Petersen, S. E. The pulvinar and visual salience. Trends Neurosci. 15, 127–132 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. O'Brien, B. J., Abel, P. L. & Olavarria, J. F. The retinal input to calbindin-D28k-defined subdivisions in macaque inferior pulvinar. Neurosci. Lett. 312, 145–148 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Jones, E. G. & Burton, H. A projection from the medial pulvinar to the amygdala in primates. Brain Res. 104, 142–147 (1976).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H. D. & Morton, J. Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition 40, 1–19 (1991).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Morton, J. & Johnson, M. H. CONSPEC and CONLERN: a two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychol. Rev. 98, 164–181 (1991).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. de Schonen, S. & Mathivet, E. First come, first served: a scenario about the development of hemispheric specialisation in face recognition during infancy. Curr. Psychol. Cogn. 9, 3–44 (1989).

    Google Scholar 

  44. Nelson, C. A. The development and neural bases of face recognition. Inf. Child Dev. 10, 3–18 (2001).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Gauthier, I. & Nelson, C. The development of face expertise. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 219–224 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Macchi Cassia, V., Simion, F. & Umiltà, C. Face preference at birth: the role of an orienting mechanism. Dev. Sci. 4, 101–108 (2001).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Simion, F., Macchi Cassia, V., Turati, C. & Valenza, E. in The Development of Face Processing in Infancy and Early Childhood: Current Perspectives (eds Pascalis, O. & Slater, A.) 13–26 (Nova Science, New York, 2003).

    Google Scholar 

  48. Turati, C., Simion, F., Milani, I. & Umiltà, C. Newborns' preference for faces: what is crucial? Dev. Psychol. 38, 875–888 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., Zulian, L. & Csibra, G. unpublished observations.

  50. de Haan, M., Humphrey, K. & Johnson, M. H. Developing a brain specialized for face perception: a converging methods approach. Dev. Psychobiol. 40, 200–212 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Atkinson, J. The Developing Visual Brain (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 2000).

    Google Scholar 

  52. Born, A. P., Rostrup, E., Miranda, M. J., Larsson, H. B. W. & Lou, H. C. Visual cortex reactivity in sedated children examined with perfusion MRI (FAIR). Magn. Reson. Imaging 20, 199–205 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Johnson, M. H. Cortical maturation and the development of visual attention in early infancy. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2, 81–95 (1990).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Csibra, G., Tucker, L. A., Volein, A. & Johnson, M. H. Cortical development and saccade planning: the ontogeny of the spike potential. Neuroreport 11, 1069–1073 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Sewards, T. V. & Sewards, M. A. Innate visual object recognition in vertebrates: some proposed pathways and mechanisms. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A 132, 861–891 (2002). A useful review of evidence for subcortical conspecific detection in the young of several vertebrate species.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Horn, G. Pathways of the past; the imprint of memory. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 5, 108–120 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  57. Rafal, R., Henik, A. & Smith, J. Extrageniculate contributions to reflex visual orienting in normal humans — a temporal hemifield advantage. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 3, 323–329 (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  58. de Gelder, B. & Stekelenburg, J. J. Nasal-temporal asymmetry of the N170 for processing faces in normal viewers but not in developmental prosopagnosia. Neurosci. Lett. 376, 40–45 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Simion, F., Valenza, E., Umiltà, C. & Dalla Barba, B. Inhibition of return in newborns is temporo-nasal asymmetrical. Inf. Behav. Dev. 18, 189–194 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Simion, F., Valenza, E., Umiltà, C. & Dalla Barba, B. Preferential orienting to faces in newborns: a temporal-nasal asymmetry. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24, 1399–1405 (1998). An important demonstration that the newborn preference for face-related patterns is due to temporal visual field input. As the temporal field feeds preferentially into the subcortical visual pathway, this supports the view that these preferences are mediated by structures in this circuit.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Farroni, T., Simion, F., Umiltà, C. & Dalla Barba, B. The gap effect in newborns. Dev. Sci. 2, 174–186 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Adolphs, R. Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4, 165–178 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  63. Johnson, M. H. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience: An Introduction 2nd edn (Blackwell, Oxford, 2005).

    Google Scholar 

  64. Bentley, P., Vuilleumier, P., Thiel, C. M., Driver, J. & Dolan, R. J. Cholinergic enhancement modulates neural correlates of selective attention and emotional processing. Neuroimage 20, 58–70 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Dorner, G., Bluth, R. & Tonjes, R. Acetylcholine concentrations in the developing brain appear to affect emotionality and mental capacity in later life. Acta Biol. Med. Ger. 41, 721–723 (1982).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Thomas, K. M. et al. Amygdala response to facial expressions in children and adults. Biol. Psychiatry 49, 309–316 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Cunningham, M. G., Bhattacharyya, S. & Benes, F. M. Amygdalo-cortical sprouting continues into early adulthood: implications for the development of normal and abnormal function during adolescence. J. Comp. Neurol. 453, 116–130 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Skuse, D., Morris, J. & Lawrence, K. The amygdala and development of the social brain. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1008, 91–101 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Lawrence, K., Kuntsi, J., Coleman, M., Campbell, R. & Skuse, D. Face and emotion recognition deficits in Turner syndrome: a possible role for X-linked genes in amygdala development. Neuropsychology 17, 39–49 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Meyer-Lindenberg, A. et al. Neural correlates of genetically abnormal social cognition in Williams syndrome. Nature Neurosci. 8, 991–993 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Bauman, M. & Kemper, T. Limbic and cerebellar abnormalities: consistent findings in infantile autism. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 47, 369 (1988).

    Google Scholar 

  72. Brothers, L., Ring, B. & Kling, A. Responses of neurons in the macaque amygdala to complex social stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. 41, 199–213 (1990).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Baron-Cohen, S. et al. The amygdala theory of autism. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 355–364 (2000). This important review presents evidence in support of a disturbance of the amygdala in ASD.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Abell, F. et al. The neuroanatomy of autism: a voxel based whole brain analysis of structural MRI scans in high functioning individuals. Neuroreport 10, 1647–1651 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Bachevalier, J. in Advances in Neuropsychiatry and Psychopharmacology: Volume 1. Schizophrenia Research (eds Tamminga, C. & Schulz, S.) 129–140 (Raven, New York, 1991).

    Google Scholar 

  76. Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., Calder, A., Keane, J. & Young, A. Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia 41, 209–220 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Ogai, M. et al. fMRI study of recognition of facial expressions in high-functioning autistic patients. Neuroreport 14, 559–563 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Hall, G. B., Szechtman, H. & Nahmias, C. Enhanced salience and emotion recognition in autism: a PET study. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 1439–1441 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Schultz, R. T. et al. The role of the fusiform face area in social cognition: implications for the pathobiology of autism. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 358, 415–427 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Baron-Cohen, S. et al. Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 1891–1898 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Bauman, M. L. & Kemper, T. L. The Neurobiology of Autism 2nd edn (John Hopkins Univ. Press, Baltimore, 2005).

    Google Scholar 

  82. Grice, S. J. et al. Neural correlates of eye-gaze detection in young children with autism. Cortex 41, 342–353 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Happe, F. Autism: cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 216–222 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Gepner, B. & Tardif, C. Spatial frequency and face processing in children with autism and Asperger syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 199–210 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Dawson, G., Webb, S. J. & McPartland, J. Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Dev. Neuropsychol. 27, 403–424 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Bentin, S., Deouell, L. & Soroker, N. Selective streaming of visual information in face recognition: evidence from congenital prosopagnosia. Neuroreport 10, 823–827 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Farah, M. J., Rabinowitz, C., Quinn, G. E. & Liu, G. T. Early commitment of neural substrates for race recognition. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 17, 117–123 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Jones, R. D. & Tranel, D. Severe developmental prosopagnosia in a child with superior intellect. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 23, 265–273 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Nunn, J. A., Postma, P. & Pearson, R. Developmental prosopagnosia: should it be taken at face value? Neurocase 7, 15–27 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Behrmann, M. & Avidan, G. Congenital prosopagnosia: face-blind from birth. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 180–187 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Barton, J. J. S., Cherkasova, M. V. & O'Connor, M. Covert recognition in acquired and developmental prosopagnosia. Neurology 57, 1161–1168 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Simion, F., Macchi-Cassia, V., Turati, C. & Valenza, E. The origins of face perception: specific versus non-specific mechanisms. Inf. Child Dev. 10, 59 (2001).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Simion, F. et al. Newborns' local processing in schematic facelike configurations. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 14, 257–273 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  94. Slater, A. et al. Newborn infants' preference for attractive faces: the role of internal and external facial features. Infancy 1, 265–274 (2000).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Slater, A., Von der Schulenburg, C., Brown, E. & Badenoch, M. Newborn infants prefer attractive faces. Inf. Behav. Dev. 21, 345–354 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. et al. Neural correlates of woman face processing by 2-month-old infants. Neuroimage 15, 454–461 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Halit, H., Csibra, G., Volein, Á. & Johnson, M. H. Face-sensitive cortical processing in early infancy. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 1228–1234 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Ghashghaei, H. & Barbas, H. Pathways for emotion: interactions of prefrontal and anterior temporal pathways in the amygdala of the rhesus monkey. Neuroscience 115, 1261–1279 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Batki, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Connellan, J. & Ahluwalia, J. Is there an innate gaze module? Evidence from human neonates. Inf. Behav. Dev. 23, 223–229 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  100. Bushnell, I. W. R., Sai, F. & Mullin, J. T. Neonatal recognition of the mother's face. Brit. J. Dev. Psychol. 7, 3–15 (1989).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Easterbrook, M., Hains, S., Muir, D. & Kisilevsky, B. Faceness or complexity: evidence from newborn visual tracking of facelike stimuli. Inf. Behav. Dev. 22, 17–35 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F. & Johnson, M. H. Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 9602–9605 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  103. Farroni, T., Pividori, D., Simion, F., Massaccesi, S. & Johnson, M. H. Eye gaze cueing of attention in newborns. Infancy 5, 39–60 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Macchi-Cassia, V., Turati, C. & Simion, F. Can a non-specific bias toward top-heavy patterns explain newborn preference? Psychol. Sci. 15, 379–383 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Pascalis, O. & de Schonen, S. Recognition memory in 3- to 4-day-old human neonates. Neuroreport 5, 1721–1724 (1994).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Umiltà, C., Simion, F. & Valenza, E. Newborn's preference for faces. Eur. Psychol. 1, 200–205 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  107. Walton, G., Bower, N. & Bower, T. Recognition of familiar faces by newborns, Inf. Behav. Dev. 15, 265–269 (1992).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  108. Kleiner, K. A. & Banks, M. S. Stimulus energy does not account for 2-month-old's preferences. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 13, 594–600 (1987).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Morton, J., Johnson, M. H. & Maurer, D. On the reasons for newborns' responses to faces. Inf. Behav. Dev. 13, 99–103 (1990).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  110. Acerra, F., Burnod, Y. & de Schonen, S. Modelling aspects of face processing in early infancy. Dev. Sci. 5, 98–117 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  111. Bednar, J. A. & Miikkulainen, R. Learning innate face preferences. Neural Comput. 15, 1525–1557 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Quinn, P. C. & Slater, A. in Face Perception in Infancy and Early Childhood: Current Perspectives (eds Pascalis, O. & Slater, A.) 3–11 (NOVA Science, New York, 2003).

    Google Scholar 

  113. Langlois, J. H. & Roggman, L. A. Attractive faces are only average. Psychol. Sci. 1, 115–121 (1990).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Gauthier, I., Tarr, M. J., Anderson, A. W., Skudlarski, P. & Gore, J. C. Activation of the middle fusiform 'face area' increases with expertise in recognizing novel objects. Nature Neurosci. 2, 574–580 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  115. Malach, R., Avidan, G., Lerner, Y., Hasson, U. & Levy, I. in Attention and Performance XX (eds Kanishwer, N. & Duncan, J.) 195–204 (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 2004).

    Google Scholar 

  116. Sun, T. et al. Early asymmetry of gene transcription in embryonic human left and right cerebral cortex. Science 308, 1794–1798 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I acknowledge financial support from the Medical Research Council, and helpful discussions with G. Csibra, M. Eimer, T. Farroni, G. Horn and M. Spratling.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The author declares no competing financial interests.

Related links

Related links

DATABASES

OMIM

Autism

FURTHER INFORMATION

Johnson's homepage

Glossary

NEGLECT

A neurological syndrome (often involving damage to the right parietal cortex) in which patients show a marked difficulty in detecting or responding to information in the contralesional field.

BLINDSIGHT

The ability of a person with a lesion in the primary visual cortex to reach towards or guess at the orientation of objects projected on the part of the visual field that corresponds to this lesion, even though they report that they can see nothing in that part of their visual field.

PROSOPAGNOSIA

The inability to visually recognize faces that were previously familiar, usually after a brain injury.

VISUAL EXTINCTION

This is often associated with damage to the parietal cortex. The patient can see a stimulus presented alone in the contralateral visual field, but cannot see it if it is presented at the same time as a stimulus in the ipsilateral visual field.

EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS

(ERPs). Electrical potentials that are generated in the brain as a consequence of the synchronized activation of neuronal networks by external stimuli. These evoked potentials are recorded at the scalp and consist of precisely timed sequences of waves or 'components'.

MAGNETOENCEPHALOGRAPHY

(MEG). A non-invasive technique that allows the detection of the changing magnetic fields that are associated with brain activity. As the magnetic fields of the brain are weak, extremely sensitive magnetic detectors, known as superconducting quantum interference devices, that work at low, superconducting temperatures (−269 °C) are used to pick up the signal.

N170/M170

The N170 is a well-studied ERP component, the latency and amplitude of which are modulated by the presence of faces in the visual input to the participant. It is a negative peak that is usually recorded at 170 ms after stimulus onset over lateral occipital and temporal recording sites. The M170 is a similar component recorded during MEG studies of face processing, and might share common generators with the N170.

VENTRAL VISUAL PATHWAY

Visual information coming from the primary visual cortex is processed in two interconnected but partly dissociable visual pathways, a 'ventral' pathway, which extends into the temporal lobe and is thought to be primarily involved in visual object recognition, and a 'dorsal' pathway, which extends into the parietal lobes and is thought to be more involved in extracting information about 'where' an object is or 'how' to execute visually guided actions towards it.

GAP EFFECT

A commonly observed phenomenon is that a saccade to a peripheral target is significantly slower when a central fixation stimulus is still present, compared with when the fixation point is removed at, or just before, the onset of the peripheral target. One explanation for this gap effect is that participants have to disengage from the fixation point before initiating their saccade to the target.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Johnson, M. Subcortical face processing. Nat Rev Neurosci 6, 766–774 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1766

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1766

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing