Key Points
-
Fixational eye movements are small displacements of the eyeballs which ensure that vision does not fade during fixation. There are three classes — tremor (the smallest), drifts and microsaccades (the largest). Traditionally, fixational eye movements have been studied with retinal stabilization techniques.
-
The functional role of fixational eye movements in perception is controversial. Microsaccades have received the most attention, and there is evidence for and against their role in perception. However, the existence of microsaccade-related activity in the visual pathway indicates that they might be involved in perception.
-
Several studies have focused on unravelling the neural code of the responses elicited by the different fixational eye movements. Microsaccades have been most extensively studied. The burst-like nature of the activity elicited by microsaccades has attracted particular attention, but its physiological meaning remains uncertain.
-
Despite the existence of fixational eye movements, perception is stable. This fact has been explained by a hypothetical microsaccadic suppression mechanism, and several models for such a mechanism have been proposed. The underlying phenomena have yet to be discovered.
-
Fixational eye movements can be modulated by environmental (illumination conditions) and cognitive (attention) factors. Other phenomena also seem to exert a modulatory influence on these movements, but such factors need to be investigated.
-
Among the outstanding questions in this field are: which circuits control fixational eye movements; what is the neural basis of microsaccadic suppression; what are the mechanisms that underlie the effect of cognitive factors during fixation; and what is the meaning of the burst firing that is correlated with microsaccades?
Abstract
Our eyes continually move even while we fix our gaze on an object. Although these fixational eye movements have a magnitude that should make them visible to us, we are unaware of them. If fixational eye movements are counteracted, our visual perception fades completely as a result of neural adaptation. So, our visual system has a built-in paradox — we must fix our gaze to inspect the minute details of our world, but if we were to fixate perfectly, the entire world would fade from view. Owing to their role in counteracting adaptation, fixational eye movements have been studied to elucidate how the brain makes our environment visible. Moreover, because we are not aware of these eye movements, they have been studied to understand the underpinnings of visual awareness. Recent studies of fixational eye movements have focused on determining how visible perception is encoded by neurons in various visual areas of the brain.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Relevant articles
Open Access articles citing this article.
-
Active fixation as an efficient coding strategy for neuromorphic vision
Scientific Reports Open Access 08 May 2023
-
Eye tracking identifies biomarkers in α-synucleinopathies versus progressive supranuclear palsy
Journal of Neurology Open Access 30 April 2022
-
Fixational drift is driven by diffusive dynamics in central neural circuitry
Nature Communications Open Access 31 March 2022
Access options
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 print issues and online access
$189.00 per year
only $15.75 per issue
Rent or buy this article
Prices vary by article type
from$1.95
to$39.95
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout





References
Riggs, L. A. & Ratliff, F. The effects of counteracting the normal movements of the eye. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 42, 872–873 (1952). This classic paper, together with references 2 and 9, showed that when all eye movements are counteracted in the laboratory, visual perception rapidly fades owing to sensory adaptation.
Ditchburn, R. W. & Ginsborg, B. L. Vision with a stabilized retinal image. Nature 170, 36–37 (1952).
Judge, S. J., Richmond, B. J. & Chu, F. C. Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position: an improved method. Vision Res. 20, 535–538 (1980).
Hartline, H. K. The nerve messages in the fibers of the visual pathway. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 30, 239–247 (1940).
Kuffler, S. W. Neurons in the retina: organization, inhibition and excitation problems. Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 17, 281–292 (1952).
Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. Receptive fields and functional architecture in two non-striate visual areas (18 and 19) of the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 28, 229–289 (1965).
Riggs, L. A., Ratliff, F., Cornsweet, J. C. & Cornsweet, T. N. The disappearance of steadily fixated visual test objects. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 43, 495–501 (1953).
Skavenski, A. A., Hansen, R. M., Steinman, R. M. & Winterson, B. J. Quality of retinal image stabilization during small natural and artificial body rotations in man. Vision Res. 19, 675–683 (1979).
Yarbus, A. L. Eye Movements and Vision (Plenum, New York, 1967).
Krauskopf, J. Effect of retinal image motion on contrast thresholds for maintained vision. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 47, 740–744 (1957).
Ditchburn, R. W., Fender, D. H. & Mayne, S. Vision with controlled movements of the retinal image. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 145, 98–107 (1959).
Gerrits, H. J. & Vendrik, A. J. Artificial movements of a stabilized image. Vision Res. 10, 1443–1456 (1970).
Sharpe, C. R. The visibility and fading of thin lines visualized by their controlled movement across the retina. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 222, 113–134 (1972).
Drysdale, A. E. The visibility of retinal blood vessels. Vision Res. 15, 813–818 (1975).
Gerrits, H. J., De Haan, B. & Vendrik, A. J. Experiments with retinal stabilized images. Relations between the observations and neural data. Vision Res. 6, 427–440 (1966).
Ratliff, F. Stationary retinal image requiring no attachments to the eye. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 48, 274–275 (1958).
Coppola, D. & Purves, D. The extraordinarily rapid disappearance of entoptic images. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93, 8001–8004 (1996). This study showed that visual fading due to retinal stabilization can happen extremely quickly, in less than 80 ms. This fast fading of retinal images in the absence of eye movements has made the field reconsider the dynamics of neural adaptation during normal vision.
Kelly, D. H. New method of stabilizing retinal images. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 65, 1184 (1975).
Gur, M. & Snodderly, D. M. Studying striate cortex neurons in behaving monkeys: benefits of image stabilization. Vision Res. 27, 2081–2087 (1987).
Rucci, M. & Desbordes, G. Contributions of fixational eye movements to the discrimination of briefly presented stimuli. J. Vis. 3, 852–864 (2003).
Pritchard, R. M. Stabilized images on the retina. Sci. Am. 204, 72–78 (1961).
Barlow, H. B. Slippage of contact lenses and other artifacts in relation to fading and regeneration of supposedly stable retinal images. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 15, 36–51 (1963).
Helmholtz, H. Helmholtz's Treatise on Physiological Optics (ed. Southall, J. P. C.) (Gryphon Editions, Birmingham, 1985).
Ratliff, F. & Riggs, L. A. Involuntary motions of the eye during monocular fixation. J. Exp. Psychol. 40, 687–701 (1950).
Carpenter, R. H. S. Movements of the Eyes (Pion, London, 1988).
Ditchburn, R. W. Eye-movements in relation to retinal action. Opt. Acta (Lond.) 1, 171–176 (1955).
Spauschus, A., Marsden, J., Halliday, D. M., Rosenberg, J. R. & Brown, P. The origin of ocular microtremor in man. Exp. Brain Res. 126, 556–562 (1999).
Greschner, M., Bongard, M., Rujan, P. & Ammermuller, J. Retinal ganglion cell synchronization by fixational eye movements improves feature stimation. Nature 5, 341–347 (2002). Ganglion neurons from the turtle retina were stimulated by stimuli that moved in a fashion that simulated eye movements during fixation. Fixational eye movements not only led to increased activity of isolated retinal ganglion neurons, but also to increased synchronization in the retinal ganglion cell network.
Martinez-Conde, S., Macknik, S. L. & Hubel, D. H. The function of bursts of spikes during visual fixation in the awake primate lateral geniculate nucleus and primary visual cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 13920–13925 (2002). The parameters of bursts of spikes following microsaccades in the lateral geniculate nucleus and in area V1 depend on whether the visual stimuli presented are optimal or non-optimal for each of these areas.
Riggs, L. A. & Ratliff, F. Visual acuity and the normal tremor of the eyes. Science 114, 17–18 (1951).
Ditchburn, R. W. & Ginsborg, B. L. Involuntary eye movements during fixation. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 119, 1–17 (1953).
Cornsweet, T. N. Determination of the stimuli for involuntary drifts and saccadic eye movements. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 46, 987–993 (1956).
Nachmias, J. Two-dimensional motion of the retinal image during monocular fixation. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 49, 901–908 (1959).
Nachmias, J. Determiners of the drift of the eye during monocular fixation. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 51, 761–766 (1961).
Steinman, R. M., Cunitz, R. J., Timberlake, G. T. & Herman, M. Voluntary control of microsaccades during maintained monocular fixation. Science 155, 1577–1579 (1967).
St Cyr, G. J. & Fender, D. H. The interplay of drifts and flicks in binocular fixation. Vision Res. 9, 245–265 (1969).
Krauskopf, J., Cornsweet, T. N. & Riggs, L. A. Analysis of eye movements during monocular and binocular fixation. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 50, 572–578 (1960).
Eizenman, M., Hallett, P. E. & Frecker, R. C. Power spectra for ocular drift and tremor. Vision Res. 25, 1635–1640 (1985).
Shakhnovich, A. R. & Thomas, J. G. Micro-tremor of the eyes of comatose patients. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 42, 117–119 (1977).
Martinez-Conde, S., Macknik, S. L. & Hubel, D. H. Microsaccadic eye movements and firing of single cells in the striate cortex of macaque monkeys. Nature Neurosci. 3, 251–258 (2000). Fixational microsaccades during the presentation of stationary stimuli led to increases in the activity of primate area V1 neurons. Increases in neural firing after microsaccades were clumped in bursts of spikes.
Ditchburn, R. W. The function of small saccades. Vision Res. 20, 271–272 (1980).
Horwitz, G. D. & Albright, T. D. Short-latency fixational saccades induced by luminance increments. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 1333–1339 (2003).
Skavenski, A. A., Robinson, D. A., Steinman, R. M. & Timberlake, G. T. Miniature eye movements of fixation in rhesus monkey. Vision Res. 15, 1269–1273 (1975).
Snodderly, D. M. & Kurtz, D. Eye position during fixation tasks: comparison of macaque and human. Vision Res. 25, 83–98 (1985).
Snodderly, D. M. Effects of light and dark environments on macaque and human fixational eye movements. Vision Res. 27, 401–415 (1987).
Fiorentini, A. & Ercoles, A. M. Involuntary eye movements during attempted monocular fixation. Atti Fond. Giorgio Ronchi 21, 199–217 (1966).
Engbert, R. & Kliegl, R. Microsaccades keep the eyes' balance during fixation. Psychol. Sci. (in the press)
Zuber, B. L. & Stark, L. Microsaccades and the velocity–amplitude relationship for saccadic eye movements. Science 150, 1459–1460 (1965). This classic study showed a linear relationship between microsaccade amplitudes and velocities. This followed the extrapolation of the same relationship for large saccades, and indicated that both large saccades and microsaccades might be generated by the same neural mechanisms.
Moller, F., Laursen, M. L., Tygesen, J. & Sjolie, A. K. Binocular quantification and characterization of microsaccades. Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. 240, 765–770 (2002).
Wurtz, R. H. Vision for the control of movement. The Friedenwald Lecture. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 37, 2130–2145 (1996).
Sparks, D. L. The brainstem control of saccadic eye movements. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 3, 952–964 (2002).
Lord, M. P. Measurement of binocular eye movements of subjects in the sitting position. Brit. J. Ophthal. 35, 21–30 (1951).
Engbert, R. & Kliegl, R. in The Mind's Eyes: Cognitive and Applied Aspects of Eye Movements (eds Hyona, J., Radach, R. & Deubel, H.) 103–117 (Elsevier, Oxford, 2003).
Westheimer, G. The spatial sense of the eye. Proctor lecture. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 18, 893–912 (1979).
Steinman, R. M., Haddad, G. M., Skavenski, A. A. & Wyman, D. Miniature eye movement. Science 181, 810–819 (1973).
Winterson, B. J. & Collewijn, H. Microsaccades during finely guided visuomotor tasks. Vision Res. 16, 1387–1390 (1976).
Kowler, E. & Steinman, R. M. The role of small saccades in counting. Vision Res. 17, 141–146 (1977).
Kowler, E. & Steinman, R. M. Miniature saccades: eye movements that do not count. Vision Res. 19, 105–108 (1979).
Bridgeman, B. & Palca, J. The role of microsaccades in high acuity observational tasks. Vision Res. 20, 813–817 (1980).
Kowler, E. & Steinman, R. M. Small saccades serve no useful purpose: reply to a letter by R. W. Ditchburn. Vision Res. 20, 273–276 (1980).
Livingstone, M. S., Freeman, D. C. & Hubel, D. H. Visual responses in V1 of freely viewing monkeys. Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 61, 27–37 (1996).
Macknik, S. L. & Livingstone, M. S. Neuronal correlates of visibility and invisibility in the primate visual system. Nature Neurosci. 1, 144–149 (1998).
Gur, M., Beylin, A. & Snodderly, D. M. Response variability of neurons in primary visual cortex (V1) of alert monkeys. J. Neurosci. 17, 2914–2920 (1997).
Gur, M. & Snodderly, D. M. Visual receptive fields of neurons in primary visual cortex (V1) move in space with the eye movements of fixation. Vision Res. 37, 257–265 (1997).
Bair, W. & O'Keefe, L. P. The influence of fixational eye movements on the response of neurons in area MT of the macaque. Vis. Neurosci. 15, 779–786 (1998).
Leopold, D. A. & Logothetis, N. K. Microsaccades differentially modulate neural activity in the striate and extrastriate visual cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 123, 341–345 (1998).
Snodderly, D. M., Kagan, I. & Gur, M. Selective activation of visual cortex neurons by fixational eye movements: implications for neural coding. Vis. Neurosci. 18, 259–277 (2001).
Reppas, J. B., Usrey, W. M. & Reid, R. C. Saccadic eye movements modulate visual responses in the lateral geniculate nucleus. Neuron 35, 961–974 (2002).
Hafed, Z. M. & Clark, J. J. Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts. Vision Res. 42, 2533–2545 (2002). This study, together with references 70 and 72, comprised the first systematic attempts to characterize the influence of cognition on the dynamics of fixational eye movements. These studies provided a rich basis for what is likely to become a lively field of enquiry.
Engbert, R. & Kliegl, R. Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention. Vision Res. 43, 1035–1045 (2003).
Rolfs, M., Engbert, R. & Kliegl, R. Microsaccade orientation supports attentional enhancement opposite to a peripheral cue: commentary on Tse, Sheinberg, and Logothetis. Psychol. Sci. (in the press).
Tse, P. U., Sheinberg, D. L. & Logothetis, N. K. Fixational eye movements are not affected by abrupt onsets that capture attention. Vision Res. 42, 1663–1669 (2002).
Tse, P. U., Sheinberg, D. S. & Logothetis, N. K. The distribution of microsaccade directions need not reveal the location of attention. Psychol. Sci. (in the press).
Gerrits, H. J. & Vendrik, A. J. The influence of stimulus movements on perception in parafoveal stabilized vision. Vision Res. 14, 175–180 (1974).
Clowes, M. B. A note on colour discrimination under conditions of retinal image constraint. Opt. Acta (Lond.) 9, 65–68 (1962).
Macknik, S. L., Martinez-Conde, S. & Haglund, M. M. The role of spatiotemporal edges in visibility and visual masking. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 7556–7560 (2000).
Leopold, D. A. & Logothetis, N. K. Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys percepts during binocular rivalry. Nature 379, 549–553 (1996).
Albrecht, D. G. & Hamilton, D. B. Striate cortex of monkey and cat: contrast response function. J. Neurophysiol. 48, 217–237 (1982).
Albrecht, D. G. Visual cortex neurons in monkey and cat: effect of contrast on the spatial and temporal phase transfer functions. Vis. Neurosci. 12, 1191–1210 (1995).
Gawne, T. J., Kjaer, T. W. & Richmond, B. J. Latency: another potential code for feature binding in striate cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 1356–1360 (1996).
Usrey, W. M., Reppas, J. B. & Reid, R. C. Paired-spike interactions and synaptic efficacy of retinal inputs to the thalamus. Nature 395, 384–387 (1998).
Kara, P. & Reid, R. C. Efficacy of retinal spikes in driving cortical responses. J. Neurosci. 23, 8547–8557 (2003).
Bridgeman, B. B. & Macknik, S. L. Saccadic suppression relies on luminance information. Psychol. Res. 58, 163–168 (1995).
Macknik, S. L., Fisher, B. D. & Bridgeman, B. Flicker distorts visual space constancy. Vision Res. 31, 2057–2064 (1991).
Wurtz, R. H. Visual cortex neurons: response to stimuli during rapid eye movements. Science 162, 1148–1150 (1968).
Wurtz, R. H. Comparison of effects of eye movements and stimulus movements on striate cortex neurons of the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 32, 987–994 (1969).
Ross, J., Morrone, M. C., Goldberg, M. E. & Burr, D. C. Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades. Trends Neurosci. 24, 113–121 (2001).
Beeler, G. W. Visual threshold changes resulting from spontaneous saccadic eye movements. Vision Res. 7, 769–775 (1967).
Krauskopf, J. Lack of inhibition during involuntary saccades. Am. J. Psychol. 79, 73–81 (1966).
Sperling, G. in Eye Movements and Their Role in Visual and Cognitive Processes (ed. Kowler, E.) 307–351 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1990).
Rucci, M., Edelman, G. E. & Wray, J. Modeling LGN responses dring free-viewing: a possible role of microscopic eye movements in the refinement of cortical orientation selectivity. J. Neurosci. 20, 4708–4720 (2000).
Olveczky, B. P., Baccus, S. A. & Meister, M. Segregation of object and background motion in the retina. Nature 423, 401–408 (2003).
Masland, R. H. The retina's fancy tricks. Nature 423, 387 (2003).
Zuber, B. L., Crider, A. & Stark, L. Saccadic suppression associated with microsaccades. Q. Prog. Rep. 74, 244–249 (1964).
Zuber, B. L. & Stark, L. Saccadic suppression: elevation of visual threshold associated with saccadic eye movements. Exp. Neurol. 16, 65–79 (1966).
Murakami, I. & Cavanagh, P. A jitter after-effect reveals motion-based stabilization of vision. Nature 395, 798–801 (1998). A striking visual illusion that showed how jittery our perception of the world would be if fixational eye movements were not systematically compensated for by the visual system.
Murakami, I. & Cavanagh, P. Visual jitter: evidence for visual-motion-based compensation of retinal slip due to small eye movements. Vision Res. 41, 173–186 (2001).
Sasaki, Y., Murakami, I., Cavanagh, P. & Tootell, R. H. Human brain activity during illusory visual jitter as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuron 35, 1147–1156 (2002).
Barash, S., Melikyan, A., Sivakov, A. & Tauber, M. Shift of visual fixation dependent on background illumination. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 2766–2781 (1998).
Steinman, R. M. Effect of target size, luminance, and color on monocular fixation. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 55, 1158–1165 (1965).
Fender, D. H. Variation of fixation direction with colour of fixation target. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 39, 294–297 (1955).
Sansbury, R. V., Skavenski, A. A., Haddad, G. M. & Steinman, R. M. Normal fixation of eccentric targets. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 63, 612–614 (1973).
Corbetta, M. et al. A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron 21, 761–773 (1998). This imaging study showed extensive anatomical overlap between the attentional and eye-movement related areas of the brain.
Barlow, H. B. Eye movements during fixation. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 116, 290–306 (1952).
Kowler, E. & Steinman, R. M. The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control. I. Periodic target steps. Vision Res. 19, 619–632 (1979).
Troxler, D. in Ophthalmologische Bibliothek (eds Himly, K. & Schmidt, J. A.) 1–53 (Springer, Jena, 1804).
Clarke, F. J. J. Rapid light adaptation of localised areas of the extra-foveal retina. Opt. Acta (Lond.) 4, 69–77 (1957).
Clarke, F. J. J. A study of Troxler's effect. Opt. Acta (Lond.) 7, 219–236 (1960).
Clarke, F. J. J. Visual recovery following local adaptation of the perpheral retina (Troxler's effect). Opt. Acta (Lond.) 8, 121–135 (1961).
Clarke, F. J. J. & Belcher, S. J. On the localization of Troxler's effect in the visual pathway. Vision Res. 2, 53–68 (1962).
Gilchrist, I. D., Brown, V. & Findlay, J. M. Saccades without eye movements. Nature 390, 130–131 (1997).
Gilchrist, I. D., Brown, V., Findlay, J. M. & Clarke, M. P. Using the eye-movement system to control the head. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265, 1831–1836 (1998).
Land, M. F., Furneaux, S. M. & Gilchrist, I. D. The organization of visually mediated actions in a subject without eye movements. Neurocase 8, 80–87 (2002).
Steinman, R. M. & Collewijn, H. Binocular retinal image motion during active head rotation. Vision Res. 20, 415–429 (1980).
Verheijen, F. J. A simple after image method demonstrating the involuntary multidirectional eye movements during fixation. Opt. Acta (Lond.) 8, 309–312 (1961).
Simon, F., Schulz, E., Rassow, B. & Haase, W. Binocular micromovement recording of human eyes:—methods. Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. 221, 293–298 (1984).
Riggs, L. A., Armington, J. C. & Ratliff, F. Motions of the retinal image during fixation. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 44, 315–321 (1954).
Adler, F. H. & Fliegelman, M. Influence of fixation on the visual acuity. Arch. Ophthalmol. 12, 475–483 (1934).
Schulz, E. Binocular micromovements in normal persons. Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. 222, 95–100 (1984).
Srebro, R. Fixation of normal and amblyopic eyes. Arch. Ophthalmol. 101, 214–217 (1983).
West, D. C. & Boyce, P. R. The effect of flicker on eye movement. Vision Res. 8, 171–192 (1968).
Boyce, P. R. Monocular fixation in human eye movement. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 167, 293–315 (1967).
Malinov, I. V., Epelboim, J., Herst, A. N. & Steinman, R. M. Characteristics of saccades and vergence in two types of sequential looking tasks. Vision Res. 40, 2083–2090 (2000).
Kingstone, A., Fendrich, R., Wessinger, C. M. & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. Are microsaccades responsible for the gap effect? Percept. Psychophys. 57, 796–801 (1995).
Sabrin, H. W. & Kertesz, A. E. Microsaccadic eye movements and binocular rivalry. Percept. Psychophys. 28, 150–154 (1980).
Lord, M. P. & Wright, W. D. Eye movements during monocular fixation. Nature 162, 25–26 (1948).
Ditchburn, R. W. & Foley-Fisher, J. A. Assembled data in eye movements. Opt. Acta (Lond.) 14, 113–118 (1967).
Acknowledgements
We thank Y. Duran for technical assistance and X. G. Troncoso for comments on the manuscript. This study was funded by the Barrow Neurological Foundation and the National Eye Institute.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Related links
Glossary
- ENTOPTIC STRUCTURES
-
Structures within the eye. When these become visible they give rise to entoptic images.
- NYSTAGMUS
-
Involuntary rhythmical oscillations of one or both eyes.
- FOVEA
-
The retinal region with maximal concentration of photoreceptors, where visual acuity is highest.
- FLICKER FUSION THRESHOLD
-
The rate of flicker at which the flickering stimulus being viewed appears non-flickering (approximately 50–60 Hz in humans).
- CONJUGATE
-
Coordinated in the two eyes.
- MAIN SEQUENCE
-
The linear correlation between saccadic speed and amplitude.
- RECEPTIVE FIELD
-
The area of the sensory space in which stimulus presentation leads to the response of a particular sensory neuron.
- BINOCULAR DISPARITY
-
The difference in gaze position of the two eyes that gives rise to stereovision.
- VISUAL MASKING
-
An illusion in which a normally visible target object is rendered invisible by a mask object.
- NEURAL CODE
-
The language, expressed as a pattern of neuronal impulses, that neurons use to send information to each other.
- EXTRARETINAL ACTIVATION
-
Responses in the visual system that occur in the absence of visual stimuli (such as one might see due to feedback from motor areas).
- SPATIAL SUMMATION
-
The way in which non-overlapping retinal stimulation is integrated within dendrites to produce a stronger neuronal response.
- BURSTS
-
Clusters of action potentials.
- TEMPORAL SUMMATION
-
The way in which non-simultaneous synaptic events combine in time. One of the basic elements of synaptic integration.
- LONG, TIGHT BURST
-
A type of burst consisting of a large number of spikes that occur in rapid succession.
- SCOTOPIC CONDITIONS
-
Dim light conditions in which only the rods of the retina are active.
- PHOTOPIC CONDITIONS
-
Bright light conditions in which only the cones of the retina are active.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Martinez-Conde, S., Macknik, S. & Hubel, D. The role of fixational eye movements in visual perception. Nat Rev Neurosci 5, 229–240 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1348
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1348
This article is cited by
-
Active fixation as an efficient coding strategy for neuromorphic vision
Scientific Reports (2023)
-
Fixational drift is driven by diffusive dynamics in central neural circuitry
Nature Communications (2022)
-
Anticipation During a Cyclic Manufacturing Process: Toward Visual Search Modeling of Human Factors
The Review of Socionetwork Strategies (2022)
-
Eye tracking identifies biomarkers in α-synucleinopathies versus progressive supranuclear palsy
Journal of Neurology (2022)