Introduction

Although symptom-based diagnoses under DSM and ICD guidelines have proven invaluable both in the clinic and for research, it has been recognized by the National Institutes of Mental Health (NIMH) that this ‘Kraepelinian’ nosological framework may not accurately capture underlying mechanisms that underlie psychiatric dysfunction in that these processes may cross diagnostic boundaries (Insel et al, 2010). For example, studies have observed shared biological and psychological features across bipolar disorder (BPD) and schizophrenia (SZ) diagnoses despite the fact that these disorders are classified by the DSM as distinct entities. These shared features include varying degrees of common genetic susceptibility loci (Craddock et al, 2006), changes in neuronal structure and function (Konopaske et al, 2014; McIntosh et al, 2009; O'Donoghue et al, 2017; Whalley et al, 2012), and cognitive disability (Bora and Pantelis, 2015). Psychosis is another particularly striking feature that is often shared between SZ and BPD (Type I), and consequently the same antipsychotics are often prescribed to treat the two diseases (Pearlson, 2015; Stahl, 2014; Tamminga et al, 2014). The degree to which these disorders may lie along a single continuum and consequently be manifestations of shared biological processes is therefore a major research question in psychiatry.

Cognitive deficits in BPD and SZ (and their associated neuronal mechanisms) are particularly important areas for further research given that no FDA-approved treatment exists for these deficits even though they predict functional outcome in both diseases (Green, 2006). Interestingly, one aspect of cognition in particular, cognitive control (CC), may be an especially intriguing potential target due the theory that it is a domain-generalizable process, such that a loss of CC may help explain dysfunction across a wide range of cognitive domains (Lesh et al, 2011; Niendam et al, 2012), as well other symptoms (Cole et al, 2014). Broadly speaking, CC refers to processes by which behavior can adapt from moment to moment based on current goals even in the face of stimuli from the environment that elicit habitual responses (Lesh et al, 2011). CC in this context should not be confused with the 'cognitive control' biotype isolated by cluster analysis by the Bipolar Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP) consortium (Clementz et al, 2016). In the present study, CC refers to overarching cognitive processes and not task performance-defined patient groups. CC encapsulates a range of functions, including goal maintenance and adaptive or reactive control (Lesh et al, 2011). Of these, the Cognitive Neuroscience Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (CNTRICS; http://cntrics.ucdavis.edu) initiative has identified goal maintenance as an important target for treatment development in SZ (Carter et al, 2012).

As suggested by CNTRICS, the AX version of the continuous performance task (AX-CPT) has become a standard method of examining the mechanisms of goal maintenance of CC in SZ. In the AX-CPT, subjects are presented with a series of cues and probes and are instructed to respond to a probe only if it was preceded by the target (‘A’) cue. Goal maintenance is measured by comparing neuronal response to A (low control) cues vs non-A (high control, ‘B’) cues. Previous AX-CPT studies have observed impaired goal maintenance, as well as diminished recruitment of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and superior parietal cortex (SPC), in SZ (Lesh et al, 2013, 2015; Niendam et al, 2014). In regard to BPD, a previous AX-CPT behavioral study reported that BPD patients performed (AX errors) intermediately between controls and SZ patients, suggestive of a continuum of deficits across disorders (Brambilla et al, 2007). A more recent behavioral study in a much larger sample using the dot probe expectancy task (DPX; an AX-CPT variant) found the same pattern of CC deficits driven by significant differences between HC, BPD, and SZ groups (Reilly et al, 2017).

The goal of the present study is to test the hypothesis that deficits that underlie goal maintenance across BPD and SZ may be described as a continuum of dysfunction that transcends DSM-based classification. Specifically, we hypothesize (1) the presence of impaired goal maintenance of CC in patients (vs controls), and (2) while SZ and BPD would show the same pattern of cognitive and functional neural circuitry deficits, this effect would vary in severity across the recent onset groups, with a significant linear trend reflecting CC in BPD that lies between controls and SZ patients. This pattern would be consistent with previous AX-CPT behavioral results (Brambilla et al, 2007) cognitive meta-analyses (Bora and Pantelis, 2015; Reichenberg and Harvey, 2007; Robinson et al, 2006), and several findings from the Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP) consortium (Hill et al, 2013; Ivleva et al, 2016; Reilly et al, 2017; Sheffield et al, 2017). As we were interested in common phenomenological endpoints (eg, psychosis, possibly suggestive of shared etiologies) we restricted our analysis to BPD Type I patients (who presented with psychotic features). We also considered this BPD variant to be clinically most ‘similar’ to SZ (rather than BPD Type II, for which depressive features are most prevalent) due to the increased genetic similarity between SZ and BPD relative to SZ and major depressive disorder (Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics et al, 2013).

Materials and methods

Subjects

In total 147 subjects (ages 16–30) participated in this study—70 patients with either SZ (n=58), schizophreniform (n=6) or schizoaffective (n=6) disorder, 24 patients with BPD Type I with psychotic features, and 53 healthy control subjects. Of the present sample, data from 43 controls and 32 SZ patients were included in a previous AX-CPT study that focused exclusively on SZ (Lesh et al, 2013), from 16 controls and 16 patients in another AX-CPT study in SZ (Yoon et al, 2008), and from 15 controls and 24 patients in a third AX-CPT study in SZ and high-risk subjects (Niendam et al, 2014). All patients were recruited as outpatients from the University of California, Davis (UCD) Early Diagnosis and Preventive Treatment (of Psychosis) (EDAPT) research clinic, with their first psychotic episode occurring within 1 year of study. Patients were assessed with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR (SCID) (First et al, 2002). Patients were followed longitudinally and diagnoses were confirmed by SCID and consensus conference 12 months after ascertainment. Patients were excluded for a diagnosis of major medical or neurological illness, head trauma, substance abuse in the previous 3 months (as well as a positive urinalysis on the day of scanning), Weschler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence-2 score (WASI) (Weschler, 1999) score<70, and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) exclusion criteria (eg, claustrophobia, metal in the body). Control subjects were excluded for all of the above as well as a history of Axis I mental illness or first-degree family history of psychosis. All participants provided written informed consent and were compensated for participation. The UCD Institutional Review Board approved the study. CPZ equivalent antipsychotic doses were calculated using published guidelines for conventional (American Psychiatric Association, 1997) and atypical (Woods, 2003) antipsychotics. Consistent with prior work (Barch et al, 2003), three core symptom dimensions were calculated. ‘Poverty’ combined emotional withdrawal, motor retardation, and blunted affect from the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) (Ventura et al, 1993) with anhedonia/asociality, avolition/apathy, alogia, and affective flattening from the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) (Andreasen, 1984a). ‘Disorganization’ combined conceptual disorganization, mannerisms and posturing, and disorientation scores from the BPRS with attention score from the SANS as well as positive formal thought disorder and bizarre behavior scores from the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS) (Andreasen, 1984b). ‘Reality distortion’ combined grandiosity, suspiciousness, hallucinations, and unusual thought content from the BPRS with hallucinations and delusions from the SAPS (Barch et al, 2003). It should be noted that these symptom dimensions have variable long-term stability (Arndt et al, 1995; Marengo et al, 2000) and we must therefore remain agnostic as to the degree of state or trait-like clinical status of the present sample.

Task Description

The AX-CPT and associated task parameters have been described in detail elsewhere (Cohen et al, 1999; Lesh et al, 2013). Briefly, participants are presented with a series of cues and probes and are instructed to make a target response (pressing a button with the index finger) to the probe letter ‘X’ only if it was preceded by the cue letter ‘A’. All cues and nontarget probes require nontarget responses (pressing a button with the middle finger). Target sequence trials (ie, ‘AX’ trials) are frequent (70% occurrence) and set up a prepotent tendency to make a target response when the probe letter X occurs. As a result, nontarget sequence trials in which any non-A cue (collectively called ‘B’ cues) is presented and followed by a probe letter X (ie, ‘BX’ trials) require the most cognitive control. AY and BY trials are included to test for generalized deficits and represent an internal control. Cues and probes were presented for 500 ms, with a 3500 ms interval between cue and probe and a 9500 ms intertrial interval. Trial distribution was 70% AX, 10% AY, 12.5% BX, 7.5% BY. The task consisted of 4 runs of 40 trials for a total of 160 trials and a total time of 37 min 20 s. The task was presented using EPrime2 software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.).

The primary behavioral outcome measure of interest was d-prime context, calculated as a function of AX hits minus BX false alarms (Cohen et al, 1999). AX-CPT d-prime context has been shown to have moderate to good reliability, with intraclass coefficients between 0.73 and 0.78 depending on delay time and 0.81 when collapsing across delay time in a pool of healthy control subjects, psychotic non-SZ (primarily mood disorder) patients, and SZ patients (Barch et al, 2009). D-prime context (z-score) was analyzed by ANCOVA with group and gender as between-subjects factors (due to gender imbalance between groups). As part of the ANCOVA, two a priori contrasts were performed. The first contrast compared healthy controls to patients (as a whole) to test the hypothesis that patients would show overall deficits in d-prime context. The second contrast was a polynomial linear trend analysis {controls>BPD>SZ} to test the hypothesis that BPD patients would show intermediate goal maintenance-related performance deficits relative to SZ patients, indicative of a continuum of deficits. The null hypothesis of this orthogonal contrast is a zero slope. To more completely characterize group effects, auxiliary pairwise post-hoc tests were also performed comparing d-prime context between each group (eg, SZ vs BPD patients). Significance for post-hoc tests was set to p<0.05; p values between 0.05 and 0.10 were considered trend-level.

An exploratory analysis of auxiliary performance metrics (accuracy and reaction time) was performed by repeated measures ANCOVA, with trial type (AX/AY/BX/BY) as a within-subjects factor and diagnosis and gender as between-subjects factors. Contrasts were not performed as part of this analysis as we did not have a priori hypotheses regarding these measures.

fMRI Scanning

Studies were performed with at 1.5 T GE Signa MR system using a standard head coil. Functional images were acquired with a gradient-echo T2* Blood Oxygenation Level Dependent (BOLD) contrast technique, with TR=2000 ms, TE=40 ms, FOV=220 mm2, flip angle=90°, and 24 axial slices (contiguous and interleaved, 4 mm thick, with a 3.4 mm2 in-plane resolution).

fMRI Preprocessing

fMR images were preprocessed using SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London). Briefly, images were slice-timing corrected, realigned, normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute template using a rigid-body transformation followed by non-linear warping, and smoothed with an 8 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. All individual fMRI runs had <4 mm of translational within-run movement, <3 degrees of rotational within-run movement, and <0.45 mm of average framewise displacement (calculated using the fsl_motion_outliers tool) (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/FSLMotionOutliers). All subjects had at least two fMRI runs surviving these criteria.

Region-of-Interest Analysis of BOLD Response

First-level effects were modeled with a double-gamma function, without temporal derivatives, using the general linear model in SPM8. ‘Task-associated’ (B>A Cue) contrast images (parameter estimates) were generated for each subject. The B>A contrast measures response under conditions of high vs low cognitive control (Lesh et al, 2015; Lesh et al, 2013). Images were motion-corrected using rigid-body motion parameters as single-subject regressors. All trial types were modeled (AX/AY/BX/BY) and only correct responses were used to create first-level images consistent with previous studies (Lesh et al, 2013, 2015).

Using first-level images, a priori hypotheses of task-associated response were examined in two bilateral, 4 mm radius spherical region-of-interests (ROIs): the DLPFC and SPC. As ROI size was chosen arbitrarily, the analysis was repeated for 6 and 8 mm radii. The DLPFC ROI was taken from a previous study from an independent Stroop task data set (MacDonald et al, 2000). The SPC ROI was taken from a previous meta-analysis of non n-back studies of executive function in SZ (Minzenberg et al, 2009). Mean task-associated response (z-score) in these ROIs was extracted using the Marsbar toolbox (Brett et al, 2002) and examined by ANCOVA with diagnosis and gender as between-subjects factors. As was the case for behavioral data, two a priori contrasts were performed. The first contrast compared healthy controls to patients (as a whole) to test the hypothesis that patients would show overall deficits in task-associated activity within these ROIs. The second contrast was a polynomial linear trend analysis {controls > BPD > SZ} to test the hypothesis that BPD patients would show mitigated BOLD response deficits relative to SZ patients, indicative of a continuum of dysfunction. As described for d-prime context, auxiliary pairwise post-hoc tests were also performed comparing ROI BOLD response between each group.

Whole-Brain Analysis

In addition to ROI analysis, an exploratory whole-brain analysis was conducted by ANOVA using the factors and contrasts defined above. Significance was set to voxelwise p<0.001 (uncorrected), clusterwise p<0.05 (FWE-corrected for multiple comparisons).

Behavioral and Clinical Correlates

Associations between CC-related (d-prime context, task-associated DLPFC and SPC ROI activation) and clinical (poverty, disorganization, reality distortion) measures were examined using Spearman's rho correlation coefficients as not all data were normally distributed. An additional analysis was conducted examining the relationship between antipsychotic dose and CC.

Correlates were analyzed across groups, as we were interested in associations that crossed diagnostic categories consistent with the RDoC framework. To ensure that correlations were not driven by group differences, associations were adjusted for the effect of group by calculating normalized residuals (z-scores) from the linear regression of group to each measure prior to correlation analyses. As these analyses were exploratory, significance was set to p<0.05 (uncorrected). P values between 0.05 and 0.10 were considered trend-level.

Results

Demographics

Demographic and clinical information for participants are shown in Table 1. Groups did not differ significantly by age or handedness but did differ by gender. Gender was therefore included as a factor in all between-group analyses. Results from other demographic and clinical between-group comparisons are provided in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic and Clinical Information for Participants

Behavioral Results

For all analyses, subjects with d-prime context or ROI BOLD response outliers>4 SD below the group mean were censored from analysis (2 SZ patients). Notably, inclusion of these outliers did not appreciably alter group results (slightly increased significance for contrasts of interest).

Performance metrics for the AX-CPT for each group are presented (as a dot plot) in Figure 1 and Supplementary Table 1. All subjects performed adequately on the task, suggesting they understood the instructions and were performing the task (‘adequately’ was defined as accuracy greater than or equal to 44% on AX trials and 50% on BY trials) (Henderson et al, 2012). For d-prime context, the overall model (including gender) was significant (F(5,139)=6.04, p<0.001) as was the main effect of group (F(2,139)=10.80, p<0.001). No main effect of gender or gender X diagnosis interaction was observed. The a priori contrast testing the hypothesis of deficits in patients (controls>patients) for d-prime context was significant (contrast estimate=−0.63, 95% CI={−0.98 to −0.28}, p=0.001). The a priori contrast testing the hypothesis of a linear trend (controls>BPD>SZ) was also significant (contrast estimate=−0.53, 95% CI={−0.75 to −0.30}, p<0.001). Pairwise post-hoc tests revealed significant differences between controls and SZ patients (p<0.001), as well as SZ and BPD patients (p=0.044). A trend-level difference was observed between controls and BPD patients (p=0.067).

Figure 1
figure 1

Plot of individual d-prime context scores for each diagnostic group on the AX-CPT. Straight line represents the slope of the linear contrast HC>BPD>SZ.

PowerPoint slide

For auxiliary behavioral measures, significant diagnosis (controls vs SZ vs BPD) X trial type (AX vs AY vs BX vs BY) interactions were observed for accuracy (Wilks' λ=0.88, F(6,274)=3.16, p=0.005) and reaction time (Wilks' λ=0.87, F(6,274)=2.96, p=0.008; Supplementary Table 1). No main effects of gender or gender X diagnosis interactions were observed for either accuracy or reaction time.

fMRI Results: ROI Analysis

Statistical parametric maps of task-associated response and dot plots showing distribution of response for each group and clusters from the linear contrast (control>BPD>SZ) are shown in Figure 2. Group means and standard deviations are presented in Supplementary Table 2. For task-associated response of the DLPFC, the overall model (including gender) was significant (F(5,139)=2.77, p=0.020) as was the main effect of group (F(2,139)=4.27, p=0.016). No effect of gender or gender X group interaction was observed. The a priori contrast testing the hypothesis of DLPFC BOLD response deficits in patients (controls>patients) was significant (contrast estimate=−0.54, 95% C.I.={−0.91 to −0.17}, p=0.004). The a priori contrast testing the hypothesis of a linear trend (controls>BPD>SZ) was also significant (contrast estimate=−0.43, 95% C.I.={−0.70 to −0.16}, p=0.002). Pairwise post-hoc tests revealed significant differences between controls and SZ patients (p=0.002), as well as a trend-level difference between controls and BPD patients (p=0.063). No trend-level or greater difference was observed between SZ and BPD patients.

Figure 2
figure 2

Left: Statistical parametric t map of task-associated BOLD response demonstrating significant linear trends (HC>BPD>SZ directional contrast) on DLPFC and SPC signal. Map masked by a whole brain gray matter mask and thresholded at whole-brain voxelwise p<0.001, k>50 voxels for visualization purposes. Map displayed in the neurologic convention (R on R). Top Right: Plot of individual task-associated BOLD response (beta weights) within the bilateral DLPFC ROI for each diagnostic group. Straight line represents the slope of the linear contrast HC>BPD>SZ. Bottom Right: Plot of task-associated BOLD response (beta weights) within the bilateral SPC ROI for each diagnostic group. Straight line represents the slope of the linear contrast HC>BPD>SZ.

PowerPoint slide

For the bilateral SPC ROI, although the overall model (including gender) did not reach significance (F(5,139)=1.85, p=0.11) the main effect of group was significant (F(2,139)=3.66, p=0.028) and no effect of gender or gender X group interaction was observed. The a priori contrast testing the hypothesis of SPC BOLD response deficits in patients (controls>patients) was significant (contrast estimate=−0.49, 95% C.I.={−0.86 to −0.11}, p=0.011). The a priori contrast testing the hypothesis of a linear trend (controls>BPD>SZ) was also significant (contrast estimate=−0.42, 95% C.I.={−0.69 to −0.14}, p=0.003). Pairwise post-hoc tests revealed significant differences between controls and SZ patients (p=0.005), a trend-level difference between controls and BPD patients (p=0.058), and no trend-level or greater difference between SZ and BPD patients.

Increasing ROI size to a 6 mm or 8 mm radius did not appreciably alter contrast results (Supplementary Table 2).

fMRI Results: Whole Brain Analysis

The a priori contrast testing the hypothesis of BOLD response deficits in patients (controls>patients) yielded significant clusters in the left DLPFC, right DLPFC, left SPC, right SPC, and anterior cingulate (ACC) (Supplementary Table 3a; Supplementary Figure 1a). The a priori contrast testing the hypothesis of a linear trend (controls>BPD>SZ) yielded significant clusters in the left DLPFC, right DLPFC, and right SPC (Supplementary Table 3b).

As a significant ACC cluster was observed using the control>patient contrast but not the linear trend contrast (control>BPD>SZ), the nature of the control>patient effect was further explored by extracting BOLD response from a 1 mm radius sphere centered at the cluster peak (x,y,z=8, 20, 48). This analysis showed the ACC cluster was driven by a different pattern in which SZ patients showed intermediate deficits between controls and BPD patients (Supplementary Figure 1b).

Clinical Correlates

After controlling for group differences, across patient groups lower d-prime context was associated with higher disorganization symptoms (rho=−0.21, p=0.048; Figure 3, top). A trend-level relationship was also observed between lower d-prime context and higher poverty symptoms (rho=−0.20, p=0.059; Figure 3, bottom). No relationship was observed between d-prime context and reality distortion symptoms. No significant relationships were observed between task-associated BOLD response and symptoms. No relationships were observed with antipsychotic dose.

Figure 3
figure 3

Associations between lower d-prime context (group-adjusted z-scores) and disorganization (group-adjusted z-scores) (top) and poverty (group-adjusted z-scores) (bottom) across patient groups. Subjects are color-coded by group for visualization purposes.

PowerPoint slide

Discussion

In agreement with our hypothesis, significant group effects were observed on performance on the AX-CPT task as well as goal maintenance-associated response in the DLPFC and SPC. A priori contrasts supported the hypothesis that recent onset patients (as a whole) showed deficits in d-prime context and CC-associated frontoparietal BOLD response in these regions. Furthermore, in support of our prediction of a continuum of dysfunction (with more severe deficits in SZ), linear contrasts of CC-related behavior within predefined frontoparietal (DLPFC and SPC) ROIs reflected a pattern of deficits in BPD patients that was intermediate between controls and SZ patients. Exploratory whole-brain analysis yielded an additional significant cluster in the ACC using the contrast controls>patients, although this cluster was driven by the pattern HC>SZ>BPD. Significant or trend-level associations were observed between poverty and disorganization symptoms and poor task performance across BPD and SZ patient groups, suggesting these relationships may transcend diagnostic categories. Importantly, these findings enrich those from previous behavioral AX-CPT studies (Brambilla et al, 2007; Reilly et al, 2017) by suggesting that the previously reported intermediate deficits in BPD (relative to SZ and healthy subjects) are also associated with reductions in frontoparietal activation.

To place these findings in context, it may be helpful to consider a recent commentary by Cole et al (2014). Cole and colleagues argue that a dysfunctional frontoparietal control system may be explicitly tied to symptomatology. Specifically, the authors suggest that in psychiatric disease, the cognitive control system is less able to regulate other functional neural systems in a goal-directed manner, leading to behavioral changes that manifest themselves as diagnosable symptoms. For example, behavioral disorganization, for which relationships to CC processes have been shown previously (Yoon et al, 2008) and in this study, may be an emerging consequence of the inability of the CC system to guide goal-directed behavior. Within this framework the reverse relationship may also exist in that in psychiatric disease, the CC system needs to expend resources in an attempt to manage symptoms and consequently may be less able to be recruited during CC tasks. Importantly, within this framework loss of frontoparietal-directed CC is expected to manifest itself across diagnostic boundaries in a manner consistent with NIMH Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) (Insel et al, 2010). Although the direction of potential causal relationships between CC and clinical indicators is beyond the scope of this study, our findings suggest that CC abnormalities exist across diagnostic spectra and are correlated with some aspects of symptomatology.

Significant group differences in task-associated response were observed in both the DLPFC and SPC, suggesting that different subsystems of the CC network may be affected across disorders. Previous work has demonstrated that the DLPFC is involved in maintaining goal representations (Rougier et al, 2005), whereas the SPC is important for top-down control of visual attention and representing stimulus-response mappings (Bunge et al, 2003). The present results suggest, therefore, that both processes are affected in BPD Type I (with psychotic features) and SZ. The observed pattern of deficits is also consistent with previous findings in BPD and SZ during working memory (Brandt et al, 2014; Hamilton et al, 2009), verbal fluency (Costafreda et al, 2011), and during the ‘resting’ state (absence of a task) (Baker et al, 2014). At odds with these findings, however, is a previous report during a sentence completion task that showed DLFPC hypoactivation in SZ patients but hyperactivation in BPD (McIntosh et al, 2008). While differences in clinical characteristics between that study and the present work (eg disparity of ~8 in mean YMRS scores of BPD groups) could explain these discrepant results, this discordancy suggests that the observed patterns of deficits observed in this study may not generalize across all cognitive domains or tasks. Prefrontal tasks that involve emotional cues or recruit emotion-related circuits, for example, may evoke hyperactive responses in BPD relative to other psychiatric groups or control subjects (Strakowski et al, 2011). Future studies may examine the effect of emotional distractors or cues on task-associated response during CC performance in order to examine this possibility, as well as the generalizability of the findings to other related clinical groups (eg, BPD Type II or those without a history of psychotic features).

Exploratory whole-brain analysis using the controls>patients (as a whole) contrast yielded an additional significant cluster in the ACC. Interestingly, this cluster appeared to be driven by a different pattern in which SZ patients showed deficits in-between that of controls and BPD. Consistent with the finding of a functional ACC deficit in psychosis, a 2009 meta-analysis of 41 studies of executive function showed loss of ACC recruitment in SZ (Minzenberg et al, 2009). Reduced ACC activity during a CC task has been observed in BPD as well (eg, Gruber et al, 2017). As the ACC is thought to be involved with performance monitoring and (not goal maintenance) of CC (Lesh et al, 2011) and was also not previously recruited during similar CC tasks (Lopez-Garcia et al, 2016), we did not hypothesize to observe differential task-associated response in this region in this study and this result therefore requires follow-up using an independent dataset. Nonetheless, this preliminary finding suggests that dysfunctional brain circuitry in SZ and BPD may not all show the same pattern of deficits and consequently BPD should be not be simply thought of as a generalized ‘less severe’ form of SZ. Rather, the HC>BPD>SZ functional pattern may be specific to the frontoparietal CC network previously implicated in SZ (Cole et al, 2014).

The results of this study do not necessarily suggest that the pattern of cognitive deficits in SZ and BP is specific to goal maintenance and CC. Indeed, the B-SNIP consortium has reported the same pattern of deficits/abnormalities in BPD and SZ using a generalized neurocognitive battery (Hill et al, 2013). In that study the same pattern of deficits persisted across all cognitive domains tested (Hill et al, 2013). Nonetheless, as abnormal CC processes are known to contribute to cognitive deficits across a range of domains (Lesh et al, 2011; Niendam et al, 2012), it is likely that the results observed in the B-SNIP study may be driven by the CC deficits reported here. Related to this point, another B-SNIP report found that generalized cognitive deficits accounted for reduced goal maintenance in BPD and SZ during the DPX (Reilly et al, 2017), although the reverse relationship (the influence of goal maintenance on general cognition) was not examined.

A potential limitation of the present study is that differences between patients and controls may be confounded by antipsychotic treatment. Although no relationships were observed between antipsychotic dose and any behavioral or fMRI-related measure in patients (after correcting for group differences), the impact of antipsychotic medication on the observed linear trends cannot be fully ascertained as the control group was not taking antipsychotics. This question may be more thoroughly addressed in a future case-control or longitudinal CC-related fMRI study designed to specifically examine antipsychotic dose-response curves. As noted by Pearlson (2015), symptoms (particularly positive symptoms) often respond to the same medication regardless of diagnosis, and it is possible that any treatment designed to target CC deficits will show efficacy across categorical distinctions.

BPD and SZ share neurochemical and neuroanatomical features in cortical areas that may help explain common deficits in CC processes. Both diseases are associated with abnormal expression of GABAergic (Volk et al, 2016), cholinergic (Kunii et al, 2015), and glutamatergic markers (Beneyto and Meador-Woodruff, 2008), as well as abnormalities in various second-messenger systems eg calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II-dependent signaling (Robison, 2014). Decreased prefrontal gyrification and dendritic spine density has also been observed in both disorders (Konopaske et al, 2014; McIntosh et al, 2009). The contribution(s) these abnormalities make (if any) to CC deficits in BPD and SZ is an important area for future study as it may help identify pharmacologic mechanisms by which to target these deficits across diseases.

Although BPD and SZ are classified as distinct disorders it is increasingly recognized that the two illnesses share common features in biological and cognitive processes. From a biological point of view, this perspective may not be surprising given the degree of genetic overlap and heritability between the two diseases (Cardno and Owen, 2014; Lee et al, 2013; Pearlson, 2015). Nonetheless, it is important to note that clinically the two diseases have numerous distinctive features (outside of symptoms) such as differences in developmental trajectory, treatment response, and clinical course (Murray et al, 2004). In this study only a single dimensional construct was investigated (the goal maintenance function of CC). It is possible (and perhaps highly likely) that along other dimensions with unique behavioral and neural correlates, such as reward processing and emotion regulation, there may be categorical distinctions between the two syndromes. While the present results provide a validation of the approach of examining dimensional measures across diagnostic boundaries and also suggest a continuum of CC dysfunction and its neural underpinnings across BPD and SZ, additional future work examining these and other constructs will be needed to develop a comprehensive understanding of the psychopathology of severe mental illness.

Conclusions

In the present study we tested the hypothesis that functional neurocognitive deficits during CC of goal maintenance transverse traditional diagnostic criteria in BPD and SZ. These results suggest that CC dysfunction may be described as a continuum of deficits across these diseases, supporting an RDoC-based framework of understanding this important aspect of these illnesses.

Funding and disclosure

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.