Introduction

Numerous points of evidence spanning many years have implicated the lateral hypothalamus (LH) in modulating the consumption of rewarding stimuli. The LH was originally implicated in modulating feeding behavior by evidence demonstrating that lesions of the LH reduced food consumption while electrical stimulation of this region promoted food intake (Anand and Brobeck, 1951; Delgado and Anand, 1953). It was later shown that rodents will work for electrical stimulation of LH neurons in proportion to their energy balance (Hoebel and Teitelbaum, 1962; Hoebel and Thompson, 1969), direct evidence that the LH is critical in reward-seeking behaviors. Since these original observations, research has shown that the LH not only modulates consummatory behaviors directed at natural rewards, such as food and water, but is also a critical region in supporting the consumption of drugs, including ethanol (Leibowitz, 2007; Sprow et al, 2015). In fact, the LH is often considered to be a key player woven into proposed reward neurocircuitry, which also includes the mesolimbic dopaminergic system and the extended amygdala (Koob, 2003; Koob and Le Moal, 2001).

The role of GABAergic receptor signaling within the LH in modulating consummatory behaviors has been well studied using pharmacological approaches (Kelly et al, 1977; Turenius et al, 2009a). Although pharmacological approaches establish that GABA receptor signaling in the LH modulates consummatory behaviors, conclusions regarding the sources of GABA innervation, or the direct role of local GABAergic neurons, are not possible using traditional pharmacological approaches. Historically, direct manipulation of specific cell populations was challenging, but recently developed optogenetic and chemogenetic approaches allow one to achieve this goal. In one recent example, Jennings et al (2015) used a combination of optogenetic, chemogenetic, and neuronal ablation procedures to show that activation of LH GABAergic neurons increased, and inhibition or ablation of GABAergic neurons decreased, appetitive (food-seeking) and consummatory (eating) behaviors.

The stimulus rewards used in the experiments conducted by Jennings et al (2015) were either food or a caloric-dense liquid solution. As such, one interpretation of their data is that the LH GABAergic neurons under investigation regulate behaviors directed at maintaining caloric balance. To directly test this interpretation, we performed a series of studies utilizing a chemogenetic approach. Designer receptors exclusively activated by designer drugs (DREADDs) are genetically modified muscarinic receptors that have lost their affinity for endogenous acetylcholine but can be activated by an otherwise physiologically inert ligand, clozapine-N-oxide (CNO). DREADDs have been developed that are coupled through the Gq or Gi pathways to stimulate or inhibit neuronal activity, respectively (Armbruster et al, 2007). DREADDs are packaged in adeno-associated viral (AAV) vectors and have been made to be cre-inducible. Thus when combined with transgenic cre-line mice, one can target DREADDs to specific neuronal populations. In the present report, we injected DREADD viral vectors into the LH of vGat-ires-cre mice (Vong et al, 2011) to selectively express DREADDs on GABAergic neurons in the LH. Here we found that activation of LH GABAergic neurons increased consumption of any available stimulus, including those entailing calories (food, sucrose, and ethanol), those lacking calories (saccharin and water), and those lacking biological relevance.

Materials and methods

Animals

All mice used in the present work were transgenic VGat-ires-cre mice on a C57BL/6J genetic background obtained from Dr Bradford Lowell (Vong et al, 2011). Four cohorts of mice were used: Cohort 1 (14 male and 14 female mice), Cohort 2 (6 male and 8 female mice), Cohort 3 (11 male and 9 female mice), and Cohort 4 (15 male mice). Mice were individually housed in plastic cages located in a vivarium with an ambient temperature of approximately 22 °C and reverse light/dark cycle with lights off at 10:00 hours. Food (IsoPro RMH 3000, Purina LabDiet, St Louis, MO) and water were available ad libitum except during testing (see below). All procedures used were in accordance with the National Institute of Health guidelines and were approved by the University of North Carolina Institutional Animal care and Use Committee.

Surgery

Mice were bilaterally microinjected with 0.5 μl/side of purified and concentrated AAV into the LH or the zona incerta (ZI). For the LH surgeries, the following stereotaxic coordinates were used: −1.2 mm posterior to bregma, ±1.2 mm lateral to midline, and −5.1 mm ventral to the skull surface. For the ZI surgeries, the stereotaxic coordinates were: −1.2 mm posterior to bregma, ±1.2 mm lateral to midline, and −4.26 mm ventral to the skull surface. Details on the DREADD virus can be found in the Supplementary Section.

Mouse Cohort 1: Binge-Like Ethanol Drinking, Food, and Water Consumption and Open-Field Locomotor Activity

We used a 4-day ‘drinking-in-the-dark’ (DID) procedure with the first cohort of mice, an animal model of binge-like ethanol drinking which promotes high levels of ethanol that are associated with blood ethanol concentrations (BECs) in excess of 80 mg/dl in a 2–4-h testing period (Rhodes et al, 2005; Thiele and Navarro, 2014). Procedural details for the DID procedure have recently been described elsewhere (Thiele et al, 2014). Briefly, on days 1–3, beginning 3 h into the dark cycle, water bottles were removed from all cages and replaced with bottles containing a 20% (v/v) ethanol solution. Mice had 2 h of access to ethanol after which ethanol bottles were removed from cages and water bottles were replaced.

Before stereotaxic surgery was performed, all animals went through one 4-day DID cycle. Consumption data collected during this initial test allowed us to distribute the animals into three groups in such a way that baseline levels of ethanol intake were similar between groups: mice were given bilateral infusions into the LH (as described above) of the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mcherry) viral vector (n=9), the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mcherry viral vector (n=10), or the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-mcherry viral vector (n=9). For surgery, mice were anesthetized with a cocktail of Ketamine (117 mg/kg) and xylazine (7.92 mg/kg). After a 21-day recovery period to allow time for virus transduction, DID testing procedures were initiated. On days 1–3, mice were habituated to intraperitoneal (i.p.) injections (5 ml/kg) by giving injection of a 0.5% DMSO solution in isotonic saline (the vehicle for CNO) just before ethanol access. The same schedule was followed on day 4, except that ethanol access was extended to 4 h and i.p. injections consisted of vehicle or CNO (3 mg/kg in a 5 ml/kg injection volume). After the 4-h consumption period in which consumption measures were recorded hourly, tail blood samples were collected from the tail vein of each animal. Blood samples were centrifuged, and 5 μl of plasma were analyzed for BECs using the Analox Alcohol Analyzer (Analox Instruments USA, Lunenburg, MA). After 3 days of rest, mice were run through another 4-day DID procedure as described above except mice were given an i.p. injection of the alternate solution (vehicle or CNO) that they did not receive during the first DID test, constituting a Latin square experimental design.

After binge-like ethanol consumption testing, these same mice were used to assess the effects of LH GABAergic neuronal manipulation on food and water consumption. Mice were given an i.p. injection of vehicle or CNO in the same dose and volume described above, and then food measure were collected hourly over 4 h beginning 3 h into the dark cycle. Mice were injected with vehicle or CNO in a Latin square design, with approximately 1 week between injections. Over the next 2 weeks, this same approach was used to assess the effects of vehicle or CNO injection on water consumption over a 2-h period beginning 3 h into the dark cycle. Finally, over the final 2 weeks the effects of vehicle or CNO injection on open-field activity were assessed over a 2-h period beginning 3 h into the dark cycle. Mice were injected with both vehicle and CNO in a Latin square design, with each injection separated by 1 week. Mice were then perfused using a standard procedure in our laboratory (Navarro et al, 2008), and brains were section and slide mounted to verify the placement of viral vectors.

Mouse Cohort 2: Binge-Like Saccharin and Sucrose Consumption

Mice from cohort 2 were randomly distributed into two groups and given bilateral infusions into the LH of either the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mcherry viral vector (n=8) or the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mcherry viral vector (n=6) using the same procedures described above. After a 3-week recovery period, the effects of vehicle or CNO injection on consumption of saccharin (0.15%) or sucrose (3%) were assessed. The consumption procedures were identical to DID procedures described above, except water was replaced by saccharin or sucrose over a 2-h test beginning 3 h into the dark cycle. A Latin square design was used such that each animal received both vehicle and CNO on separate test days separated by a week. Mice were first tested with the saccharin solution for the first 2 weeks and then with the sucrose over the next 2 weeks. Mice were then perfused to verify the placement of viral vectors.

Mouse Cohort 3: Binge-Like Ethanol Drinking and Food Intake Following GABAergic Neuronal Manipulations in the ZI

DID and food intake procedures were identical to those outlined for mouse cohort 1 above. After a presurgery run through DID procedures, mice were distributed into three groups based on ethanol consumption as above and treated with one of the three viral vectors aimed at the ZI: AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mcherry (n=7), AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mcherry (n=7), or AAV8-hSyn-DIO-mcherry (n=6). Mice were then tested for the effects of vehicle or CNO injections (presented in a Latin square design as above) on binge-like ethanol drinking and then food intake. Mice were then perfused to verify the placement of viral vectors.

Mouse Cohort 4: Wood Gnawing Behavior

Mice from cohort 4 were randomly distributed into two groups and given bilateral infusions into the LH of either the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mcherry viral vector (n=8) or the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-mcherry viral vector (n=7) using the same procedures described above. Details on the wood gnawing test can be found in Supplementary Section.

Statistical Analyses

All data were analyzed using repeated-measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) with viral vector and sex (when appropriate) as between-subjects variables and vehicle vs CNO treatment as a within-subjects factor. For clarity, only significant effects (main or interactions) are described in detail below. When main effects or interaction effects were significant, post-hoc comparisons, when necessary, were performed using Bonferroni-corrected t-tests. All data are presented as mean±SEM, and significance was accepted at p<0.05 (two-tailed).

Results

Mouse Cohort 1: Binge-Like Ethanol Drinking, Food and Water Consumption, and Open-Field Locomotor Activity

Results from the binge-like ethanol consumption study (a–c) and examples of AAV8-hSyn-DIO-mcherry (d), AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mcherry (e), and AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mcherry (f) virus expression in the LH are presented in Figure 1. A repeated-measures (CNO vs vehicle × virus condition × sex) ANOVA performed on the 2-h binge-like ethanol consumption data showed a significant main effect of virus condition (F(2, 20)=7.33, p=0.004) and a significant CNO vs vehicle × virus condition interaction effect (F(2, 20)=35.93, p<0.001). Interestingly, post-hoc tests comparing CNO vs vehicle treatment showed that CNO treatment significantly increased 2 h ethanol consumption in mice expressing the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mcherry virus in the LH, while CNO treatment significantly blunted 2 h binge-like ethanol drinking in mice expressing the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mcherry virus in the LH. There were no differences in binge-like ethanol drinking between vehicle and CNO treatment in mice expressing the control AAV8-hSyn-DIO-mcherry virus. A similar repeated-measures ANOVA run on the 4-h binge-like ethanol consumption data revealed a significant CNO vs vehicle × virus condition interaction effect (F(2, 20)=4.58, p=0.023). Despite the significant interaction effect, post-hoc tests failed to reveal significant differences between the CNO- and vehicle-treated groups at any of the virus conditions. Finally, a repeated-measures ANOVA run on the BEC data revealed a significant CNO vs vehicle main effect (F(1, 20)=4.98, p=0.037), but post-hoc tests failed to show a significant difference between the CNO- and vehicle-treated groups.

Figure 1
figure 1

Results from the binge-like ethanol consumption study. Two- (a) and 4 h (b) binge-like ethanol (20%, v/v) consumption and blood ethanol concentrations (BECs; c) and examples of AAV8-hSyn-DIO-mcherry (Control; d), AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mcherry (hM3D(Gq); e), and AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mcherry (hM4D(Gi); f) virus expression in the LH. Relative to vehicle treatment, CNO significantly increased 2 h ethanol consumption, or reduced 2 h ethanol consumption, in the hM3D(Gq)- or hM4D(Gi)-treated groups, respectively. All data are presented as mean±SEM. *Signifies p<0.05 relative to vehicle within each DREADD virus condition as determined by Bonferroni corrected t-tests (two-tailed). LH, lateral hypothalamus; ZI, zona incerta.

PowerPoint slide

Data showing 2 (a) and 4 h (b) food intake, 2 h water consumption (c), and 2 h open-field locomotor activity (d) are depicted in Figure 2. A repeated-measures (CNO vs vehicle × virus condition × sex) ANOVA run on 2-h food intake data revealed a significant main effect of virus condition (F(2, 21)=13.52, p<0.001) and a significant CNO vs vehicle × virus condition interaction effect (F(2, 21)=20.15, p<0.001). Post-hoc tests indicated that relative to vehicle, CNO significantly increased food intake, or reduced food intake in mice expressing the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mcherry or AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mcherry virus in the LH, respectively. On the other hand, there was no effect of CNO vs vehicle treatment in mice expressing the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-mcherry in the LH. A similar repeated-measures ANOVA run on 4-h food intake data revealed a main effect of virus condition (F(2, 21)=6.36, p=0.007) and a significant CNO vs vehicle × virus condition interaction effect (F(2, 21)=4.57, p=0.023). Post-hoc tests failed to reveal significant differences between the CNO- and vehicle-treated groups at any of the virus conditions. A repeated-measures ANOVA run on 2-h water consumption data revealed main effects of CON vs vehicle condition (F(1, 21)=79.15, p<0.001) and virus condition (F(2, 21)=71.43, p<0.001), and a significant interaction effect between CNO vs vehicle × virus condition variables (F(2, 21)=96.78, p<0.001). Post-hoc tests run at each virus condition revealed that relative to vehicle treatment CNO significantly increased water drinking in mice expressing the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mcherry virus but did not alter water drinking at any of the other virus conditions. Finally, a repeated-measures ANOVA run on 2-h open-field locomotor activity data revealed significant main effects of CNO vs vehicle treatment condition (F(1, 21)=9.21, p=0.006) and virus condition (F(2, 21)=4.49, p=0.024), but the interaction between variables was not significant.

Figure 2
figure 2

Results reflecting 2 (a) and 4 h (b) food intake, 2 h water consumption (c), and 2 h open-field locomotor activity assessment (d). Relative to vehicle treatment, CNO significantly increased 2 h food intake, or reduced 2 h ethanol consumption, in groups expressing hM3D(Gq) or hM4D(Gi) viruses in the LH, respectively. Also relative to vehicle treatment CNO significantly increased water drinking in the hM3D(Gq) virus condition. All data are presented as mean±SEM. *Signifies p<0.05 relative to vehicle within each DREADD virus condition as determined by Bonferroni-corrected t-tests (two-tailed).

PowerPoint slide

Mouse Cohort 2: Binge-Like Saccharin and Sucrose Consumption

Two hour saccharin (a) and sucrose (b) consumption are presented in Figure 3. A repeated-measures (CNO vs vehicle × virus condition × sex) ANOVA performed on saccharin intake data revealed a significant main effect of virus condition (F(1, 10)=6.44, p=0.03) and a significant CNO vs vehicle × virus condition interaction effect (F(1, 10)=10.05, p=0.01). Post-hoc tests indicated that relative to vehicle treatment, treatment with CNO significantly increased saccharin intake or reduced saccharin intake in mice expressing the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mcherry or AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mcherry virus in the LH, respectively. A repeated-measures ANOVA run on sucrose intake data revealed significant main effects of CNO vs vehicle (F(1, 10)=20.89, p=0.001) and virus condition (F(1, 10)=31.49, p<0.001) effects and a significant CNO vs vehicle × virus condition interaction effect (F(1, 10)=118.41, p<0.001). Post-hoc tests indicated that relative to vehicle treatment, treatment with CNO significantly increased sucrose intake or reduced sucrose intake in mice expressing the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mcherry or AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mcherry virus in the LH, respectively.

Figure 3
figure 3

Results reflecting 2 h 0.15% (w/v) saccharin (a), 3% (w/v) sucrose (b) consumption testing, and the 2-h wood gnawing test (c). Relative to vehicle treatment, CNO significantly increased 2 h saccharin and sucrose intake, or reduced 2 h saccharin and sucrose intake, in groups expressing hM3D(Gq) or hM4D(Gi) viruses in the LH, respectively. Further, CNO significantly increased wood gnawing (reflected as a reduction of wood weight) specifically in the group expressing the hM3D(Gq) virus. All data are presented as mean±SEM. *Signifies p<0.05 relative to vehicle within each DREADD virus condition as determined by Bonferroni-corrected t-tests (two-tailed).

PowerPoint slide

Mouse Cohort 3: Binge-Like Ethanol Drinking and Food Intake Following GABAergic Neuronal Manipulations in the ZI

Two hour ethanol consumption (a) and food intake (b), and examples of virus expression (c–e) in mice with viral vectors aimed specifically at the ZI are presented in Figure 4. Repeated-measures (CNO vs vehicle × virus condition × sex) ANOVAs performed on ethanol consumption data revealed a significant CNO vs vehicle × sex interaction effect (F(1, 14)=6.58, p=0.022), which reflected greater consumption of ethanol following CNO (1.75±0.23 g/kg/2 h) relative to vehicle (0.92±0.25 g/kg/2 h) in female mice but slightly lower consumption of ethanol following CNO (1.03±0.21 g/kg/2 h) relative to vehicle (1.17±0.23 g/kg/2 h) in male mice. Post-hoc test comparing ethanol consumption between vehicle treatment and CNO treatment failed to show significant effects for male mice, though CNO-treated female mice did drink more ethanol than female vehicle-treated mice. This outcome is difficult to explain as CNO is considered to be inert (Urban and Roth, 2015). A repeated-measures ANOVA performed on 2-h food intake data revealed a significant main effect of sex (F(1, 14)=8.35, p=0.012), reflecting greater consumption of food by female mice (25±2.29 g/kg/2 h) relative to male mice (16.62±2.09 g/kg/2 h). No other effects were statistically significant.

Figure 4
figure 4

Results from the zona incerta (ZI) study. Two-hour 20% (v/v) ethanol consumption (a) and food intake (b), and examples of AAV8-hSyn-DIO-mcherry (Control; c), AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mcherry (hM3D(Gq); d), and AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mcherry (hM4D(Gi); e) virus expression in the ZI. When viruses were transduced specifically in the ZI, CNO treatment did not influence ethanol consumption or food intake in any of the virus conditions. All data are presented as mean±SEM.

PowerPoint slide

Mouse Cohort 4: Wood Gnawing Behavior

Results from the gnawing behavior experiment are presented in Figure 3c. A repeated-measures (CNO vs vehicle × virus condition) ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of both drug and virus (F(1, 13)=11.064, p=0.005; F(1, 13)=5.472, p=0.036, respectively) as well as a significant drug × virus interaction (F(1, 13)=8.707, p=0.011). Post-hoc analyses revealed the group of mice expressing the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mCherry virus and injected CNO were significantly different from all other groups, reflecting a significant reduction of wood weight over the 2-h test that was not evident in the other groups.

Discussion

The present report provides novel evidence that GABAergic neurons of the LH modulate consummatory behaviors directed at any available stimulus, including those entailing calories (food, sucrose, and ethanol), those lacking calories (saccharin, water), and those lacking biological relevance (wood). The most critical observation was that activation of GABAergic neurons in the LH induced robust consummatory behaviors, while silencing of GABAergic neurons in the LH robustly blunted consummatory behaviors. Alterations of consummatory behaviors cannot easily be attributed to secondary changes stemming from impairment of motor function, as activation or inhibition of GABAergic neurons in the LH did not significantly alter locomotor activity over a 2-h test, yet this same manipulation promoted robust changes in the intake of rewarding substances. Unexpectedly, there was a non-significant trend for reduced locomotor activity following activation of GABAergic neurons in the LH, but as this same manipulation increased consumption, blunted activity is unlikely to be a confounding factor when interpreting consummatory behaviors.

Previous pharmacological data have established that GABA receptor signaling in the LH modulates consummatory behaviors (Kelly et al, 1977; Turenius et al, 2009a). Consistent with recent observations (Jennings et al, 2015), the present data give new insight that was not possible with a strictly pharmacological approach by providing direct evidence for a role of local GABAergic neurons in the LH in modulating consummatory behaviors. Importantly, our work provides the first evidence that LH GABAergic neurons modulate consumption of any available stimulus, regardless of caloric content or biological relevance. Interestingly, LH infusion of GABAA receptor agonists blunt, and antagonists increase, food intake (Turenius et al, 2009a), essentially opposite to the pattern of results presented here with regards to activation and silencing of local GABAergic transmission. These results suggest the possibility that GABAA receptors are expressed on local GABAergic neurons in the LH or on presynaptic neurons that provide excitatory input to GABAergic neurons in the LH and, viewed this way, allow pharmacological and chemogenetic data to be explained with consistency.

It is of interest to consider the possible neurocircuitry by which LH GABAergic neurons modulate consummatory behaviors. MCH/GABAergic neurons in the LH project to the VTA and NAc (Bittencourt et al, 1992; Dallvechia-Adams et al, 2002; Saito et al, 2001; Sears et al, 2010) and modulate consumption of natural and drug rewards (Morganstern et al, 2010; Parker and Bloom, 2012). However, as we used the same vGat-ires-cre mice used by Jennings et al (2015) and the same DREADD constructs, MCH/GABAergic neurons are unlikely to be involved as cre is not present in MCH neurons of the current vGat-ires-cre mice (Jennings et al, 2015). Interestingly, a portion of NAc-projecting GABAergic neurons from the LH likely modulate NAc activity independent of neuropeptide signaling (Urstadt and Stanley, 2015). If GABAergic signaling in these pathways is necessary for the presently observed phenotypes, then one would predict that local injection of GABAA receptor agonist into the NAc would promote consumption (consistent with activation of projecting GABAergic neurons from the LH), while infusion of GABAA receptor antagonist into the NAc would blunt reward consumption (consistent with silencing of projecting GABAergic neurons from the LH). In fact, NAc activation of GABAA receptor signaling increases, while inhibition of GABAA receptor signaling decreases, reward (food or ethanol) consumption (Klitenick and Wirtshafter, 1988; Melon and Boehm, 2011; Nowak et al, 1998; Urstadt et al, 2013). Additional research is necessary to confirm the potential role of LH to NAc GABAergic pathways in the modulation of reward consumption. Interestingly, activation of a VTA-projecting GABAergic pathway was recently shown to increase food intake and aberrant gnawing behaviors (Nieh et al, 2015), which is also a possible pathway underlying the present observations.

It is important to consider the basic properties of the DREADDs used in the present report. Though we did not quantify the level of viral vector expression, there were apparent differences in the expression levels between the different virus groups (eg, Figure 1d–f). In particular, the control virus appears to exhibit more intense florescent activity relative to the DREADD viral vectors, though the pattern or expression appears to be similar between viral vectors. However, given that behavior mapped onto the different viral vector groups as predicted (ie, increased consumption following activation of the Gq DREADD, reduced consumption following activation of the Gi DREADD, and no changes in consumption in the control virus group), any existing differences in viral vector expression are unlikely to account for differences in behavior between the different virus groups. In all studies that included the use of the control AAV8-hSyn-DIO-mCherry, there were no differences between groups given i.p. injection of vehicle or the 3 mg/kg dose of CNO, verifying that CNO injection alone was not responsible for the observed phenotypes in animals expressing the activating AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mCherry or silencing AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry viruses. Additionally, in no cases were there significant differences between the different viral vector groups under baseline (vehicle injection) conditions, verifying that the DREADD viral vectors alone, when compared with the control virus, did not promote behavioral effects in the absence of CNO. Controlling the spread of virus transduction is difficult, and in fact in our LH study we observed strong virus transduction in GABAergic neurons in both the LH and ZI. Similar to the LH, much of the ZI is comprised of a dense population of GABAergic neurons (Watson et al, 2014). Although little is known about the functional role of the ZI, the subthalamus region, which includes the ZI, has been implicated in modulating consumption of natural and drug rewards (Lardeux et al, 2009). Thus we could not initially rule out the possibility that observed changes in reward consumption were modulated by GABAergic transmission in the ZI rather than, or in addition to, GABAergic transmission in the LH. To address this issue, we adjusted our infusion coordinates to a more dorsal position, thus allowing us to selectively target our viral vectors to the ZI. In this experiment, i.p. injection of CNO failed to alter food intake or ethanol consumption in a way that depended on viral vector condition. This allowed us to rule out a role for the ZI and to conclude with confidence that GABAergic neurons, specifically in the LH, modulate consummatory behaviors. Finally, functional DREADD activity has been verified in numerous previous citations (Ferguson et al, 2013; Hasegawa et al, 2014; Krashes et al, 2011; Sasaki et al, 2011), and we showed that our Vgat-ires-cre mice expressing the AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mCherry DREADD in the LH exhibited increased c-Fos expression and electrophysiological activity following CNO application (see Supplementary Materials, Supplementary Figures S1 and S2).

In summary, here we provide the first direct evidence that the LH GABAergic neurons under investigation modulate the consumption of any available stimulus, include those entailing calories (food, sucrose, and ethanol), those lacking calories (saccharin and water), and those lacking biological relevance (wood). These data indicate that, rather than specifically regulating behaviors to maintain homeostatic caloric balance, the LH GABAergic neurons under investigation modulate general consummatory behaviors, thus other upstream or parallel circuits must modulate specific motivationally driven behaviors (eg, hunger or thirst). Additional research will be necessary to identify the specific GABAergic neurocircuits that are involved.

Funding and Disclosure

This research was funded by NIH grant AA013573, AA015148, and AA022048 to TET, AA022044 to MN, AA019454, AA020911, AA011605, and MH105892 to TLK, AA022549 to EGL-G, and AA023559 to KEP. All the authors declare no conflict of interest.