Introduction

Glutamate receptors mediate most excitatory synaptic transmission in the mammalian central nervous system, and the metabotropic glutamate receptor (mGluR) family consists of eight G-protein-coupled receptors that modulate the activity of neural circuits (Hollmann and Heinemann, 1994; Nakanishi and Masu, 1994; Nicoletti et al, 2011). mGluRs are divided into three groups on the basis of signal transduction mechanisms and pharmacological properties. mGluR7 belongs to group III mGluRs, which localize presynaptically, close to neurotransmitter release sites (Okamoto et al, 1994). mGluR7 shows target cell-specific distribution that is differentially concentrated at particular axon terminals, depending on the nature of the postsynaptic neurons (Shigemoto et al, 1996). In many cases, single dendritic or somatic profiles are densely decorated by axon terminals with mGluR7 immunoreactivity (Shigemoto et al, 1996; Kinoshita et al, 1998). Interestingly, mGluR7 internalization is critical for metaplasticity, and thus mGluR7 functions as a switch between long-term depression and long-term potentiation (Pelkey et al, 2005, 2008).

Although differential localization and electrophysiological properties suggest the importance of mGluR7 in neural circuits, the physiological relevance of this receptor remains to be fully elucidated. Previously, we reported that mGluR7 knockout (KO) mice showed a deficit in fear response and conditioned taste aversion, which are suggested to be regulated by the amygdala (Masugi et al, 1999). Based on our reports, many papers have discussed the link between mGluR7 and anxiety (Cryan et al, 2003; Mitsukawa et al, 2006; Fendt et al, 2008).

To assess a wider role for mGluR7 in emotional behavior, we focused on aggression. Aggression is fundamental for survival, and pheromonal signals have important roles in evoking intermale aggression (Leypold et al, 2002; Stowers et al, 2002; Nelson and Trainor, 2007). mGluR7 is widely distributed throughout the central nervous system, including vomeronasal circuits involving the accessory olfactory bulb (AOB), the medial amygdala (MeA), and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST). In the BNST, some dendrites are decorated with mGluR7-expressing axon terminals (Kinoshita et al, 1998). The BNST is an extremely complex mass of gray matter (Ju and Swanson, 1989) that is composed of several subnuclei of varying reported numbers and locations (Alheid et al, 1995; Ju and Swanson, 1989; Moga et al, 1989; Franklin and Paxinos, 2007) and divided into anterior (BNSTa) and posterior (BNSTp) divisions. The BNSTa has started to receive increased attention since it was discovered that it regulates anxiety (Kim et al, 2013) and motivational states (Jennings et al, 2013a, b), but the cytoarchitecture and function of the BNSTp are still poorly understood. In the present study, we focused on the role of mGluR7 in the BNSTp regulating social behaviors, as exemplified by intermale aggression.

Materials and Methods

See also Supplementary Information online.

Animals

Sexually inexperienced, gonadally intact male mice (12–24 weeks old) were used. mGluR7 KO and wild-type littermates were obtained from heterozygous mating couples, which were produced by back-crossing the mGluR7 KO line lacking the first coding exon and next intron of the mGluR7 gene (Masugi et al, 1999; Sansig et al, 2001; Cryan et al, 2003) into the C57BL/6N background for at least 11 generations. For microinjection experiments, C57BL/6N male mice were originally purchased from a commercial breeder (SHIMIZU Laboratory Supplies, Kyoto, Japan). All animal protocols were compliant with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the Committee for Animal Research, Kyoto Prefectural University of Medicine, and Kyoto University.

Aggressive Behavior and Social Behavior (Resident–Intruder Test)

Mice (n=10 for each genotypes) were tested for aggressive behaviors six times (once a week) in the resident–intruder paradigm (Ogawa et al, 1998). Each male was tested in his home cage (as a resident) against a group-housed (3–5 mice per cage) olfactory bulbectomized male ICR intruder mouse (Denenberg et al, 1973; Ogawa et al, 1996) for 15 min. Cumulative duration of all aggressive bouts, and latency to the first aggressive act were all recorded. We defined aggressive behaviors into three distinct categories in the following way:

1. Intense aggression: Biting, boxing, wrestling, tumbling.

2. Mild aggression: Chasing, attempted mount, mount.

3. Sexual behavior: Mount with vigorously palpating the flanks of the intruder, just like trying to induce lordosis of female mice.

We also recorded several social investigating behaviors simultaneously:

4. Anogenital sniffing: Nose-to-anogenital area investigation.

5. Sniff other areas: Nose-to-whole body investigation other than anogenital area.

6. Grooming the intruder.

Urine Preference Test (Olfaction Test)

Singly housed and sexually inexperienced wild-type (n=21) and mGluR7 KO males (n=21) not previously tested for aggression were used to determine whether they exhibited a preference for male or hormone-primed female urine or saline (Mandiyan et al, 2005). A fresh absorbent cotton pad, handled only with gloves, was wetted with 40 μl of male urine, female urine, or saline. Two cotton pads wetted with different odorant sources were presented simultaneously to the mice at random locations in the home cage for 10 min. The duration of sniffing was recorded.

Urine Exposure for c-Fos Induction

Individually housed and sexually inexperienced male mGluR7 KO mice and their littermates not previously tested for aggression were used for c-Fos induction. Mice were trained daily, for 2 weeks, in the manipulation used for urine exposure. During the dark phase of the light/dark cycle, animals were taken out of their home cage and received 10 μl × 3 of saline onto their nose and then placed back into their cage. On the day of urine exposure, they were divided into three groups depending on the odor stimulus that they were going to be exposed to. Group 1 (n=6 for each genotypes) was exposed to intact male urine, group 2 (n=5 for each genotypes) to hormone-primed female urine, and group 3 (n=6 for each genotypes) to saline as control.

Ninety minutes later, mice were anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital and subjected to transcardiac perfusion with formaldehyde (4%; freshly depolymerized from paraformaldehyde) in 0.1 M sodium phosphate buffer. After perfusion, the brains were removed, and postfixed in the same fixative for 12 h. Brains were then cryoprotected in 30% sucrose/0.1 M sodium phosphate buffer and quickly frozen with liquid carbon dioxide and cut into 40-μm serial coronal sections using a cryostat (CM3050 S; Leica Microsystems, Wetzlar, Germany).

Pharmacological Treatment and Site-Directed Injections

For the BNST and MeA, 0.25 μl (5 μg/μl) of MMPIP (MMPIP250) dissolved in a solution of dimethyl sulfoxide (DMSO) or DMSO (DMSO250) as a control was infused. For lateral ventricle injection, 0.25 μl or 0.75 μl of MMPIP (MMPIP750) or 0.75 μl of DMSO (DMSO750) was infused. Testing began 60 min after infusion. Within each group, a within-subjects repeated-measures design was used.

Statistics

Behavioral data were analyzed by a two-way ANOVA for repeated measurements or unpaired t-test for independent samples or paired t-test for matched samples. Some behavioral data (in which variances were not homogeneous between genotype groups) were analyzed by nonparametric tests (Mann–Whitney U-test for independent samples or Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test for matched samples).

Results

Impaired Intermale Aggression in mGluR7 KO Mice

Using the resident–intruder paradigm, we performed six once-a week tests of the aggressive behavior of mGluR7 KO male resident mice toward an olfactory bulbectomized male intruder mouse. Because olfaction is important for aggression, olfactory bulbectomized intruders rarely show aggression. However, because their gonads are intact, they can elicit aggression in resident male mice (Ogawa et al, 1998). All tested mice were singly housed to induce territorial aggression through our study. Although this is one of the standard protocols, we should be aware that social isolation affects behavioral, physiological, and biochemical parameters (Miczek et al, 2001). We found that the mean duration of intense aggressive behavior (biting, boxing, wrestling, and tumbling; main effect of genotype, F(1,18)=28.3, P<0.001; main effect of trial, F(5,18)=1.8, P=0.12; genotype × trial interaction, F(5,18)=1.6, P=0.17, Figure 1a) and the mean latency to the first intense aggressive act (main effect of genotype, F(1,18)=54.4, P<0.001; main effect of trial, F(5,18)=4.6, P<0.001; genotype × trial interaction, F(5,18)=3.0, P=0.016; Figure 1b) of the mGluR7 KO mice were different from those of their wild-type littermates, as assessed by repeated-measures ANOVA. The mean duration and latency were different in all six trials, as assessed by unpaired t-test or Mann–Whitney U-test (P<0.01, Figure 1). In the wild-type controls, the intense aggression gradually increased with fighting experience, as previously reported (Lagerspetz and Lagerspetz, 1971), whereas mGluR7 KO mice showed almost no intense aggression, even in the sixth trial. These results show that mGluR7 KO mice have a severe deficit in aggression.

Figure 1
figure 1

Impaired aggression in male mGluR7 KO mice toward male intruders. Cumulative duration (a) and latency (b) of aggressive behavior, as measured by the resident–intruder paradigm. Male wild-type mice showed intense aggressions, which increased with experience, whereas male mGluR7 KO mice rarely showed intense aggressive behavior throughout the test. Male mGluR7 KO mice more frequently showed sexual behavior toward male intruders than their wild-type littermates. n=10. *P<0.05 vs wild-types, §P<0.01 vs wild-types in each individual trial. Error bars indicate SEM.

PowerPoint slide

Chasing, attempted mount, and mount are ways of showing dominance between males but are also observed in male-to-female sexual behavior. We defined these behaviors as mild aggression. Both groups showed almost equivalent levels of the mean duration of mild aggression (main effect of genotype, F(1,18)=0.1, P=0.73; main effect of trial, F(5,18)=0.8, P=0.54; genotype × trial interaction, F(5,18)=2.4, P=0.046; Figure 1a) and the mean latency to the first mild aggressive behavior (main effect of genotype, F(1,18)=1.5, P=0.24; main effect of trial, F(5,18)=0.4, P=0.82; genotype × trial interaction, F(5,18)=1.3, P=0.27; Figure 1b). Only in the first trial, the mean duration and latency of mild aggression of mGluR7 KO mice were different from those of wild-type mice (z=−2.2, P=0.030, Mann–Whitney U-test; t(18)=2.9, P=0.0095, unpaired t-test; respectively; Figure 1). These results suggest that mGluR7 KO mice have normal level of motivation to interact with the intruder and that impaired intense aggression in mGluR7 KO mice is not due to reduced motivation of social interaction.

Interestingly, mGluR7 KO mice showed sexual behaviors (mount and vigorously palpating the flanks of the intruder) toward male intruders, behaviors that are normally used to induce lordosis in female mice. Sexual behaviors toward male intruders was also seen in 2 out of 10 wild-type mice at the beginning of the trials, but no wild-type mouse showed sexual behavior in the fourth and sixth trials. Repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that there was statistical significance between the two groups in the mean latency to the first sexual behavior (main effect of genotype, F(1,18)=9.9, P=0.006; main effect of trial, F(5,18)=0.4, P=0.87; genotype × trial interaction, F(5,18)=1.7, P=0.15; Figure 1b), but no significance in the mean duration of first sexual behavior (main effect of genotype, F(1,18)=3.9, P=0.064; main effect of trial, F(5,18)=0.2, P=0.96; genotype × trial interaction, F(5,18)=0.7, P=0.62; Figure 1a). However, there were considerable differences in the mean duration of sexual behavior in the fourth and sixth trials, (z=−1.5, P=0.031; z=−1.9, P=0.013; respectively; Mann–Whitney U-test; Figure 1a) and in the mean latency to the first sexual behavior in the fourth to sixth trials (z=−1.5, P=0.031, Mann–Whitney U-test; t(18)=−2.9, P=0.0098, unpaired t-test; z=−1.9, P=0.013, Mann–Whitney U-test; respectively; Figure 1b).

Altered Social Investigation in mGluR7 KO Mice

Social recognition in mice is primarily based on olfaction. Urinary pheromones and their binding proteins have important roles in the individual recognition mechanism (Hurst et al, 2001). During social encounters, a male mouse investigates a novel mouse by sniffing the anogenital region, as well as the rest of the body, including nose-to-nose sniffing. Here we further analyzed the social investigating behavior. Wild-type mice reacted quickly toward intruder mice by investigating the anogenital region, followed by a bite or fighting. Interestingly, mGluR7 KO mice showed a marked reduction in the duration of anogenital sniffing (main effect of genotype, F(1,18)=17.2, P<0.001; main effect of trial, F(5,18)=6.8, P<0.001; genotype × trial interaction, F(5,18)=4.6, P<0.001; Figure 2a) and took a longer time to initiate the first anogenital sniffing (main effect of genotype, F(1,18)=33.8, P<0.001; main effect of trial, F(5,18)=1.2, P=0.31; genotype × trial interaction, F(5,18)=1.2, P=0.30; Figure 2b), as assessed by repeated-measures ANOVA.

Figure 2
figure 2

Altered social investigating behavior in male mGluR7 KO mice. Cumulative duration (a) and latency (b) of social behavior as measured by the resident–intruder paradigm. Duration of anogenital sniffing of intruder mice was reduced in mGluR7 KO mice. Note that mGluR7 KO mice frequently showed grooming behavior that was rarely seen in wild-type mice. n=10, *P<0.05 vs wild types, §P<0.01 vs wild types on each individual trial. Error bars indicate SEM.

PowerPoint slide

In contrast, a slight increase was observed in the time spent sniffing the other parts of the body. mGluR7 KO mice sniffed the intruder more than their wild-type littermates (main effect of genotype, F(1,18)=14.2, P=0.0014; main effect of trial, F(5,18)=5.3, P<0.001; genotype × trial interaction, F(5,18)=1.3, P=0.29; Figure 2a) and quicker than wild types (main effect of genotype, F(1,18)=16.3, P<0.001; main effect of trial, F(5,18)=1.2, P=0.30; genotype × trial interaction, F(5,18)=1.3, P=0.29; Figure 2b), as assessed by repeated-measures ANOVA. These results suggest that reduced duration of anogenital sniffing in mGluR7 KO mice is not due to a reduced interest in investigating the intruder.

mGluR7 KO mice showed grooming behaviors toward the intruder. The mean duration of intruder grooming (main effect of genotype, F(1,18)=18.7, P<0.001; main effect of trial, F(5,18)=1.0, P=0.41; genotype × trial interaction, F(5,18)=1.2, P=0.34; Figure 2a) and the mean latency to the first grooming (main effect of genotype, F(1,18)=20.4, P<0.001; main effect of trial, F(5,18)=0.8, P=0.50; genotype × trial interaction, F(5,18)=1.9, P=0.11; Figure 2b) were different between wild-type and mGluR7 KO mice.

Altered Urine Preference in mGluR7 KO Mice

Because we found reduced duration of anogenital sniffing in mGluR7 KO mice and because urine pheromones are important for eliciting sexual and aggressive behaviors (Leypold et al, 2002; Stowers et al, 2002; Chamero et al, 2007), we next examined urine preference. Mice were exposed to two absorbent cotton pads wetted with male or hormone-primed female urine for 10 min. As expected, wild-type males showed a preference for female urine (t(7)=7.4, P<0.0001, paired t-test; Figure 3a). mGluR7 KO mice also had a preference for female urine (t(7)=2.8, P=0.025, paired t-test), but the cumulative duration that they spent sniffing the female urine was significantly shorter than that of wild-type mice (t(14)=−3.7, P=0.0026, unpaired t-test).

Figure 3
figure 3

Altered urine preference in male mGluR7 KO mice. (a) Wild-type mice sniffed female urine for a longer duration than their mGluR7 KO littermates. (b) Wild-type mice spent significantly longer duration of sniffing male urine than sniffing saline; no such preference was observed in mGluR7 KO mice. n=8 for (a), n=13 for (b). *P<0.05, **P<0.01. Error bars indicate SEM. See also Supplementary Figure S1.

PowerPoint slide

We next presented male urine and saline simultaneously. Wild-type mice spent significantly more time investigating the male urine than the saline (t(12)=−4.5, P=0.008, paired t-test; Figure 3b). In contrast, mGluR7 KO mice failed to sniff either cotton pad preferentially (t(12)=1.0, P=0.32, paired t-test). Furthermore, the duration that wild-type mice sniffed the male urine was longer than that of mGluR7 KO mice sniffed the male urine or saline (z=−3.7, P=0.0002; z=−2.1, P=0.032; respectively; Mann-Whitney U-test). Using a habituation–dishabituation test, we found that mGluR7 KO mice can discriminate urine odors and that they can respond to novel stimulus (Supplementary Figure S1). Thus these results show that mGluR7 KO mice have altered urine preference.

Reduced c-Fos Induction after Urine Exposure in the BNSTp of mGluR7 KO Mice

The response to urine pheromones, which is important for social recognition and initiation of aggressive behaviors, begins with pheromone detection in the vomeronasal organ (Chamero et al, 2007), and the information is then conveyed to the AOB. The AOB directly projects to the MeA, which itself projects to the BNST; the BNST also receives direct projections from the AOB (Yoon et al, 2005). Figure 4a schematically illustrates the flow of pheromonal information in the rodent. Altered urine preference and aggressive behavior of mGluR7 KO mice may be a consequence of mGluR7 dysfunction in this circuitry. To identify the candidate sites for the processing of olfactory cues, we compared the neural activation patterns in mGluR7 KO and wild-type mice after urine exposure using the protein product of the immediate early gene c-fos.

Figure 4
figure 4

Reduced c-Fos activation in the BNSTp of mGluR7 KO mice. (a) Schematic illustration of the flow of pheromonal information in the rodent brain. (b–f) Quantification of c-Fos-expressing cells in each region. In both genotypes, exposure to urine increased c-Fos-ir in the mitral cell layer (Mit) and granule cell layer (Gr) of the accessory olfactory bulb (AOB), the posterodorsal (MePD) and posteroventral medial amygdala (MePV), and posterior division of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNSTp), and the response to female urine was greater than that to male urine. (d) Representative microscopic images of c-Fos-ir in the BNSTp of male wild-type mice after male urine exposure. The boxes approximate the regions analyzed. (e, f) Representative microscopic images and quantification of c-Fos-ir in the BNSTp of male wild-type and mGluR7 KO mice after saline, male urine, and female urine exposure. The scale bars indicate 100 μm. Note that a remarkable reduction in c-Fos-ir after urine exposure was seen in the BNSTp of mGluR7 KO mice. *P<0.05, **P<0.01. Error bars indicate SEM. See also Supplementary Figure S2.

PowerPoint slide

Because there were differences in the duration of olfactory investigation, we directly applied urine or saline to the nose of mice. After 90 min of urine exposure, mice were killed and immunohistochemistry for c-Fos was performed. We compared various brain regions between wild-type and mGluR7 KO mice exposed to male urine, hormone-primed female urine, or saline.

First, we examined the AOB, the first relay station of the pheromonal information. The analysis of the c-Fos-immunoreactivity (ir) of various brain regions is shown in Supplementary Figure S2. The c-Fos responses for male and female urine were very similar between wild-type and mGluR7 KO mice in both the mitral cell layer (t(10)=0.1, P=0.92; t(8)=0.6, P=0.58; respectively; unpaired t-test; Figure 4b, left panel, and Supplementary Figure S2) and the granule cell layer (t(10)=0.8, P=0.46; t(8)=−1.2, P=0.28; respectively; unpaired t-test; Figure 4b, right panel, and Supplementary Figure S2).

Next we examined c-Fos expression in secondary vomeronasal centers, namely, the posterior region of the MeA. Again, c-Fos responses for male and female urine were similar between wild-type and mGluR7 KO mice in both the posterodorsal MeA (MePD; t(10)=0.7, P=0.52; t(8)=−0.3, P=0.81; respectively; unpaired t-test; Figure 4c, left panel, and Supplementary Figure S2) and posteroventral MeA (MePV; t(10)=1.2, P=0.28; t(8)=−0.2, P=0.87; respectively; unpaired t-test; Figure 4c, right panel, and Supplementary Figure S2). Importantly, these results suggest that pheromonal information in mGluR7 KO mice is normal until it reaches the MeA.

Finally, we examined the BNSTp, which is connected to the MeA and AOB (Figure 4a). The c-Fos-ir in the BNSTp induced by exposure to both male and female urine in mGluR7 KO mice was reduced compared with wild types (t(10)=−3.1, P=0.011; t(8)=−10.1, P=0.0001; respectively; unpaired t-test; Figure 4d–f). These results indicate that mGluR7 KO mice have a deficit in processing pheromonal information within the BNSTp.

Reproduction of the mGluR7 KO Phenotype by Intra-BNSTp Injection of the mGluR7-Selective Antagonist MMPIP

The reduced c-Fos induction in the BNSTp of mGluR7 KO mice strongly suggested that mGluR7-mediated olfactory processing in this brain region is essential for intermale aggression. To complete the links between impaired aggression and reduced c-Fos-ir after urine exposure and to specify the site of action of mGluR7, we performed the resident–intruder test after intracerebral microinjection of the mGluR7-selective antagonist MMPIP. The BNSTp has strong connections with the MePD and MePV (Dong and Swanson, 2004; Bienkowski et al, 2013). The area of reduced c-Fos-ir and the anatomical relationships strongly suggest that this area is the prime candidate for the action in our study; we therefore injected MMPIP into the BNSTp. Because the surgery itself increased the latency to the first aggressive behavior of most mice and to avoid repeated microinjections, we trained the mice until the latency to the first incident of intense aggression became <180 s before testing the effect of MMPIP in our aggressive behavior tests. The mice used in the microinjection study are summarized in Supplementary Table S1.

Intra-BNSTp microinjection of MMPIP significantly reduced the durations of intense aggressive behavior (t(9)=4.1, P=0.0015, paired t-test; Figure 5a, left panel) and anogenital sniffing (t(9)=4.6, P=0.0045, paired t-test; Figure 5b, left panel). Moreover, 2 out of 10 MMPIP-injected mice showed sexual behavior toward male intruders, and 3 showed grooming behavior, whereas vehicle-injected mice did not show such behaviors (statistical assessment not possible due to low animal numbers). These results indicate that MMPIP infusion into the BNSTp induced very similar effects to mGluR7 deficiency in the resident–intruder test. Intra-MeA injection of MMPIP had no effect on any of the parameters measured (Figure 5a and b, middle panels).

Figure 5
figure 5

Effects of MMPIP injection on aggressive behavior and social investigation in wild-type male mice. Cumulative duration of aggressive behavior (a) and social behavior (b), as measured by the resident–intruder paradigm. Intra-posterior division of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNSTp) injections of MMPIP reduced aggression, whereas intra-medial amygdaloid (MeA) injections had no effect. Intra-lateral ventricle injections needed a higher dose of MMPIP to induce behavioral alterations, indicating that the effect of intra-BNSTp injection was not due to diffusion into the lateral ventricle. n=10 for the BNSTp, n=7 for the MeA, n=5 for the lateral ventricle; *P<0.05, **P<0.01. Error bars indicate SEM. See also Supplementary Table S1.

PowerPoint slide

Although the injections were clearly within the BNSTp or MeA, we cannot exclude the possibility that diffusion into adjacent regions impacted the behavior. To rule out such a possibility, we injected MMPIP into the lateral ventricle, because the BNSTp is located very close to the lateral ventricle and MMPIP might diffuse into the ventricle. Intra-lateral ventricle microinjection of the same amount of MMPIP that reduced the aggression in the BNSTp had no effect on any of the parameters measured. Then we administered triple the amount of MMPIP into the lateral ventricle; although there was no statistical significance in the duration of intense aggression between MMPIP750 and the vehicle control, DMSO750 (t(4)=1.4, P=0.25, paired t-test; Figure 5a, right panel), slight reduction was observed in MMPIP750 compared with MMPIP250 injection (t(4)=−4.0, P=0.016, paired t-test). These results suggest that the effects of MMPIP infusion into the BNSTp are not due to diffusion into the lateral ventricle and that an excess dose of MMPIP into the lateral ventricle may affect adjacent areas, including the BNSTp. Taken together, these results indicate that mGluR7 located in the BNSTp has important roles in controlling intermale aggression.

Discussion

The present analysis of mGluR7 KO mice, c-Fos expression, and cerebral MMPIP microinjection indicate that inhibition of mGluR7 impairs intermale aggression owing to information processing dysfunction within the BNST. It is well known that the vomeronasal system is critical for social recognition; therefore, congenital loss of vomeronasal function reduces male-to-male aggression and male-to-female sexual behavior but increases male-to-male sexual behavior (Leypold et al, 2002; Stowers et al, 2002; Chamero et al, 2007), while the main olfactory system also has an involvement (Mandiyan et al, 2005). Male-to-male sexual behavior, reduced duration of anogenital sniffing, and altered urine preference are all consistent with impaired pheromonal information processing, indicating that male mGluR7 KO mice show severe loss of social recognition to other males. The c-Fos-ir pattern in the mGluR7 KO mice indicates that pheromonal information in these mice is detected by the vomeronasal organ and processed normally until it reaches the MeA, but the BNST cannot respond properly. Thus mGluR7 has a crucial role in excitation of the BNST neurons to manage social perception.

Although mGluR7 has been postulated to be one of the most important mGluRs, the physiological roles of mGluR7 in the brain are mostly unclear because of lacking pharmacological tools until recently. MMPIP is a negative allosteric modulator that possesses high subtype selectivity (Conn et al, 2009). Here microinjection of MMPIP into the BNST clearly reproduced the mGluR7 KO phenotype in all parameters tested. Importantly, our microinjection study also showed that the impaired aggression of mGluR7 KO mice cannot be attributed to deficits in the formation of neural circuits without mGluR7 during neural development but that modulation of synapses by mGluR7 is necessary for evoking aggression.

The Role of the BNST in Regulating Emotional Behavior

The BNSTa is currently receiving increased attention, because this area regulates several emotional and motivational processing and because some components of the important pathways in the BNSTa have been revealed, despite the complex circuits involved (Kim et al, 2013; Jennings et al, 2013a, 2013b). The most obvious cell groups in the BNSTp include the principal, interfascicular, and transverse nuclei (Ju and Swanson, 1989). The principal nucleus has crucial roles in controlling the neuroendocrine system and reproductive behavior (Choi et al, 2007; Simerly, 2002), but little is known about the interfascicular and transverse nuclei. Only fragmentary and controversial evidence suggests their involvement in defensive behaviors (Shaikh et al, 1986; Patil and Brid, 2010). Through our study, we noticed that cannulation or microinjection into the BNSTp affects mice aggression, some mice never show aggression, and some mice became hyperaggressive (Supplementary Table S1). These observations indicate the heterogeneous functions of the BNST and explain why previous lesion studies of the BNST have been controversial (Shaikh et al, 1986; Patil and Brid, 2010).

Although the BNST is composed of mainly γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)-releasing cells, glutamatergic neurons are more common in the BNSTp than in the BNSTa (Poulin et al, 2009; Kudo et al, 2012). Blockade of AMPA receptor activity within the BNST is reported to reduce aggression (Carrillo et al, 2011), suggesting that the glutamatergic system in the BNST facilitates aggression. It is likely that mGluR7 modulates the synaptic efficacy of these glutamatergic terminals, although it is unknown if the circuits are extrinsic or intrinsic.

Serotonin has long been considered as a key transmitter in the neurocircuitry controlling aggression (Ferris et al, 1997; Miczek et al, 2001; Nelson and Trainor, 2007). Low-dose cocaine treatment during adolescence facilitates aggression and induces marked decrease in serotonergic innervations in several aggression areas, including the BNST (DeLeon et al, 2002). As excitation of the BNSTp correlate with aggression, the serotonergic signaling in this brain region may function to inhibit the activity of neurons critical to elicit aggression. Because mGluR7 deficiency reduced the urine-induced neural activity in the BNSTp, it is plausible to speculate a functional interaction between mGluR7 and the serotonergic system. The neuropeptides arginine vasopressin (AVP) and oxytocin are important modulators of several social behaviors (Veenema and Neumann, 2008). In addition to paraventricular and supraoptic nuclei, the BNST also contains AVP-synthesizing neurons (De Vries and Buijs, 1983). There is compelling evidence that AVP is important for social recognition (Caldwell et al, 2008), and the expression level of the oxytocin receptor in the BNST is associated with aggression (Calcagnoli et al, 2014). Furthermore, the relationship between AVP and the serotonergic system in the regulation of aggressive behavior has been demonstrated in several studies (Ferris et al, 1997; D'Eath et al, 2005). Because mGluR7 is expressed on both glutamatergic and GABAergic terminals (Kinoshita et al, 1998; Somogyi et al, 2003), mGluR7 may also regulate the release of serotonin, AVP, oxytocin, or GABA, directly or indirectly.

The BNSTp contains many kinds of neurotransmitters (Moga et al, 1989), which should be examined in a future study. Although the complexity of the intrinsic and extrinsic circuits hampered elucidation of the mechanism, we clearly showed that mGluR7 in the BNST is essential for intermale aggression. Our finding is a critical clue for unraveling how the BNST innately evaluates the biological meaning of new objects and regulates emotions.

The BNSTp has extensive connections with the MePD and hypothalamic periventricular region (Dong and Swanson, 2004; Choi et al, 2005), and it is notable that GABAergic neurons in the MePD have recently been reported to promote aggression, as well as two other innate social behaviors, mating and social grooming (Hong et al, 2014). Furthermore, the ventrolateral part of the ventromedial nucleus is reported to control male-to-male and male-to-female social behaviors, such as aggressive behavior, mount, and sniffing (Lee et al, 2014). mGluR7 could have a critical role in some part of this pathway.

Possible Mechanisms for How mGluR7 Regulates the Excitability of the BNST

There are multiple possibilities for how mGluR7 regulates the excitability of groups of neurons. The reduced c-Fos-ir in the BNST of mGluR7 KO mice (Figure 4e and f) provides important evidence for resolving the mechanism. It is quite likely that mGluR7-expressing axons are glutamatergic and assembled to innervate specific GABAergic interneurons in the BNST, because particular inhibitory neurons are decorated with many axon terminals intensely labeled for group III mGluRs in several brain areas (Shigemoto et al, 1996, 1997; Kinoshita et al, 1998). If so, mGluR7 works as an autoreceptor that inhibits glutamate release and maintains the appropriate inhibition mediated by decorated interneurons. In this case, mGluR7 deficiency would inhibit the excitation of BNST neurons that regulate the intermale aggression, via hyperactivation of decorated inhibitory interneurons. The unique expression of mGluR7—that is, target cell specificity and decoration pattern (Shigemoto et al, 1996; Kinoshita et al, 1998)—enables mGluR7 to regulate decorated interneurons strictly and to control the excitability of the local circuit strongly.

In case mGluR7 expressed in glutamatergic axon terminals connects to the excitatory neurons, it is plausible to assume a facilitatory effect of mGluR7. In addition to presynaptic inhibition, mGluR7 has recently been implicated in a novel form of metaplasticity in the hippocampus (Pelkey et al, 2005, 2008). These elegant studies showed that mGluR7 works as a gatekeeper for metaplasticity. In other words, mGluR7 can determine the direction of plasticity, long-term depression vs long-term potentiation. Because this process involves internalization of mGluR7, it has been thought that mGluR7 is merely inactivated, but our c-Fos data indicate another scenario: the synaptic transmission of mGluR7-internalized presynapses must be different from those presynapses that genuinely lack mGluR7. Alteration of synaptic efficacy from depression to potentiation must have a particular impact on enhanced transmission, as supported by the fact that strong stimulation with mGluR7-specific agonists can increase glutamate release (Martín et al, 2010).

In addition to glutamatergic terminals, mGluR7 is also expressed at GABAergic terminals and modulates GABA release (Kinoshita et al, 1998; Somogyi et al, 2003; Summa et al, 2013), suggesting that mGluR7 at GABAergic terminals enhances neuronal excitation by modulating GABA release. In all those cases, mGluR7 must act as an ‘enhancer of excitation’. Thus mGluR7 is certainly required for strong excitation of neurons in the BNST in order to process olfactory information, which is required for social recognition. As mGluR4 and mGluR8, which belong to group III mGluRs, are also known to show distinct localization similar to that of mGluR7 in several brain areas (Shigemoto et al, 1997), it is intriguing to speculate their cooperative or reciprocal interactions in regulating neural activities. Although the physiological relevance of their properties remains to be fully elucidated, here we demonstrate the pivotal role of mGluR7 as an enhancer of excitation in vivo and its significance in intermale aggression.

Future Perspectives and Conclusions

Like other subtypes of mGluRs, mGluR7 is anticipated to be involved in human disease (Nicoletti et al, 2011). mGluR7 KO mice show a deficit in social recognition, aggression, anxiety and fear, stress responses, motivation, aversion learning, and memory (Masugi et al, 1999; Sansig et al, 2001; Cryan et al, 2003; Callaerts-Vegh et al, 2006), and this phenotype suggest that mGluR7 is an essential component of social behavior and innate behavior. By investigating how mGluR7 regulates these emotional behaviors, our current findings could also pave the way toward future discoveries of novel therapies for emotional disorders in humans.

Although the heterogeneity and complexity of the BNST hampered identification of the profiles of mGluR7-expressing neurons and their targets, our study shines a light on the function of the BNST by focusing on mGluR7. In the future, we should elucidate the constituents of the cytoarchitecture and physiological function of the BNST, and we will understand the mechanisms of emotional behaviors, such as aggression, social behavior, and fear. Quite surprisingly, the presynaptic molecule mGluR7 has such a large effect on the excitability of groups of BNST neurons. Further investigations of the role of mGluR7 in the BNST are likely to shed light on the novel principles of synaptic modulation within the neural circuits.

Funding and Disclosure

This work was supported by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology in Japan (KAKENHI 20240036 and 24300128 to MK, and KAKENHI 20500313 and 23500415 to MMT), and Grant FL 729/2-1 from the German Research Foundation (DFG) to PJF. The authors declare no conflict of interest.