Introduction

Emotions, motivations, and reinforcement are a closely related, evolutionarily-conserved phenomena maintaining the integrity of an individual and promoting survival in a natural environment (Atasoy et al, 2012; LeDoux, 2012; Sternson, 2013). However, if such survival circuits are out of balance, maladaptation may arise and result in the development of anxiety disorders. Thus, a better understanding of fear and anxiety includes also the interaction with other life-sustaining brain circuitries and their reciprocal integration.

We therefore hypothesized that modulation of one survival circuit will provoke a significant impact on the other survival circuits. For instance, if a decrease in blood glucose signals that energy homeostasis is out of balance, release of hormones and activation of hypothalamic nuclei will be initiated (Balleine, 2005; Sohn et al, 2013; Williams and Elmquist, 2012). As a consequence, food intake and search for food will prevail and emotional behavior will be adapted accordingly. Indeed, recent experiments in Drosophila suggest a close relation between food intake and the formation of aversive memories (Hirano et al, 2013; Placais and Preat, 2013).

Here, we investigated the neurobiological effects of fasting on the different phases of mammalian fear processing in mice, in particular fear acquisition, consolidation, and, most importantly, fear extinction. The amygdala complex, located in the temporal lobe, is centrally involved in coordinating fear-related processes. It receives information about fear-provoking stimuli via thalamic and cortical afferents and regulates the resulting fear response by efferent projections targeting hypothalamus and brain stem (Ehrlich et al, 2009). The central amygdala (CEA) represents the major output station for generating an adaptive fear response. It consists of a highly elaborate micronetwork that receives afferent projections from the adjacent basolateral amygdala (BLA), intercalated cell masses, and also from more distant brain areas such as hypothalamus and brain stem. Recent evidence suggests that CEA is also essential for suppression of food intake (Cai et al, 2014; Carter et al, 2013) and for directing motivational behavior (Robinson et al, 2014), providing hence a possible hub for integrating survival circuits for fear and hunger. Interestingly, among the multiple neuromodulators that shape amygdala output, the neuropeptide Y system is involved in both feeding and fear. In particular, central NPY Y4 receptors that are targeted by peripherally released pancreatic polypeptide (PP) may link peripheral feeding-related signals to emotional processes in the brain (Holzer et al, 2012).

Experimentally, amygdala functioning and related fear learning can be tested by Pavlovian fear conditioning, in which a subject learns to associate an initially neutral stimulus, such as a tone (conditioned stimulus, CS), with an aversive stimulus, typically a mild electric food shock (unconditioned stimulus, US) (LeDoux, 2000). As a consequence, the presentation of the CS alone or the context (consisting of the conditioning environment, such as light, texture, or odor of the chamber) in which the fear memory was acquired will result in a species-specific fear reaction. Repetitive presentations of the CS in the absence of a shock, however, result in a reduction of the acquired fear reaction. This learning process is termed fear extinction and is the underlying principle of exposure therapy in human patients suffering from anxiety disorders (Davis, 2011; Herry et al, 2010; Myers and Davis, 2007; Ricardo and Koh, 1978).

Here, we provide evidence that the fear-related mechanisms controlled by amygdala circuitries are strongly correlated with those regulating food intake and energy balance. We further demonstrate that short-term fasting results in the suppression of fear by enhancing feed-forward inhibition in an amygdala microcircuit, whereas genetic deletion of the Y4 receptor reduced appetite and impaired fear extinction.

Materials and Methods

Animals

Wild-type (WT) and Y4KO mice, both on a C57Bl/6NCrl background, were bred at the Institute of Pharmacology, Medical University of Innsbruck. All procedures involving animals and animal care were conducted in accordance with international laws and policies (Directive 2010/63/EU of the European parliament and of the council of 22 September 2010 on the protection of animals used for scientific purposes; Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, US National Research Council, 2011) and were approved by the Austrian Ministry of Science. All effort was taken to minimize the number of animals used and their suffering.

Behavioral Experiments

Experiments were performed in adult male C57Bl/6NCrl mice (10–12 weeks old, weighing 22–28 g) during the light phase of the light/dark cycle. Y4KO mice were backcrossed for at least 10 generations to a C57Bl/6NCrl background. They were housed in groups of three to five animals under standard laboratory conditions (12 h/12 h light/dark cycle, lights on: 0700 hours, food and water ad libitum). Generation of Y4KO mice has been described in detail previously (Sainsbury et al, 2002; Tasan et al, 2009).

Fear Conditioning

Fear acquisition, context testing, and reinstatement were performed in context A, consisting of a transparent acrylic rodent conditioning chamber with a metal grid floor that was enclosed by a sound attenuating chamber. Illumination for context A was 80 lx and the chambers were cleaned with 70% ethanol. Fear recall, fear extinction, extinction recall, and reinstatement testing were performed in a different context consisting of a dimly illuminated (10 lx) chamber with black walls and cleaned with 1% acetic acid (context B). On day 1, mice were fear conditioned in context A by repetitive pairing of an auditory stimulus (CS, 30 s white noise, 80 dB) with a mild electric foot shock (US, 2 s, 0.5 mA). All animals received five CS, each of them coterminating with a US. On day 2, mice were tested for their context fear memory in context A (15 min). Fear extinction (15 CS presentations, each 30 s, interstimulus interval 5 s) and extinction recall (5 CS presentations, each 30 s, interstimulus interval 5 s) were performed in context B on days 3 and 4, respectively. As Y4KO mice did not show fear extinction, we performed an extensive extinction protocol, consisting of 5 extinction sessions, with 15 CS each (30 s, interstimulus interval 5 s) and extinction recall was tested the following day (5 CS).

Fasting Procedures

Mice were fasted overnight for 16 h before fear acquisition, for fear conditioning experiments, and starting immediately after or 5 h after fear acquisition for 16 h, for extinction experiments. For fasting, mice were single housed; food and bedding was removed but water was available ad libitum. Control mice were also single housed for the same time without bedding but with access to food and water ad libitum. Bedding was removed because mice that have no access to food tend to eat the bedding of the cage, which may trigger the activation of feeding-related neuronal circuits and release of hormones.

Electrophysiology

Slice electrophysiology was performed on acute slices 24 h after the final behavioral experiment. See Figure 4h and Supplementary Information for details.

Immunohistochemistry

cFos, cFos/GABA, cFos/FOXP2, and cFos/PKCδ were performed as described previously in detail (Tasan et al, 2011). See Supplementary Information for details.

Statistical Analysis

Data are presented as means±SEM and were analyzed for normal distribution and equal variances using the GraphPad Prism 5 software (San Diego, CA). Fear-conditioning experiments were analyzed by repeated two-way ANOVA (time, genotype/treatment, interaction) and Bonferroni post hoc test for selected comparisons. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc test was used to analyze changes in bodyweight and Mann–Whitney test for analyzing US sensitivity threshold.

Results

Short-Term Fasting Specifically Impairs Long-Term Memory but not Short-Term Memory of Cued and Context Fear

To investigate the role of short-term fasting on the acquisition of conditioned fear, male C57BL/6N mice were fasted overnight for 16 h before fear conditioning and their performance was compared with non-fasted littermates (Figure 1a). Short-term fasting resulted in a mean reduction of body weight by 17.4% (Supplementary Figure 1). Acquisition of conditioned fear was unchanged in fasted and non-fasted mice (Figure 1b, two-way ANOVA for repeated measurements: time—F(4,80)=60.74, P<0.0001, treatment—F(1,20)=0.24, P>0.05, interaction F(4,80)=2.51, P>0.05). Context fear memory, however (Figure 1c, 3 min context testing on day 2: t(20)=3.21, P<0.01), tested 24 h after fear acquisition and refeeding by exposing the mice to the original conditioning chamber (context A), was significantly reduced, whereas context fear acquisition was unchanged (Supplementary Figure 2). Owing to reduced context fear expression, context extinction appeared to be facilitated in fasted mice compared with fed mice (Supplementary Figure 2, two-way ANOVA for repeated measurements: time—F(14,280)=6.77, P<0.0001, treatment—F(1,20)=9.84, P<0.01, interaction F(14,280)=0.45, P>0.05). Similarly, freezing to the CS under fed conditions, tested 48 h after fear acquisition by exposing mice to the auditory stimulus alone (CS test, long-term memory (LTM); Figure 1a+d) in a different chamber (context B), was significantly lower in mice that were fasted before fear acquisition (Figure 1d; t(20)=3.20, P<0.01). However, short-term memory (STM) tested (Figure 1a+d, CS test STM, under fed conditions) in separate groups of mice (Figure 1a) was similar in fasted and non-fasted mice, as demonstrated by CS-induced freezing 30 and 150 min after fear acquisition (Figure 1d; t(10)=0.67, P>0.05 and t(10)=1.26, P>0.05, respectively). As the effect of fasting on fear memory developed slowly, we performed fear acquisition after 16 h of fasting and 24 h of refeeding, further demonstrating that fasting did not affect fear acquisition (Supplementary Figure 3). Collectively, these data suggest impaired consolidation or recall of conditioned fear in mice that were fasted before fear acquisition, whereas learning and STM remain unaffected.

Figure 1
figure 1

Acute short-term fasting before fear conditioning inhibits formation of context- and conditioned stimulus (CS)-induced long-term fear memory (LTM) while not affecting short-term memory (STM). (a) Fasting started 16 h before and continued during fear acquisition. All mice had access to food ad libitum after fear acquisition and before and during fear testing. (b) Both fasted and fed mice exhibited similar baseline freezing and acquisition of conditioned fear. (c) Reduced context freezing of mice that were fasted before and during fear acquisition. (d) Mice that were fasted before and during fear acquisition displayed reduced CS-induced freezing 48 h after fear conditioning, whereas STM tested 30 and 150 min after fear acquisition was similar to controls (repeated two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for acquisition, Student’s t-test for context and CS testing, **P<0.01; LTM—food, n=11, fasting: n=11; STM 30 min—food: n=6, fasting: n=6; STM 150 min: food: n=6, fasting: n=6).

PowerPoint slide

Short-Term Fasting Facilitates Extinction Learning and Promotes Extinction Recall

To understand if fasting-induced inhibition of fear depends on memory consolidation and to investigate the effect of fasting on fear extinction, a separate group of mice was tested for fear recall after 16 h of fasting. Fasting was initiated here either immediately or 24 h after fear acquisition (Figure 2a). There was no change in CS-induced freezing during fear recall in mice that were fasted immediately or 24 h after fear acquisition compared with non-fasted controls, suggesting that fasting induced-changes develop slowly, over time (eg, 16 h), and have to be present within the consolidation window to affect long-term fear memory. Interestingly, freezing to the CS upon fear recall was similar in animals that were fasted after fear acquisition and non-fasted controls (Figure 2c; t(16)=0.38, P>0.05), suggesting equal expression of fear in fasted and non-fasted mice. Fear extinction learning, however, was significantly facilitated in fasted mice (Figure 2d; two-way ANOVA for repeated measurements, time—F(24,432)=9.03, P<0.0001, treatment—F(1,18)=5.44, P<0.05 and interaction F(24,432)=3.55, P<0.0001). More importantly, extinction recall, tested under fed conditions 24 h after fear extinction training, by exposing the mice to 5 CS in context B was still reduced in those mice that were fasted before extinction learning (Figure 2e; t(16)=3.61, P<0.01). Taken together, these data indicate that 16 h of acute fasting does not alter learning in general, but rather modulates fear memory by specifically influencing the emotional valance of learning processes. Thus, fasting inhibits the consolidation of an acquired fear memory but promotes the acquisition and consolidation of fear extinction.

Figure 2
figure 2

Short-term fasting before fear extinction improves extinction learning. (a) Following fear conditioning, mice were fasted for 16 h before and during extinction training. Extinction recall was tested 24 h later with food available ad libitum. (b) Following fear acquisition, mice were divided into two equal groups, one that was fasted and one with food available. (c) No difference in conditioned stimulus (CS) induced freezing was observed between fasted and non-fasted mice, (d) but facilitated fear extinction in mice that were fasted 16 h before and during fear extinction learning, and (e) reduced CS-induced freezing in extinction recall of mice that were fasted before and during extinction learning (repeated two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for acquisition and extinction learning, Student’s t-test for CS-induced freezing and extinction recall testing, *P<0.05, **P<0.01; Food: n=10, fasting: n=10).

PowerPoint slide

Genetic Deletion of the Y4 Receptor Reduces Appetite and Impairs Fear Extinction

If survival circuits, such as feeding and fear, were indeed influencing each other, we hypothesized that mice with altered feeding behavior or genetic ablation of feeding-related genes would also display specific changes in fear extinction behavior (Gutman et al, 2008; Verma et al, 2012). Y4 receptors are expressed in the CNS and are activated by PP that is released from the pancreas in response to feeding. Interestingly, Y4KO mice display decreased body weight and reduced food intake, both suggesting chronic suppression of the hunger circuit (Lin et al, 2004; Sainsbury et al, 2002). To investigate the relation of a feeding-related gene and satiety to fear extinction, we subjected Y4KO mice to Pavolvian fear conditioning (Figure 3a). Acquisition (Figure 3b, two-way ANOVA for repeated measurements, time—F(4,48)=27.29, P<0.0001, genotype—F(1,12)=2.54, P>0.05, interaction F(4,48)=1.28, P>0.05) and recall of conditioned fear (Figure 3c; t(13)=0.54, P>0.05) were unchanged in Y4KO mice compared with controls. Fear extinction, however, was significantly impaired in Y4KO mice (Figure 3d and e; two-way ANOVA for repeated measurements, time—F(14,182)=0.82, P>0.05, genotype—F(1,13)=26.90, P<0.001, interaction F(14,182)=3.08, P<0.001, and fear recall—t(13)=3.86, P<0.01), suggesting that genetic alterations in the feeding circuit considerably alter fear behavior. Sensitivity to the electric foot-shock was not different from controls (Supplementary Figure 4). These results suggest that modification of the feeding circuit has a significant impact on fear processing. In particular, dysregulated appetite correlated with impaired fear extinction.

Figure 3
figure 3

Impaired fear extinction in Y4 receptor knockout (KO) mice was rescued by repeated fasting episodes before extinction learning. (a) Y4KO mice and wild-type controls were subjected to fear acquisition, conditioned stimulus (CS) testing 24 h later, and to a total of four extinction sessions on days 2–6, followed by extinction recall on day 7. (b) No difference in fear acquisition and (c) fear recall, but (d) impaired extinction learning (note the apparent reduction of freezing levels in Y4KO mice during CS1 and 3, that was, however, due to jumping behavior, thus probably indicating an increased stress reaction) and (e) extinction recall in Y4KO mice compared with wild-type controls. (f) After fear acquisition, one group of Y4KO mice was subjected to repeated cycles of fasting before and during extinction learning followed by extinction recall under fed conditions. (g) Following fear acquisition, Y4KO mice were divided into two equal groups, one that was fasted before extinction training and one that was not, (h) no difference in fear expression between fasted and non-fasted Y4KO mice, (i) rescued fear extinction in fasted Y4KO mice compared with fed Y4KO controls, (j) successive reduction of freezing in extinction recall on testing days 3, 5, and 7 tested under fed conditions and reduced reinstatement on day 21 in Y4KO mice that were fasted before extinction training, suggesting permanent suppression of fear (repeated two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for acquisition and extinction learning, Student’s t-test for CS-induced freezing and extinction recall and reinstatement testing, *P<0.05, **P<0.01, ***P<0.001; WT vs Y4KO—WT: n=9, Y4KO: n=6 and Y4KO fasted vs non-fasted—Y4KO fasting: n=8, Y4KO food: n=8). KO, knock out.

PowerPoint slide

Short-Term Fasting Rescues Impaired Fear Extinction in Y4KO Mice

Next, we tried to rescue the specifically impaired fear extinction in Y4KO mice by subjecting them to three cycles of extinction-recall sessions, each consisting of 16 h fasting before extinction training followed by extinction recall tested under fed conditions 24 h later (Figure 3f and Supplementary Figure 1 for reduction of body weight). CS-induced freezing in context B on the testing day was similar in fasted and fed Y4KO mice (Figure 3h; t(16)=0.20, P>0.05), indicating equal acquisition and expression of conditioned fear. Extinction learning, however, was significantly enhanced in Y4KO mice that were subjected to 16 h fasting compared with fed Y4KO mice (Figure 3i; two-way ANOVA for repeated measurements, time—F(24,168)=0.72, P>0.05, treatment—F(1,7)=7.46, P<0.05, interaction F(7,168)=1.01, P>0.05). More importantly, this extinction memory was preserved upon refeeding, as demonstrated by the reduced freezing behavior during extinction recall in context B (Figure 3j; t(7)=1.63, P>0.05; t(7)=2.55, P<0.05; t(7)=4.84, P<0.01 for extinction recalls 1, 2, and 3, respectively), suggesting that impaired fear extinction can be rescued by modulation of the feeding circuit.

To investigate whether this reduction of fear was permanent, we subjected these mice 2 weeks after the last extinction trial to a reinstatement paradigm, consisting of one unsignaled foot-shock in context A and testing of CS-induced freezing in context B on days 20 and 21, respectively (Figure 3f). Importantly, CS-induced freezing during reinstatement testing was still reduced in Y4KO mice that were repetitively fasted before extinction learning (Figure 3j; t(7)=3.89, P<0.01), suggesting a long-lasting, stress-resistant suppression of fear.

Short-Term Fasting Specifically Activates Medial Intercalated Neurons and Facilitates Feed-Forward Inhibition from the Basolateral to the Centromedial Amygdala

We next investigated the underlying synaptic correlates linking feeding and fear extinction circuits. Following fear acquisition, Y4KO mice were fasted for 16 h, subjected to extinction training, and brains were processed for immunohistochemistry 90 min after the end of fear extinction training. Compared with non-fasted Y4KO mice (Figure 4a–c), expression of the immediate-early gene cFos was increased in those Y4KO mice that were fasted before extinction training (Figure 4d–f), specifically in the medial intercalated cells (mITCs), a brain nucleus associated with fear extinction (Busti et al, 2011; Likhtik et al, 2008) (Figure 4g; t(14)=2.60, P<0.05). Thus, short-term fasting activates specific neuronal populations in extinction-related brain areas.

Figure 4
figure 4

Fasting in Y4KO mice results in activation of the medial intercalated cell (mITC) and enhanced basolateral amygdala (BLA) to centromedial amygdala (CEm) feed-forward inhibition. (a–c) Expression of immediate-early gene cFos in the ITC of a fasted Y4KO compared with (d–f), a non-fasted Y4KO control reveals (g) increased activation of ITC neurons, (h) experimental setup for ex vivo electrophysiology indicating home-cage controls, wild-type (WT) and Y4KO undergoing fear acquisition on day 1, extinction on day 2, and electrophysiology on day 3 under fed conditions and experimental group with a 16 h fasting period before and during extinction training, (i) enhanced feed-forward inhibition from BLA to CEm via mITC in WT mice after successful fear extinction, (j) lack of increased feed-forward inhibition in fed Y4KO after extinction training corresponding to impaired fear extinction and (k) rescue of impaired fear extinction in Y4KO mice by fasting facilitates enhanced feed-forward inhibition between BLA and CEm. (l) Example traces from WT and Y4KO mice of the individual groups with increasing stimulation intensities (n=WT home cage 4 mice, 12 cells; WT food+Ext 4 mice, 15 cells; Y4KO home cage 5 mice, 13 cells; Y4KO food+Ext 4 mice, 14 cells; Y4KO fasting+Ext 4 mice, 12 cells; *P<0.05, two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for repeated measurements for electrophysiology, and Y4KO food: n=8, Y4KO fasting: n=8; Student’s t test for c-Fos immunohistochemistry). KO, knock out.

PowerPoint slide

As shown previously in rats, fear extinction results in enhanced feed-forward inhibition from the BLA to the CEm, mediated by increased activity of mITC neurons (Amano et al, 2010). To determine whether facilitated fear extinction of fasted mice corresponded to alterations in synaptic neurotransmission, we performed whole-cell patch-clamp recordings to measure BLA to CEm feed-forward inhibition in acute amygdala slices of mice 24 h after two extinction trainings. Stimulation of the BLA at intensities ranging from 100 to 500 μA consistently evoked an IPSP in CEm neurons with an initial, brief EPSP component (Figure 4l). Compared with untrained C57BL/6N mice (WT home cage), the amplitude of IPSPs was increased 24 h after successful fear extinction (Figure 4i; two-way ANOVA F(2,36)=4.93, P<0.01). Bath application of the AMPA receptor antagonist, DNQX, abolished both the IPSP and EPSP (Supplementary Figure 5A), whereas application of GABA receptor antagonist, picrotoxin, completely blocked the IPSP. These data confirm that fear extinction results in enhanced feed-forward inhibition in an amygdala microcircuit connecting the BLA with the CEm.

In Y4KO mice (home cage), similar as in WT mice, electrical stimulation of the BLA consistently evoked a brief EPSP, followed by a larger IPSP in CEm neurons (Figure 4l). The amplitude of evoked IPSPs did not significantly differ between untrained fed or fasted Y4KO mice (both home cage), or Y4KO mice that underwent fear acquisition (Supplementary Figures 5B–D). However, in contrast to wild-type mice, and in line with the impaired fear extinction of Y4KO mice, feed-forward inhibition from BLA to CEm neurons also remained unchanged in Y4KO mice 24 h after two consecutive fear extinction trainings (Figure 4j). However, CEm neurons recorded from Y4KO mice that were fasted for 16 h before the two fear extinction trainings, and thus had successfully acquired extinction memory, exhibited larger IPSPs in response to BLA stimulation (Figure 4k; two-way ANOVA F4,60=2.90, P<0.05). As fasted Y4KO mice displayed reduced freezing levels already after two extinction sessions (Figure 3j), we decided to perform electrophysiological recordings after the second extinction. No changes in general membrane properties were observed (Supplementary Figure 6). These data suggest that the impaired fear extinction in Y4KO mice is owing to the lack of inhibitory synaptic plasticity in BLA to CEm projections and that fasting before fear extinction rescues fear extinction by specifically activating medial ITC neurons, leading to a marked increase in BLA to CEm feed-forward inhibition.

Discussion

Here, we demonstrated that short-term fasting differentially affects fear and fear extinction learning. This differential effect on two fear-related learning processes suggests that the effect of acute fasting on fear was not a uniform inhibition or promotion of learning and memory per se. It rather indicates that short-term fasting manipulates these memories toward a reduction of fear. This is interesting in the light of an evolutionarily-conserved mechanism that adapts explorative behavior depending on internal homeostatic demand. Thus, in situations of increased hunger a subject will be prepared to take a higher risk, whereas in a saturated state safety concerns will predominate.

Furthermore, we demonstrated that genetic deletion of the Y4 receptor impaired fear extinction, an effect that was rescued by short-term fasting. PP that is released peripherally upon food intake serves as the main ligand for Y4 receptors (Holzer et al, 2012). In the brain, Y4 receptors are localized in specific areas that are in part open to the blood–brain barrier, such as hypothalamus and brain stem (Tasan et al, 2009). These brain regions are tightly integrated into the ascending reticular-activating system that may provide the necessary arousal for successful fear extinction. In particular, β-adrenoreceptor-dependent activation of the nucleus tractus solitarii (NTS) is essential for amygdala activation and respective memory formation (McGaugh, 2004; Mueller et al, 2008). Interestingly, Y4 receptors are highly expressed in the NTS and peripheral PP injection results in fast activation of NTS neurons (Tasan et al, 2009). Furthermore, a reduction of adrenergic tonus and sympathetic activity has been reported in Y4KO mice (Smith-White et al, 2002). Thus, deletion of Y4 receptors in the NTS may inhibit extinction learning by dampening the necessary arousal. On the other hand, short-term fasting may have rescued fear extinction in Y4KO mice by alternative activation systems and release of stress hormones (McGaugh, 2004).

Besides PP, NPY also displays affinity for central Y4 receptors at nanomolar concentrations (Bard et al, 1995; Gehlert et al, 1996), suggesting that central Y4 receptors may be either targeted by peripherally released PP or by central NPY. Expression of the immediate-early gene cFos demonstrated that short-term fasting specifically activates AGRP/NPY neurons in the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus (Supplementary Figure 7). Although activation of some arcuate AGRP/NPY neuron projections promote food intake others do not (Betley et al, 2013; Wu et al, 2009). In particular, AGRP/NPY neurons that are projecting to limbic areas, such as the amygdala, may be crucial for adapting emotional behaviors to the internal homeostatic situation. This could also be achieved by NPY release that is involved in both regulating feeding and reducing anxiety (Bacchi et al, 2006; Kask et al, 2002; Tasan et al, 2010). Recent evidence demonstrated that NPY suppresses the expression of conditioned fear and promotes fear extinction (Fendt et al, 2009; Gutman et al, 2008; Lach and de Lima, 2013), extending the anxiolytic properties of NPY to models of learned fear. In fact, similar to Y4KO mice, NPYKO mice also fail to extinguish learned fear (Verma et al, 2012), further emphasizing the central role of feeding-related genes in the modulation of fear extinction. In our experiments, the extensive activation of AGRP/NPY neurons and the consecutive release of NPY upon fasting may have significantly contributed to the facilitated extinction learning. Furthermore, NPY is released from a considerable number of amygdala interneurons, and injection of NPY into the BLA may facilitate fear extinction (Gutman et al, 2008).

The amygdala complex is crucially involved in mediating fear- and anxiety-related behaviors (Pape and Pare, 2010; Quirk and Mueller, 2008). This is achieved by extensive reciprocal connections with hypothalamus, brain stem, and cortical areas. Short-term fasting results in a drop of glucose levels, activation of the autonomic nervous system, and release of stress hormones. This general activation is important, as a certain degree of arousal is essential for successful fear extinction (McGaugh, 2004). For instance, targeting the catecholaminergic system by yohimbine or L-DOPA, but also release of stress hormones, such as glucocorticoids, have been shown to facilitate fear extinction learning in rodents and humans (Fitzgerald et al, 2014; Holmes and Quirk, 2010; McGuire et al, 2014; Soravia et al, 2006). The duration of the fasting period may be a crucial factor. Although prolonged fasting (24 h and more) resulted in facilitated extinction it also increased freezing behavior to the first CS, probably by augmenting stress levels. On the other hand, shorter fasting periods may not provide the necessary motivation to reduce fear. Thus, fasting may trigger the synchronous release of different neuromodulators and hormones that ultimately promote synaptic plasticity in the amygdala as the central stage for fear and extinction learning. Specifically, using cFos mapping, our experiments indicate that the interaction of fear and hunger takes place on the level of the amygdala and more precisely in the medial intercalated neurons. These neurons are extensively activated after successful extinction learning (Busti et al, 2011), whereas ablation of the mITCs completely abolishes consolidation of fear extinction (Likhtik et al, 2008). Recently, the electrophysiological correlate of fear extinction has been pinpointed to an amygdala microcircuit connecting the BLA with the CEm via mITC (Amano et al, 2010). Here, we investigated this microcircuit in the mouse brain and demonstrated that short-term fasting before extinction training activates inhibitory neurons in the mITCs projecting to the main output nucleus of the amygdala, the CEm, with a concomitant increase in feed-forward inhibition from the BLA and improved extinction memory. Taken together with the reduced freezing behavior during extinction recall tested under fed conditions, these results confirm that short-term fasting does not unspecifically increase locomotion, but rather promotes extinction learning by activating mITCs in an extinction-relevant amygdala microcircuit.

It is important to note that fasting affects both context fear and fear extinction, two phenomena that are sensitive to environmental encoding and hippocampal damage (Corcoran et al, 2005; Hobin et al, 2006). The hippocampus modulates fear behavior by direct connections to the amygdala or indirectly via activation of the prefrontal cortex (Maren, 2005). Interestingly, the infralimbic region of the prefrontal cortex activates inhibitory intercalated neurons (Amir et al, 2011; Pinard et al, 2012), a population of neurons that were activated in our experiments by short-term fasting and consequently reduced CEm activation by feed-forward inhibition (Supplementary Figure 8).

Fear memories are strong and often persist lifelong, whereas extinction memories are rather labile and transient, resulting in relapse of fear, in particular, under stressful situations (Ji and Maren, 2007; Quirk and Mueller, 2008). Here, we demonstrate that short-term fasting before fear extinction not only rescues impaired fear extinction in Y4KO mice but more importantly results in a permanent suppression of fear, even under stressful situations. Given that treating human anxiety disorders by exposure therapy is not equally effective in all patients and relieves symptoms only temporarily, an efficient supportive treatment is required. Our results indicate that genetic deletion of genes that reduce appetite may also impair fear extinction and that bypassing the circuit, for example, by increasing appetite through periods of mild fasting, can rescue impaired fear extinction. Thus, fasting before exposure therapy may be a valuable supportive therapeutic option with fast translation into clinics and may be well accepted by patients. Furthermore, the molecular machinery of the feeding circuit may provide novel targets for pharmacological treatment of anxiety disorders.

Funding and Disclosure

There are no personal financial holdings of any of the authors that could be perceived as constituting a potential conflict of interest.