Introduction

About one-sixth of the population in Western countries has been reported to be at risk of poverty (Destatis, 2013; Short, 2013). Consistent evidence indicates that poverty is a main determinant of increased psychopathology such as conduct disorder (CD) (Costello et al, 2003; Evans et al, 2013). CD is characterized by a persistent pattern of rule violation and antisocial behavior including aggressive, deceptive, and destructive behaviors toward peers and adults (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). In their natural experiment including income intervention, Costello et al (2003) demonstrated that poverty is linked to the development of CD implying social causation pathways, that is, exposure to adversity and stress. Numerous studies have confirmed the relationship between poverty and externalizing psychopathology (eg, Emerson et al, 2011; Midouhas et al, 2013). Moreover, the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) was identified as one region that is compromised in CD and antisocial behavior (Blair, 2004; Rubia, 2011). This region is typically involved during the regulation of emotional or motivational behavior (Rubia, 2011). Consistent with this, a number of studies have found blunted neural activity during reward processing (Rubia et al, 2009) and passive avoidance learning (Finger et al, 2011), indicating deficient learning of reward or punishment contingencies, which may, in turn, result in less effective decision making. Likewise, a decreased OFC volume has been found in CD patients (Huebner et al, 2008; Yang and Raine, 2009; Fairchild et al, 2011).

Given these relationships of both poverty and OFC deficits with CD, an intriguing question is whether poverty increases the risk for CD by affecting OFC integrity. However, this hypothesis has not been tested in previous work, and only a small number of neuroimaging studies have investigated the impact of childhood poverty/low socioeconomic status (SES, including poverty and low parental education) on brain structure and function even in a cross-sectional design, with longitudinal data in adults being rare. In children, the majority of studies have revealed deficits in various brain regions. As an example, Luby et al (2013) found smaller amygdala and hippocampal volumes in poor children, with the latter being mediated by caregiver support. In contrast, poverty has been linked to smaller hippocampal, but unaltered amygdala volumes in children and adolescents (Hanson et al, 2011; Noble et al, 2012), whereas no such associations were observed by Butterworth et al (2012). Furthermore, in infants from a poor family background, lower frontal and parietal gray matter volumes have been reported, which were found to be associated with externalizing psychopathology (Hanson et al, 2013). Likewise, SES has been related to cortical thickness in the anterior cingulate gyrus and the left superior frontal gyrus (Lawson et al, 2013). However, this relationship appeared to be mainly driven by parental education. Finally, Jednorog et al (2012) demonstrated an effect of SES on the volumes of various brain regions, including the insula, superior frontal gyrus, and hippocampus. On a functional level, poverty has been related to less prefrontal activity during emotion regulation, which was found to be mediated by chronic stress exposure (Kim et al, 2013) and less default-mode network connectivity, which was inversely related to stress reactivity (Sripada et al, 2014). Although previous research has provided preliminary evidence of structural alterations in children exposed to poverty, no study so far has examined the long-term impact of this early life adversity on brain anatomy until adulthood.

Likewise, given the cross-sectional nature of previous studies (eg, Noble et al, 2012), control for additional confounds may not have been possible. As an example, poverty and poor parental education are known to be closely linked, although they might have differential effects on health outcomes (Lawson et al, 2013). Moreover, low SES has been associated with poor birth outcomes such as low birth weight and preterm birth (Potijk et al, 2013). Likewise, history of parental psychopathology (Shelton et al, 2011) should be considered to exclude a shared genetic liability. Finally, given the fact that poverty is a distal environmental risk factor, more proximal measures such as stressful life events (SLE) are likely to mediate its impact (Kim et al, 2013). Moreover, additional pathways have to be considered when investigating the specific effect of poverty on externalizing psychopathology and brain morphology such as exposure to SLEs (Flouri et al, 2013), smoking during pregnancy (Holz et al, 2014), childhood maltreatment (Kunitz et al, 1998), and maternal support (Luby et al, 2013), all of which being increased in poor families (Eiden et al, 2013; Kim et al, 2013; Eckenrode et al, 2014). Furthermore, CD is highly comorbid with ADHD (Hamshere et al, 2013) and substance abuse (Carpentier et al, 2012), which also have to be taken into account.

Using longitudinal data from an epidemiological cohort study of 167 young adults with no current psychopathology followed since birth (Laucht et al, 2000), the present investigation allowed us to account for all the confounders previously mentioned when assessing the specific long-term effect of early life poverty on OFC volume during adulthood. Furthermore, we aimed to (i) replicate the well-established link between poverty and child and adolescent CD, (ii) examine the association of OFC volume alterations with CD, and (iii) test for a possible mediation of the impact of poverty on CD by SLEs, smoking during pregnancy, childhood maltreatment, and maternal responsiveness, as well as with current OFC volume, which is assumed to be a stable trait-like marker (Blair, 2004).

Materials and Methods

Sample

This investigation was conducted in the framework of the Mannheim Study of Children at Risk, an ongoing epidemiological cohort study of the long-term outcome of early risk factors (for full details cf., Laucht et al, 2000 and see Supplementary Materials and Methods). Assessments were conducted at the age of 3 months and at regular intervals throughout development, most recently at the age of 25 years (Supplementary Figure 1). Psychopathology was assessed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I German version; Wittchen et al, 1997), and only healthy participants were included. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Heidelberg and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Assessments

Early life Poverty

Early life poverty was determined by a standardized interview conducted with the mother at the 3-month assessment and defined as an income level below the risk-of-poverty threshold, which is set at 60% of the national median equivalized disposable income adjusted for household size according to the Federal Statistical Office of Germany (Destatis, 2012), resulting in an dichotomous variable (0=not exposed and 1=exposed). This definition of poverty is also commonly used in international contexts (Emerson et al, 2011; Lelkes and Gasior, 2011; Destatis, 2012). From N=167 participants, 33 (19.8%) were categorized as being at risk of poverty (details in Supplementary Materials and Methods). In addition, we validated our results using the log-transformed income-to-needs ratio (US Census Bureau, 2004), to account for the nonlinearity and to weight the effect of being at risk of poverty more than the differentiation in the upper income range (respective methods and results are described in the supplement).

Covariates and Confounders

The results were adjusted for several important confounders, such as sex, parental characteristics (education, psychopathology, and delinquency), obstetric adversity, and current poverty (see Supplementary Materials and Methods for details). To address confounding with regard to CD, we, additionally, assessed lifetime ADHD symptoms and lifetime substance abuse, including nicotine, alcohol, and cannabis abuse (see Supplementary Materials and Methods).

Life Stress

To assess exposure to life stress (LS), a semistructured parent interview was conducted at each assessment until the age of 15 years. From the age of 19 years onwards, the young adults were interviewed. The interview, a modified and shortened version of the Munich Events List (Maier-Diewald et al, 1983), evaluated the occurrence of adverse life events during a period of 1 year before the assessment. The items (42–59, adjusted for developmental period) covered all relevant areas of children’s and young adults’ LS, including family, school, parents, health, legal troubles, and living conditions, such as birth of a sibling, death of a close relative, or parents’ separation. Two composite scores were computed by summing up the z-standardized scores, depending on the time point of the outcome assessment (details in Supplementary Materials and Methods).

Prenatal Maternal Smoking

Maternal smoking during pregnancy was determined by a standardized interview with the mother conducted at the 3-month assessment. In all, 77.2% (N=129) mothers were nonsmokers, 8.4% (N=14) reported smoking 1–5 cigarettes per day, and 14.4% (N=24) more than 5 cigarettes per day as previously described by Holz et al (2014).

Maternal Responsiveness

An observational procedure was used to assess the quality of mother–infant interaction at the participant’s age of 3 months as described by Laucht et al (2001). Mother–infant dyads were observed and videotaped during a 10-min semistructured session at our laboratory. A score for maternal responsiveness in interaction with her infant was computed by summing up the mother scales, including emotion, physical affect, vocalization, lack of verbal restrictions, congruency, variability, reactivity, and stimulation, across minutes (mean=317.43, SD=34.80). More detailed information can be found in the supplement.

Childhood Maltreatment

At the age of 23 years, participants completed the brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ; Bernstein et al, 2003). The CTQ entails a retrospective assessment of five types of self-reported childhood maltreatment, that is, sexual, physical, and emotional abuse, as well as emotional and physical neglect (details in Supplementary Materials and Methods). The CTQ yields a total score of each type of maltreatment, which was used in the current investigation.

CD Symptoms

CD symptoms according to DSM-IV were assessed in 165 individuals using the Mannheim Parent Interview (Esser et al, 1989) at the age of 8 and 11 years and the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children (Kaufman et al, 1996) at the age of 15 and 19 years. The MEI is a highly structured interview adapted from Rutter’s parent interviews, which was modified to include all symptoms related to major DSM-IV diagnoses, and has been shown to be a sensitive measure of child disturbance (Holz et al, 2014). The K-SADS is a widely used structured diagnostic interview completed independently with parents and children (at the age of 19 years with adolescents only). A sum score of the z-transformed values was computed. Further details on psychometric properties are in Supplementary Materials and Methods. Supplementary Table S1 lists the number of subjects with DSM-IV CD diagnosis (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), the rates being well in line with the known higher prevalence in boys compared with girls during childhood and adolescence (Nock et al, 2006). None of them was in the clinical range at age 25 years.

Voxel-Based Morphometry

We acquired 1 × 1 × 1 mm3 T1-weighted anatomical images with 192 slices covering the whole brain (matrix 256 × 256, repetition time=2300 ms, echo time=3.03 ms, 50% distance factor, field of view 256 × 256 × 192 mm3, flip angle 9°) using a 3 T scanner (Magnetom TRIO; Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a standard 12-channel head coil. Images were bias-corrected and classified into gray matter, white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, and non-brain tissue using Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration through Exponentiated Lie algebra registration toolbox (Ashburner, 2007). Data were segmented based on tissue probability maps, indexing the prior probability of any voxel in a registered image being grey or white matter, or cerebrospinal fluid. An average template from the data was created, to which the images were registered. Additionally, the images were affine-transformed to MNI space and smoothed with an 8-mm FWHM kernel. Total intracranial volume was calculated by adding the tissue probabilities of gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid and used as a covariate. For group statistics of gray matter images, early life poverty (0=not present and 1=present) was entered as a covariate of interest while additionally controlling for the covariates previously mentioned. To test our a priori hypothesis, as described in previous research (Blair, 2004; Rubia, 2011; Blair, 2013), anatomical ROI masks comprising the OFC were derived from the WFU PickAtlas v.2.4 (Maldjian et al, 2003), where family-wise error (FWE) correction was applied with a threshold of p<0.05. Additionally, exploratory whole brain analyses were performed with a p<0.001 uncorrected threshold and a criterion of 10 adjacent voxels. For display purposes, the statistical threshold was set at p=0.005 uncorrected in the ROI. To visualize and further analyze the effects using regression analysis, mean contrast values of each participant were extracted from the significant cluster and exported to SPSS Statistics 20 (IBM, Armonk, NY). Given the previously reported association of the OFC with CD (Blair, 2004; Rubia, 2011), we examined whether brain volume was related to CD symptoms, performing partial correlations controlled for the covariates previously mentioned.

Mediation

Mediation was tested under consideration of all covariates previously mentioned (see Supplementary Materials and Methods).

Results

Sample Characteristics

As expected, higher rates of parental psychiatric disorder and delinquency were found in the group at risk of early poverty compared with those non-exposed. Moreover, in line with previous research, the poverty group exhibited more CD and ADHD symptoms and were exposed to higher levels of LS and smoking during pregnancy, as well as to less maternal responsiveness. No group differences with regard to gender, obstetric adversity, parental education, current poverty, total intracranial volume, lifetime substance abuse, and CTQ emerged (Table 1).

Table 1 Sample Characteristics

In contrast, the association between the log-transformed income-to-needs ratio and CD just failed to reach significance (p=0.05; see Supplementary Results).

Voxel-Based Morphometry

Individuals exposed to early life poverty (N=33) exhibited a smaller OFC volume, when adjusted for total intracranial volume, sex, parental characteristics (education, psychopathology, and delinquency), obstetric adversity, and current poverty, when compared with those not exposed to poverty (N=134; t(158)=3.80, pFWE=0.02, peak 6, 45–18; Figure 1). Further volume decreases in those exposed to early poverty when compared with the non-exposed group were found in the inferior temporal gyrus, superior orbitofrontal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, precentral gyrus, medial frontal gyrus, and the bilateral insula using a more liberal threshold of puncorr=0.001. There was no region showing volume increases in individuals exposed to poverty. After adjustment for CD symptoms as an additional covariate, the results remained significant (t(155)=3.46, pFWE=0.045). In contrast to early poverty, current poverty was unrelated to differences in OFC volume (t(157)=2.08, pFWE=0.80). When using the continuous log-transformed income-to-needs ratio as a covariate of interest, the results were even more significant (described in Supplementary Results).

Figure 1
figure 1

Effect of exposure to early life poverty on orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) volume. Left: Volume is decreased in individuals exposed to poverty (pFWE=0.02). Right: Corresponding parameter estimates of the non-exposed and the exposed groups (N=167; mean±SEM are indicated, ***p<0.001).

PowerPoint slide

Association Between OFC Volume and CD

There was an inverse association between OFC volume and CD symptoms (r=−0.19, p=0.01; Figure 2), indicating that OFC volume decreased with the number of CD symptoms.

Figure 2
figure 2

Scatter plot of the correlation between the significant cluster in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and conduct disorder (CD) symptoms (N=165).

PowerPoint slide

Mediation

A significant effect of exposure to early life poverty on CD symptoms was obtained (β=0.99, SE=0.476, p=0.039; Figure 3a). Including SLEs as a mediator revealed a significant indirect effect (Sobel test, p=0.037), with a significant path between exposure to poverty and LS (β=1.59, SE=0.70, p=0.025), and between LS and CD symptoms (β=0.27, SE=0.05, p<0.001). Moreover, the direct path between poverty and CD was no longer significant (β=0.58, SE=0.45, p=0.20), suggesting that the effect was fully mediated by LS. In contrast, OFC volume proved to be not mediated by LS (Sobel test p=0.97), with no significant path between LS and OFC volume (β=0.00, SE=0.001, p=0.82). Accordingly, the effect of poverty on OFC volume remained significant (β=−0.05, SE=0.01, p<0.001). In addition, smoking during pregnancy mediated the relationship between poverty and CD (p=0.004; Figure 3b), with significant paths between poverty and smoking during pregnancy (β=0.57, SE=0.13, p<0.001) and between smoking during pregnancy and CD (β=1.05, SE=0.29, p<0.001). The effect of poverty on CD was no longer significant (β=0.40, SE=0.49, p=0.82), indicating full mediation. However, mediation did not apply to the association between poverty and OFC volume (p=0.10), which remained significant when including smoking during pregnancy (t(156)=3.33, pFWE=0.04). Thus, in addition to life events, we present another possible pathway by which poverty may exert its effects on CD. In contrast, maternal responsiveness and childhood trauma did neither mediate the association of poverty with CD nor with OFC volume (all p>0.92). Finally, we were interested in whether the effect of poverty on CD symptoms could have been mediated by OFC volume decrease. The Sobel test revealed full mediation (p=0.04; Figure 3c), comprising significant paths between poverty and OFC volume (β=−0.05, SE=0.01, p<0.001) and OFC volume and CD (β=−8.41, SE=3.44, p=0.02), with the direct effect being no longer significant (β=0.61, SE=0.49, p=0.22).

Figure 3
figure 3

Mediation analysis. Path model showing that life stress (a, from age 3 months up to 19 years, p=0.04), smoking during pregnancy (b, p=0.004) and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) volume (c, p=0.04) fully mediated the association between exposure to poverty and conduct disorder (CD) symptoms.

PowerPoint slide

Role of Comorbidity

Considering the impact of comorbid psychopathology, our results suggest that substance abuse neither had a significant impact on the association between poverty and OFC volume (t(155)=3.7, pFWE=0.02) nor on the association between poverty and CD (β=0.945, SE=0.46, p=0.04). No significant partial correlations between OFC volume and the different substance abuse scores were found (all p>0.26). In contrast, consistent with the well-known comorbidity between CD and ADHD, the association between poverty and CD remained no longer significant when additionally controlled for ADHD (β=0.209, SE=0.40, p=0.61). However, the association with OFC volume was unaffected (t(154)=3.51, pFWE=0.04). Likewise, under consideration of all covariates, lifetime ADHD symptoms showed an inverse relationship with OFC volume at a trend level (r=−0.15, p=0.06).

Discussion

The investigation reported here provides first evidence of the long-term impact of early life poverty on OFC volume in young adults. Specifically, our results from a prospective study over 25 years indicate that individuals who were born into poor circumstances exhibited a decreased volume in the OFC, irrespective of a range of potential confounders. Moreover, we replicated pertinent findings of increased CD symptoms as a consequence of childhood poverty, with this effect being mediated by the exposure to LS, smoking during pregnancy, and OFC volume decrease.

The OFC is referred to as a hot paralimbic region (Rubia, 2011), which is particularly involved in the top-down regulation of emotional and motivational behavior, impulse control, as well as goal-directed behavior (Davidson et al, 2000). OFC activation is typically elicited during the perception of angry faces (Blair, 2004) as well as during the processing of rewards, which are both crucial for social behavior (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2004). Remarkably, this structure has previously been found to be associated with environmental risk (eg, Meyer-Lindenberg and Tost, 2012). In addition, a decreased OFC response has been shown to predict aggressive behavior (Coccaro et al, 2007), whereas reduced volumes have been observed in violent, antisocial, and psychopathic individuals (Yang and Raine, 2009). CD is characterized by aggression, destructive behavior, deceitfulness or theft, and the violation of rules, suggesting a key regulatory deficit. A smaller volume in the OFC might lead to impairment in integrating emotional and motivational features of social interactions, which, in turn, might result in an inability to reflect the consequences of actions and subsequently give rise to antisocial and violent behavior. In line with this suggestion, we found that a smaller OFC volume was related to more conduct problems.

As poverty does not exert its influence on the individual in a direct manner, more proximal measures are likely to mediate the effect. In general, three possible pathways may link poverty and health. First, psychosocial adversity (Luby et al, 2013), such as exposure to LS, could be considered as a possible mediator of the impact of poverty on behavioral or brain outcomes. Although our findings provided support for this hypothesis with regard to CD symptoms, no such mediation was found with regard to OFC volume. In contrast, childhood maltreatment and maternal responsiveness were found to not impact the relationship of poverty with CD and OFC volume. In addition to a mediating role of LS, we found smoking during pregnancy as mediating the relationship between poverty and CD. This is well in line with studies revealing increased rates of smoking during pregnancy in women from a low-income background (Eiden et al, 2013), suggesting a second pathway, that is, increased exposure to substances during pregnancy, by which poverty may exerts its influence. A third material perspective that has been discussed, which could not be examined here, highlights that living in poverty results in diminished access to resources and to malnutrition (Lynch et al, 2000). For example, vitamin D deficiency leads to abnormal brain development such as reduced neuronal differentiation, with vitamin D receptors being prevalent in the orbital and cingulate cortices (Eyles et al, 2009). Notably, vitamin D intake is lower in people living in poverty (Moore et al, 2014). Most likely, these pathways are not mutually exclusive and may rather act in concert to promote the indirect effect of poverty on health.

Interestingly, when considering the role of comorbidity, ADHD but not substance abuse significantly impacted the relationship between poverty and CD. This is not surprising with regard to studies showing that about 30–50% of children exhibit comorbid CD and ADHD (Hamshere et al, 2013). However, conduct problems often precede the development of substance abuse (Hawkins et al, 1992), which may explain why the association between poverty and CD remained significant under consideration of substance abuse. In contrast, none of the comorbid symptoms had an impact on the association with OFC volume, which may speak for a higher sensitivity of the brain outcome. Consistent with the role of the OFC in emotional and motivational behavior (Davidson et al, 2000) and aggression (Coccaro et al, 2007), an inverse relationship of OFC volume was found with CD but not with ADHD or substance abuse.

Several limitations of our study have to be considered when evaluating the results. First, CD symptoms were not assessed concurrently with structural MRI data; thus, cause–effect relationships are difficult to establish. Although the latter were collected during young adulthood (at the age of 25 years), CD symptoms were obtained during childhood and adolescence (between the ages of 8 and 19 years), a developmental period characterized by a high prevalence of antisocial behavior. Moreover, as the MRI findings were obtained in currently healthy individuals, they may be interpreted as indicating a higher susceptibility to CD or even the long-term vestiges of this disorder into adulthood. However, our MRI results could not be attributed to CD, as adjustment for CD symptoms did not change the principal finding. In accordance with previous reports, suggesting that the OFC functions as a stable marker and mediator of aggressive behavior (Blair, 2004), we found that the relationship between poverty and CD symptoms was mediated by OFC volume decrease. However, this finding has to be interpreted with caution, given the temporal order constraints of conventional mediation analysis. Thus, future studies should assess both outcomes simultaneously to confirm this finding. In this line, although both smoking during pregnancy and early life poverty were assessed at the same time point (at the age of 3 months), we assumed, based on the pertinent literature, that poverty preceded pregnancy (Eiden et al, 2013). However, we cannot be sure whether smoking during pregnancy per se is the mediating agent or possible neurobiological consequences acting in early life, such as negative effects of nicotine on acetylcholine receptors and cholinergic neurotransmission in the offspring (eg, Slotkin et al, 2004). Second, only a small proportion of our sample was at risk of poverty. Although this rate is even above the general rates of poverty in Western countries (Destatis, 2013; Short, 2013), it may result in an underpowered analysis and could have biased the findings. Therefore, we validated our MRI finding using the income-to-needs ratio as a continuous measure of poverty. Although the impact on OFC volume was robustly confirmed and was even more significant, the effect on CD symptoms fell just short of significance (p=0.05). This may imply that both effects are promoted via different neural trajectories, comprising, on the one hand, a pure linear, and, on the other hand, a nonlinear relationship. In contrast to the impact on OFC volume, the effect on CD may rather reflect a general impact of being at risk of poverty, including, among others, inadequate conditions of health care and nutrition, as well as the self-perception of being underprivileged. It is likely that there is no linear association in the upper range of the income-to-needs ratio, that is, rich people are not more prone to develop CD when compared with richer ones indicating that effects are likely to arise in the lower range of poverty (Barajas et al, 2008). In line with this, previous studies have provided evidence for a robust categorical relationship between poverty and CD (eg, Costello et al, 2003; Emerson et al, 2011; Midouhas et al, 2013). Third, genetic aspects may have a role in mediating the effect of poverty on OFC volume. However, by controlling for parental mental health and delinquency, we partly accounted for a possible genetic impact. Fourth, poverty is a distal measure associated with a variety of variables, which may be society-specific, that is, poverty may imply different confounding factors in different societies. Thus, any association with health outcomes may not be generalizable across societies. Fifth, we do not have data on maternal dietary patterns during pregnancy. However, poverty is a summary of different mechanisms converging and thus provides a strong measure of cumulating risks. Future studies are warranted to clarify the role of diet.

In conclusion, poverty may have a deleterious long-term impact on OFC structure, with lower volumes being related to CD. In this respect, the present study contributes to providing a better understanding of the mechanisms underlying the detrimental effects of early environmental adversity on brain development and behavior during later life. Hence, our results strengthen the importance of early postnatal intervention programs aiming to minimize income-related disparities.

Funding and Disclosure

This work was supported by grants from the German Research Foundation (grant number DFG LA 733/1-2) to ML, DB, AML, and TB. The funding source had no role in study design, in collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, in the writing of the report, and in the decision to submit the article for publication. TB served in an advisory or consultancy role for Hexal Pharma, Lilly, Medice, Novartis, Otsuka, Oxford outcomes, PCM Scientific, Shire, and Viforpharma. He received conference attendance support and conference support or received speaker's fee by Lilly, Medice, Novartis, and Shire. He is/has been involved in clinical trials conducted by Lilly, Shire, and Viforpharma. AML receives consultant fees and travel expenses from AstraZeneca, Hoffmann-La Roche, Lundbeck Foundation, speaker's fees from Pfizer Pharma, Lilly Deutschland, Glaxo SmithKline, Janssen Cilag, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Lundbeck, and AstraZeneca.