INTRODUCTION

Substantial changes in brain and behavior occur between weaning and early adulthood. For example, there is a marked increase in peer–peer interactions, signified by an abundance of social play behavior, which peaks during the juvenile/early adolescent phase and declines to low levels after sexual maturation (Panksepp, 1981). Proper social interactions during this phase of life are thought to be important for social and cognitive development (Pellis and Pellis, 2009; Špinka et al, 2001; Vanderschuren et al, 1997). Indeed, social isolation of rats during the juvenile/early adolescent phases produces long-lasting impairments in social capacities and cognitive control of behavior, even after prolonged re-socialization (van den Berg et al, 1999; Baarendse et al, 2013), and social traumas during childhood and adolescence can increase the risk for psychopathology in later life (Braun and Bock, 2011). Likewise, abnormalities in social play behavior are observed in childhood psychiatric disorders such as autism and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Alessandri, 1992; Manning and Wainwright, 2010). Therefore, identifying the neural underpinnings of social play behavior will increase our understanding of normal social development as well as of the etiology of childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders.

Although the neural substrates of social play behavior are incompletely understood (Siviy and Panksepp, 2011; Trezza et al, 2010; Vanderschuren et al, 1997), there is evidence to support a role for the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and striatum in the regulation of social play behavior (Bell et al, 2009; Panksepp et al, 1994; Pellis et al, 1993, 2006; Schneider and Koch, 2005; Trezza et al, 2011, 2012). As it is in these regions that profound neural changes occur during the juvenile and adolescent period (Casey and Jones, 2010; Crone and Dahl, 2012; Spear, 2000), the abundance of social play behavior suggests that play is related to the maturation of the PFC and striatum.

Using c-fos as an activity marker, enhanced neuronal activity has been observed after social play behavior in several medial PFC regions, ie, anterior cingulate, prelimbic cortex (PrL) and infralimbic cortex (IL), as well as in orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) regions (Cheng et al, 2008; Gordon et al, 2002; Van Kerkhof et al, 2013). In keeping with these findings, the structure of social play and its sensitivity to social cues was found to be altered after neonatal lesions of the PFC or OFC (Bell et al, 2009; Panksepp et al, 1994; Pellis et al, 2006; Schneider and Koch, 2005).

With regard to the striatum, studies in nonhuman primates found that a larger size of the striatum was associated with more time spent on social play behavior, while there was no correlation with the level of non-social play behavior (Graham, 2011). Recent studies have also indicated a role for the ventral parts of the striatum, ie, nucleus accumbens (NAcc), in the modulation of social play behavior by opioids and endocannabinoids (Trezza et al, 2011, 2012). Furthermore, neonatal dopamine depletion in the striatum caused rats to use less complex defence strategies during a playful encounter and to be more likely to switch to other types of behavior (Pellis et al, 1993). This indicates that striatal dopamine contributes to the maintenance of the sequential organization of play fighting. Interestingly, this somewhat resembles the effect of neonatal lesions of the medial PFC (Bell et al, 2009), suggesting involvement of projections from medial PFC to striatum in social play behavior. Further evidence for an involvement of corticostriatal projections is provided by immediate early gene expression studies, demonstrating that social play behavior induces c-fos expression in the striatum (Cheng et al, 2008; Gordon et al, 2002; Van Kerkhof et al, 2013). Interestingly, the levels of c-fos induced in medial PFC regions were found to correlate with the levels of c-fos in their striatal projection areas, suggesting that these projections are involved in social play behavior (Van Kerkhof et al, 2013).

On the basis of these studies, it is plausible that the PFC and striatum play important roles in the regulation of social play behavior. However, most of these studies used either indirect approaches (Cheng et al, 2008; Gordon et al, 2002; Van Kerkhof et al, 2013) or neonatal lesions (Bell et al, 2009; Panksepp et al, 1994; Pellis et al, 1993, 2006; Schneider and Koch, 2005), which may disrupt normal brain development and/or lead to compensatory changes in non-affected structures. Therefore, here we investigated the role of PFC and striatum subregions in social play behavior using a temporary pharmacological inactivation technique, which allows for transient disruption of functional activity but leaves normal brain development and function intact (Martin, 1991; McFarland and Kalivas, 2001; Van Duuren et al, 2007).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Male Wistar rats (Charles River, Sulzfeld, Germany) arrived in our animal facility at 21 days of age. They were housed in groups of four in 40 × 26 × 20 cm3 Macrolon cages under controlled conditions (ie, temperature 20–21 °C, 55–65% relative humidity and 12/12 h light cycle with lights on at 0700 hours). Food and water were available ad libitum. All animals were experimentally naive. Before the start of the experiment, rats were handled at least twice. All experiments were approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of Utrecht University and were conducted in accordance with Dutch laws (Wet op de Dierproeven, 1996) and European regulations (Guideline 86/609/EEC).

Surgical Procedures

The surgical procedures were based on previous experiments (Trezza et al, 2011, 2012). At 27–28 days of age, rats were anesthetized with 0.08 ml/100 g (s.c.) of Hypnorm (Janssen, Belgium) and positioned in a stereotaxic apparatus (David Kopf, USA). Guide cannulae (24 gauge; Cooper’s Needleworks, UK) were implanted bilaterally. The cannulae were aimed 0.5 mm above the PrL (coordinates: anterior-posterior (AP) +2.6 mm from Bregma; medial-lateral (ML) ±0.8 mm from the midline; dorsal-ventral (DV) −3.2 mm from skull surface), the IL (coordinates: AP +2.6 mm; ML ±0.8 mm; DV −4.1 mm), the medial/ventral OFC (MO/VO; coordinates: AP +3.3 mm; ML ±0.8 mm; DV −5.3 mm), the dorsomedial striatum (DMS; coordinates: AP +1.5 mm; ML ±1.9 mm; DV −5.1 mm), the NAcc core (coordinates: AP, +1.5 mm; ML ±1.9 mm; DV −6.5 mm), or 1.0 mm above the NAcc shell (coordinates: AP +1.5 mm; ML ±0.8 mm; DV −5.3 mm). Coordinates were based on previous experiments (Trezza et al, 2011, 2012) or determined by pilot placements in 4-week-old rats.

Cannulae were secured with stainless steel screws and dental acrylic. Stainless steel stylets (29 gauge) were inserted into the guide cannulae to maintain patency. After surgery, rats were individually housed for 4 days to recover, after which they were housed with their original cage mates.

Drugs and Infusion Procedures

The GABA-A receptor agonist muscimol and the GABA-B receptor agonist (RS)-baclofen (Tocris Bioscience, UK) were dissolved in saline. In all regions 1.0 nmol/0.3 μl baclofen and 0.1 nmol/0.3 μl muscimol (B&M) was administered, with the exception of the NAcc shell into which 0.1 nmol/0.3 μl baclofen and 0.01 nmol/0.3 μl muscimol was infused. Lower doses of B&M were infused into the NAcc shell because the 1.0 nmol baclofen/0.1 nmol muscimol dose caused intense, non-specific hyperphagia (see eg, Stratford and Kelley, 1997), which resulted in a complete blockade of social play. The AMPA/kainate receptor antagonist, 6,7-dinitroquinoxaline-2,3(1H,4H)-dione (DNQX) (Sigma, USA), was dissolved in 50% DMSO in saline, and a dose of 3.0 nmol/0.3 μl was administered. Infusion procedures were as previously described (Trezza et al, 2011, 2012). In short, bilateral infusions of drugs or equivalent volume of saline were administered using 30-gauge injection needles (Bilaney, Germany) connected to 10 μl Hamilton micro-syringes by polyethylene tubing. Over 60 s, 0.3 μl of the solution was infused using a syringe pump (Harvard Apparatus, USA). The injection needles remained within the guide cannulae for 60 s following drug infusion to facilitate diffusion and to prevent backflow of drug along the cannula track. After the infusion, the stylets were replaced.

Behavioral Testing

Experiments were performed in a sound-attenuated chamber under dim light conditions. The testing arena was a Plexiglas cage (40 × 40 × 60 cm3), with approximately 2 cm of wood shavings covering the floor. Animals were paired with an unfamiliar partner (ie, not a cage mate). Animals in a test pair did not differ >10 g in body weight.

One week after surgery, the rats were habituated to the experimental procedures on 2 consecutive days. On the first day, they were individually placed in the test cage for 10 min. On the second day, they were isolated for 2.5 h. Pairs of rats were then infused with saline and placed together in the test cage for 15 min to habituate them to the infusion and testing procedures. On the test day, rats were again isolated for 2.5 h. Both rats in a pair were then simultaneously infused, placed into separate holding cages for 5 min, and subsequently placed together into the test cage for 15 min.

Behavior of the animals was recorded on video tape and subsequently assessed using the Observer software (Noldus Information Technology, The Netherlands) by an observer blind to treatment. The structure of social play behavior in rats has been previously described in detail (Baenninger, 1967; Bolles and Woods, 1964; Panksepp and Beatty, 1980; Pellis and Pellis, 1987; Pellis et al, 1989; Poole and Fish, 1975; for reviews, see Panksepp et al, 1984; Pellis and Pellis, 1998; Trezza et al, 2010; Vanderschuren et al, 1997). In rats, a bout of social play behavior starts with one rat soliciting (‘pouncing’) another animal, by attempting to nose or rub the nape of its neck. The animal that is pounced upon can respond in different ways. If the animal that is pounced upon responds by evading, the soliciting rat may start to chase it, thus making another attempt to launch a play bout. The solicited animal may also rear towards the soliciting animal and the two animals may rapidly push, paw, and grab each other (‘boxing’). If the animal that is pounced upon fully rotates to its dorsal surface, ‘pinning’ is the result, ie, one animal lying with its dorsal surface on the floor with the other animal standing over it. From this position, the supine animal can initiate another play bout, by trying to gain access to the other animal’s neck. Thus, during social play, pouncing is considered an index of play solicitation, while pinning functions as a releaser of a prolonged play bout (Panksepp and Beatty, 1980; Pellis and Pellis, 1987; Pellis et al, 1989; Poole and Fish, 1975). Pinning and pouncing frequencies can be easily quantified and are considered the most characteristic parameters of social play behavior in rats (Panksepp and Beatty, 1980). During the social encounter, animals may also display social behaviors not directly associated with play, such as sniffing or grooming the partner’s body (Panksepp and Beatty, 1980; Vanderschuren et al, 1995). As social play behavior in rats strongly depends on the playfulness of its partner (Pellis and McKenna, 1992; Trezza and Vanderschuren, 2008), in the present study, both animals in a play pair were similarly treated and a pair of animals was considered as one experimental unit. The following parameters were therefore scored per pair of animals:

Social behaviors related to play:

  • Frequency of pinning

  • Frequency of pouncing

  • Total play duration: the total amount of time spent in playful social interactions (ie, pinning, pouncing, boxing, or chasing the play partner).

Social behaviors unrelated to play:

  • Time spent in social exploration: the total amount of time spent in non-playful forms of social interaction (ie, one animal sniffing or grooming any part of the partner’s body).

Pinning, pouncing, and other playful behaviors usually occur very rapidly and they are of short duration. Therefore, scoring their individual frequency is more informative than scoring their duration. Moreover, we have also found that pinning and pouncing are very reliable play parameters that occur consistently and with considerable frequency during playful encounters (see also Panksepp and Beatty, 1980; Vanderschuren et al, 1995), whereas the occurrence of chasing and boxing can be quite variable. In order to evaluate the amount of time spent in playful social behaviors (eg, pinning, pouncing) vs social behaviors unrelated to play (eg, social exploration: sniffing, social grooming), we included the total play duration as a parameter as well. Changes in boxing and chasing provided only minor contributions to the effects on total play duration observed (data not shown).

To assess whether effects of the drug treatment on social play were secondary to changes in locomotor activity, rats (at 41 days of age) were tested for horizontal locomotor activity as described previously (Trezza et al, 2009; Veeneman et al, 2011). Between the test for social play behavior and locomotor activity, there was at least one day without testing or infusions. The infusion protocol was as described above. After infusion, the rats were transferred to a plastic cage (l × w × h, 50 × 33 × 40 cm3), and their position was tracked five times per second for 30 min using a video-tracking system (EthoVision, Noldus Information Technology, The Netherlands).

Histological Confirmation of Injection Sites

Confirmation of the injection sites was performed as previously described (Trezza et al, 2011, 2012). Only pairs of rats in which both animals had bilateral needle tracks terminating in the target area were included in the final analysis (Figure 1).

Figure 1
figure 1

Schematic representation of brain sections with microinjection placements in the prelimbic cortex (a), infralimbic cortex (b), medial/ventral orbitofrontal cortex (c), dorsomedial striatum (d), nucleus accumbens core (e), and nucleus accumbens shell (f). AP, distance anterior to Bregma (in mm). Adapted from Paxinos and Watson (2007).

PowerPoint slide

Statistical Analysis

Pinning and pouncing frequencies, total play duration (s), and time spent in social exploration (s) were expressed as mean±SEM, and data were analyzed using a paired samples Student’s t-test. Horizontal locomotor activity was expressed as mean±SEM of the travelled distance (cm) and analyzed using a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA.

RESULTS

Pharmacological Inactivation of Prefrontal Regions

Infusion of B&M into the PrL decreased social play behavior (Figure 2). The frequencies of pinning (Figure 2a) and pouncing (Figure 2b) and total play duration (Figure 2c) were reduced (pinning: t=3.145, df=8, p=0.014; pouncing: t=3.509, df=8, p=0.008; total play: t=2.734, df=8, p=0.026). The duration of social exploration was increased (t=−2.322, df=8, p=0.049; Figure 2d), whereas no change was observed in locomotor activity (Ftreatment (1,23)=0.801, p=0.380; Ftime(5,115)=66.445, p<0.001; Ftreatment × time (5,115)=0.177, p=0.971; Figure 2e).

Figure 2
figure 2

The effect of B&M administration (1.0 nmol/0.3 μl and 0.1 nmol/0.3 μl) into prefrontal regions on social play behavior. Data are presented as mean±SEM. B&M administration into the prelimbic cortex (n=9) reduced the levels of pinning (a) and pouncing (b), as well as the total time spent on play (c). B&M administration increased the time spent on social exploration (d), whereas it had no effect on locomotor activity (e). Infusion of B&M into the infralimbic cortex (n=8) reduced the levels of pinning (f), pouncing (g), and the total time spent on play (h), whereas no effects were observed on social exploration (i) or locomotor activity (j). After administration of B&M into the medial/ventral orbitofrontal cortex (n=6), a reduction in pinning (k), pouncing (l), and the total time spent on play was observed (m), whereas no effect on social exploration (n) or locomotor activity (o) was found. *p<0.05, **p<0.01 compared with saline (SAL; paired samples Student’s t-test).

PowerPoint slide

After administration of B&M into the IL, a decrease in pinning, pouncing, and total play duration was observed (pinning: t=4.616, df=7, p=0.002; pouncing: t=5.285, df=7, p=0.001; total play duration: t=4.420, df=7, p=0.003; Figure 2f–h). Comparable with the PrL, infusion of B&M into the IL tended to increase social exploration (t=−2.105, df=7, p=0.073; Figure 2i) but did not alter locomotor activity (Ftreatment (1,18)=0.817, p=0.378; Ftime(5,90)=57.835, p<0.001; Ftreatment × time (5,90)=0.223, p=0.952; Figure 2j).

Administration of B&M into the MO/VO resulted in a decrease in pinning, pouncing, and total play duration (pinning: t=4.408, df=5, p=0.007; pouncing: t=4.146, df=5, p=0.009; total play duration: t=4.190, df=5, p=0.009; Figure 2k–m), whereas no effects were observed on social exploration (Figure 2n) or locomotor activity (Figure 2o; social exploration: t=−0.384, df=5, p=0.717; locomotor activity: Ftreatment (1,15)=0.160, p=0.695; Ftime(5,75)=36.445, p<0.001; Ftreatment × time (5,75)=0.354, p=0.878).

The effect size of the changes in pinning, pouncing, and total play duration (ie, percentage of decrease compared with saline) were comparable in the three regions (pinning: PrL: −41.14±10.45; IL: −57.47±12.01; MO/VO: −50.80±8.65; pouncing: PrL: −32.51±8.46; IL: −55.29±8.23; MO/VO: −40.33±8.26; total play duration: PrL: −32.72±10.14; IL: −49.91±13.56; MO/VO: −43.46±8.73). No difference was observed when the reduction in pinning, pouncing, or total play duration was compared between the three regions (pinning: Fregion (2,20)=0.633, p=0.541; pouncing: Fregion (2,20)=0.2.338, p=0.122; total play duration (Fregion (2,20)=0.621, p=0.548).

Pharmacological Inactivation of Striatal Regions

Administration of B&M into the DMS did not affect pinning (t=−1.726, df=12, p=0.110; Figure 3a), but a trend towards an increase in the frequency of pouncing was observed (t=−2.168, df=12, p=0.051; Figure 3b). Treatment with B&M also tended to decrease social exploration (t=2.126, df=12, p=0.055; Figure 3d), but it did not affect total play duration (t=−1.214, df=12, p=0.248; Figure 3c) or locomotor activity (Ftreatment (1,17)=0.512, p=0.484; Ftime(5,85)=24.767, p<0.001; Ftreatment × time (5,85)=0.684, p=0.637; Figure 3e).

Figure 3
figure 3

The effect of B&M administration into striatal regions on social play behavior. Data are presented as mean±SEM. B&M was administered into the dorsomedial striatum (a–e; n=13) and nucleus accumbens core (f–j; n=10) in a dose of 1.0 nmol/0.3 μl and 0.1 nmol/0.3 μl and into the nucleus accumbens shell (k–p; n=5)) in a dose of 0.1 nmol/0.3 μl and 0.01 nmol/0.3 μl. Administration of B&M did not affect pinning (a,f,k), pouncing (b,g,l), social exploration (d,i,n), and locomotor activity (e,j,po), although in the dorsomedial striatum a trend towards an increase in pouncing (p=0.051; b) and a trend towards a reduction in the time spent on social exploration (p=0.055; d) was found. The total time spent on social play behavior was increased after B&M administration into the nucleus accumbens core (h), but not after administration into the dorsomedial striatum (c) or nucleus accumbens shell (m). *p<0.05 compared with vehicle (VEH; paired samples Student's t-test).

PowerPoint slide

Although infusion of B&M into the NAcc core did not influence pinning (t=−1.138, df=10, p=0.281) or pouncing frequencies (t=−0.278, df=10, p=0.787) (Figure 3f–g), it did increase the total time spent on social play behavior (t=−2.733, df=10, p=0.021; Figure 3h), suggesting that the duration of play bouts was enhanced. Administration of B&M into the NAcc core had no effect on social exploration (t=−0.916, df=10, p=0.381; Figure 3i) or locomotor activity (Ftreatment (1,13)=0.462, p=0.509; Ftime(5,65)=47.795, p<0.001; Ftreatment × time (5,65)=0.220, p=0.953; Figure 3j).

Administration of B&M at the dose used in the other regions (1.0 nmol baclofen and 0.1 nmol muscimol/0.3 μl) into the NAcc shell resulted in a complete blockade of play (data not shown), which was secondary to striking effects on ingestive behavior. Animals would spend all their time on non-specific eating (eating eg, sawdust and faeces; see Stratford and Kelley, 1997). Therefore, a lower dose of B&M (0.1 nmol baclofen and 0.01 nmol muscimol/0.3 μl), which had no general disruptive effects on behavior was administered into the NAcc shell. This dose had no effect on any of the parameters measured (pinning: t=0.353, df=4, p=0.742; pouncing: t=0.105, df=4, p=0.922; total play duration: t=−0.263, df=4, p=0.806; social exploration: t=−0.683, df=4, p=0.532; locomotor activity: Ftreatment (1,13)=0.113, p=0.742; Ftime(5,65)=31.444, p<0.001; Ftreatment × time (5,65)=2.342, p=0.051; Figure 3k–o).

Involvement of Glutamatergic Input to the Striatum

In view of the effects of B&M in the prefrontal regions and the striatum (with an effect in the NAcc core and a trend in the DMS), and the glutamatergic projections from the PFC to the striatum (Groenewegen and Uylings, 2010; Voorn et al, 2004), the role of glutamatergic input into the striatum in social play was investigated using intra-striatal administration of the AMPA receptor antagonist DNQX. Administration of DNQX into the DMS specifically influenced social play behavior, as it increased pinning (t=−2.552, df=8, p=0.034; Figure 4a) and pouncing (t=1.403, df=8, p=0.032; Figure 4b) frequencies, and total play duration (t=−4.035, df=8, p=0.004; Figure 4c), without affecting social exploration (t=1.403, df=8, p=0.198; Figure 4d) or locomotor activity (Ftreatment (1,18)=0.059, p=0.811; Ftreatment × time (5,90)=1.395, p=0.234; Figure 4e).

Figure 4
figure 4

The effect of DNQX administration (3.0 nmol/0.3 μl) into the dorsomedial striatum and nucleus accumbens core on social play behavior. Data are presented as mean±SEM. DNQX administration into the dorsomedial striatum (n=9) increased the frequency of pinning (a), pouncing (b), and the total time spent on play behavior (c), whereas it did not affect the time spent on social exploration (d) or locomotor activity (e). Infusion of B&M into the nucleus accumbens core (n=8) did not change the level of pinning (f), pouncing (g), total play duration (h), social exploration (i), or locomotor activity (j). *p<0.05, **p<0.01 compared with vehicle (VEH; paired samples Student’s t-test).

PowerPoint slide

Infusion of DNQX into the NAcc core did not affect any of the parameters measured (pinning: t=−1.769, df=7 p=0.120; pouncing: t=−1.254, df=7, p=0.250; total play duration: t=−0.959, df=7, p=0.370; social exploration: t=1.382, df=7, p=0.210; locomotor activity: Ftreatment (1,17)=0.877, p=0.362; Ftreatment × time (5,85)=0.182, p=0.969; Figure 4f–j). DNQX was not administered into the NAcc shell, as this is known to enhance feeding behavior in a comparable fashion as administration of B&M into this region (Maldonado-Irizarry et al, 1995).

DISCUSSION

Importance of the PFC for Social Play Behavior

Temporary inactivation of the medial PFC regions reduced the frequency of the most characteristic play parameters in rats, ie, pinning and pouncing, as well as the total time spent on social play behavior, without altering locomotor activity. Inactivation of the PrL, and to a lesser extent the IL, but not the MO/VO, increased the time the animals spent on social behaviors unrelated to play, such as social grooming or sniffing. Combined, the reductions in pouncing (ie, play initiation), pinning (a response to pouncing) as well as total time spent playing, indicates that temporary inactivation of medial PFC regions caused a decrease in social playfulness and that this is not the result of a general decrement in social interest or an overall disruption of behavior. Interestingly, the increase in social exploration after inactivation of the PrL and IL (even though the effect in the IL did not reach statistical significance) suggests that the initiation of social behavior was intact but that this was less often followed by the launch of a playful interaction, which indirectly resulted in an increase in social exploration. Together, these data therefore suggest that functional activity of the medial PFC serves to facilitate playful initiatives.

Altered responses to play initiations have previously been reported after neonatal PFC lesions (Bell et al, 2009; Panksepp et al, 1994; Pellis et al, 2006; Schneider and Koch, 2005). Lesions of the lateral and ventral OFC have been found to disrupt the partner-related modulation of play (Pellis et al, 2006). Thus, control but not lesioned rats respond differently to dominant, subordinate, and female rats. Animals with medial PFC lesions still show partner-related changes in social play behavior, but they use less complex defensive strategies (Bell et al, 2009). These, and other, studies indicate that after neonatal PFC lesions, play behavior is still observed but that its structure and sensitivity to social cues is altered (Bell et al, 2009; Panksepp et al, 1994; Pellis et al, 2006; Schneider and Koch, 2005). In the present study, however, the effect of temporary PFC inactivation was more substantial and, in contrast to the neonatal lesion studies, play initiations (ie, pouncing) were markedly reduced.

The methodological approach of the aforementioned lesion studies differs in several respects from the present study. Relatively large lesions were made at an early age (postnatal days 3–7), which may have impaired brain development and damaged fibres of passage. Conversely, after neonatal lesions, other regions may compensate for the loss of function. By contrast, local administration of GABA receptor agonists, as used here, temporarily reduces regional output without affecting brain development and connectivity (Martin, 1991; McFarland and Kalivas, 2001; van Duuren et al, 2007). Hence, our data show that disruption of medial PFC signalling markedly interferes with the expression of social play behavior, indicating that functional integrity of the medial PFC is required for social play behavior.

In the present study, comparable effects of inactivation were observed in the three PFC regions tested. Muscimol administration into a certain cortical region may influence neuronal activity in adjacent regions through local cortico-cortical circuits (Martin, 1991). However, if in the present study the effects of B&M in some regions were mediated via cortico-cortical interactions, the behavioral effects would have to be distance-dependent. As no difference was observed in the effect size between the PFC regions, we conclude that each of the three PFC regions has an important role in social play behavior.

The PFC and OFC have been widely implicated in executive functions, such as attention, planning, cognitive flexibility, and decision making (Dalley et al, 2004; Wallis, 2007). Given the complex and unpredictable nature of social interactions, it is likely that frontal cortical regions subserve executive functions in social situations as well (Adolphs, 2003). Inactivation of the PrL, IL, and MO/VO may, therefore, disrupt social play behavior by interfering with cognitive flexibility and decision-making processes necessary for a proper social interaction. Studies in the past decade have found considerable anatomical (Groenewegen and Uylings, 2010; Hoover and Vertes, 2011; Vertes, 2004) and functional heterogeneity within the PFC and OFC (Chudasama et al, 2003; Gourley et al, 2010; Killcross and Coutureau, 2003; Mar et al, 2011; Peters et al, 2009). This begs the question of the exact contribution of PrL, IL, and MO/VO in social play behavior, given that their inactivation produces comparable reductions in social play. Interestingly, cellular activation studies have found similar patterns of activity of PrL and IL neurons during social behavior in late adolescent rats (Wall et al, 2012) and during sexual behavior in adult rats (Balfour et al, 2006). Consistent with this, we have recently found comparable increases in c-fos expression in PrL, IL, MO, and VO after social play, although the magnitude of the effect differed somewhat between these regions (Van Kerkhof et al, 2013). These studies, together with the present data, suggest that these prefrontal subregions have comparable roles in social interactions, even though their contribution to non-social cognitive functions is clearly distinct. How these dissociable cognitive functions of PFC subregions map onto comparable contributions to social interaction is an important topic for future studies.

Inactivation of the NAcc Core Enhances the Duration of Social Play Behavior

Previous findings have implicated the NAcc in social play behavior (Gordon et al, 2002; Trezza et al, 2011, 2012; Van Kerkhof et al, 2013). Thus, after social play behavior, c-fos activity was enhanced in the NAcc core and shell (Gordon et al, 2002; Van Kerkhof et al, 2013). In addition, the play-enhancing effects of mu-opioid receptor agonists and cannabinoid agonists were found to be exerted within the NAcc (Trezza et al, 2011, 2012). In view of the involvement of the NAcc in hedonics and incentive motivation (Berridge and Kringelbach, 2008; Kelley, 2004; Salamone and Correa, 2012), these findings suggest a role for the NAcc in the positive emotional properties of social play behavior. Indeed, blockade of NAcc mu-opioid receptors prevented the development of social play-induced conditioned place preference (Trezza et al, 2011).

Administration of B&M (1.0 nmol/0.3 μl and 0.1 nmol/0.3 μl) into the NAcc shell resulted in a complete blockade of play. This effect was secondary to intense hyperphagia, leaving no opportunity for the animals to engage in social interaction (see Stratford and Kelley, 1997). A lower dose of B&M, which did not induce eating, did not alter social play. As the effects of NAcc shell infusions of GABA receptor agonists and AMPA receptor antagonists on ingestive behavior are highly comparable (Maldonado-Irizarry et al, 1995; Stratford and Kelley, 1997), the effect of intra-shell DNQX infusion on social play was therefore not investigated. Thus, understanding the involvement of NAcc shell function in social play behavior remains difficult.

Administration of B&M into the NAcc core enhanced the time spent on social play behavior, but not the frequency of any of the play parameters, indicating that after NAcc core inactivation, the length of a play bout is enhanced. This effect was not observed after administration of DNQX into the NAcc core, which suggests that blocking only glutamatergic inputs into the NAcc core is not sufficient to alter play duration. The facilitatory effect of intra-NAcc core B&M on social play resonates well with the notion of reduced NAcc neuronal activity during reward-related behavior (Carlezon and Thomas, 2009). Consistent with this idea, it has been shown that firing in a subset of NAcc neurons is suppressed to support the maintenance of appetitive behavior (Taha and Fields, 2006; Krause et al, 2010). Infusion of B&M into the NAcc core may inhibit these neurons, permitting the prolongation of social play. Consistent with this, lesions of the NAcc core have been shown to enhance the relative rewarding value of social behavior compared with cocaine, as studied using concurrent social behavior vs cocaine-induced conditioned place preference (Fritz et al, 2011).

Glutamatergic DMS Inputs Inhibit Social Play Behavior

Administration of DNQX into the DMS selectively increased social play behavior, as it enhanced pinning, pouncing, and total play duration, without affecting social exploration or locomotor activity. This pattern of effects, ie, increases in play initiation, responding to initiation as well as total time spent playing, suggests that blockade of AMPA/kainate receptors in the DMS specifically enhanced playful social behavior. Thus, glutamatergic input into the DMS exerts an inhibitory influence on social play behavior. Remarkably, infusion of B&M into the DMS only tended to increase pouncing. It is possible that inactivation of the DMS with B&M affects opposing processes resulting in the absence of a clear effect as observed after AMPA/kainate receptor blockade. Stated otherwise, an optimal balance of different inputs into the DMS may be necessary for proper expression of social play behavior, whereby B&M infusion affects these inputs, as well as the activity of striatal output cells, in a comparable fashion, which results in a smaller net effect of inactivation.

We previously observed a correlation between social play-induced c-fos activity in the DMS and its inputs from the PrL (Van Kerkhof et al, 2013), suggesting that glutamatergic inputs from the PrL to the DMS are involved in social play behavior. However, inhibition of PrL output reduced social play behavior, whereas blocking glutamatergic inputs into the DMS increased it. Thus, involvement of a glutamatergic PrL-to-DMS projection in social play does not appear likely. However, as inactivation of the PrL will inhibit all its outputs, the specific involvement of the PrL-DMS projection may be overshadowed by the effect of PrL B&M on other PrL outputs. In addition, it is possible that other glutamatergic inputs are responsible for the effects of DMS manipulation, such as those from the amygdala and thalamus. Indeed, these regions have been implicated in social play behavior (Siviy and Panksepp, 2011; Trezza et al, 2010, 2012,).

Previous lesion and inactivation studies have shown involvement of the DMS in response selection (Devan et al, 1999; Corbit and Janak, 2007) and response inhibition (Eagle and Robbins, 2003). Thus, animals lacking a functional DMS display various forms of disinhibited behavior. Of particular interest are the findings that a DMS lesion impairs (Eagle and Robbins, 2003), whereas treatment with methylphenidate improve stop signal reaction time task performance (Eagle et al, 2007). These findings echo the effects of methylphenidate (Vanderschuren et al, 2008), which reduces social play, and intra-DMS DNQX, which enhances it (present study). We do not think that DMS inactivation causes an overall disinhibition of behavior. In the present study, the effects of intra-DMS DNQX on social play were behaviorally specific, as no changes were observed in social exploration and locomotor activity. Moreover, animals with DMS lesions show no change in responding for food under a progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement (Eagle et al, 1999), and operant responding remains sensitive to presentation of reward-associated stimuli after DMS inactivation (although the specificity of responding is impaired; Corbit and Janak, 2007). Together, these data suggest that DMS mechanisms exert inhibitory control over vigorous forms of behavior, such as social play, but that inactivation or lesion of the DMS leaves the structure of behavior and its appropriateness to the situation intact (Whishaw et al, 2007).

Concluding Remarks

The present study indicates a more direct involvement of the medial PFC and OFC in social play behavior than previously thought. Besides facilitating the performance of appropriate responses to play initiations from the partner, as previously reported (Bell et al, 2009; Pellis et al, 2006), functional integrity of the medial PFC/OFC is important for the animal to initiate a playful interaction itself. Furthermore, glutamatergic inputs into the DMS exert inhibitory control over social play behavior, whereas functional inhibition of the NAcc core prolongs playful interactions. These results provide important clues to understand how the PFC and striatum are involved in social play behavior.