INTRODUCTION

It is thought that ‘The development of addiction depends on the interaction of agent, host and environment…’ (O'Brien, 2008). Much research has shown that ‘host’- and environment-related factors can modulate the transition from controlled drug use to addiction (Badiani and Robinson, 2004; Le Moal, 2009). Much less attention has been paid to characteristics of the ‘agent’ and how these might accelerate or delay the progression to addiction. One such characteristic is the speed at which a drug reaches its target sites in the brain. Addiction to cocaine, amphetamine, methamphetamine, nicotine, or heroin is more likely and more severe in individuals who smoke or inject these drugs than in individuals who use slower routes of administration (eg, intranasal, oral, or transdermal administration). Individuals who smoke or inject drugs use drugs more frequently, for a longer time, spend more money on drugs, report a greater loss of control over drug taking, and are more likely to overdose (Barrio et al, 2001; Carpenter et al, 1998; Ferri and Gossop, 1999; Gossop et al, 1992; Hatsukami and Fischman, 1996; Hughes, 1989; Rawson et al, 2007; Winger et al, 1992).

Rapidly administered drugs are hypothesized to be more addictive, because they are more pleasurable such that the likelihood of continuing or escalating drug use is increased (de Wit et al, 1993; Hatsukami and Fischman, 1996). Indeed, rapid delivery enhances the positive subjective effects of drugs (Abreu et al, 2001; de Wit et al, 1993; Kollins et al, 1998), and this could contribute to drug use. A related hypothesis is that rapidly administered drugs are more addictive, because they are more reinforcing (Balster and Schuster, 1973; Kato et al, 1987; Panlilio et al, 1998). The idea is that because drug effects are more immediate with rapid drug delivery, associations formed between drug-taking behaviors and ensuing drug effects are stronger and the likelihood of repeating these behaviors is increased.

However, animal studies on reinforcing efficacy have produced inconsistent findings. Some show that rapid intravenous delivery enhances the acute reinforcing effects of drugs (Balster and Schuster, 1973; Kato et al, 1987; Panlilio et al, 1998; Schindler et al, 2011; Wakasa et al, 1995; Woolverton and Wang, 2004), and others show that it does not (Crombag et al, 2008; Liu et al, 2005; Sorge and Clarke, 2009). The reasons for the discrepancy are not clear. However, the procedures often used (limiting drug access to 1–3 h/session; (Crombag et al, 2008; Panlilio et al, 1998; Pickens et al, 1969; Schindler et al, 2011; Sorge and Clarke, 2009)) are known to promote stable patterns of drug intake, rather than the progressive and deteriorating patterns of drug-taking behavior that define addiction (Ahmed, 2012; Roberts et al, 2007). Recently, the influence of the speed of drug delivery on drug reinforcement has been assessed using extended self-administration (SA) sessions (Wakabayashi et al, 2010), where animals self-administer during 6 h/session, and are subsequently more likely to develop patterns of drug taking relevant to addiction (Ahmed, 2012). This work has shown that under limited cocaine access (1 h/session), the speed of intravenous cocaine delivery (5, 45 or 90 s) does not influence the reinforcing properties of the drug (Wakabayashi et al, 2010). When access to cocaine is extended to 6 h, however, faster cocaine injections lead to greater drug intake and a more persistent vulnerability to drug-primed reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior (Wakabayashi et al, 2010).

It has been proposed that regardless of whether rapid delivery might or might not make drugs more reinforcing, rapid delivery increases the potential for addiction because it facilitates the brain changes that promote excessive motivation for drugs (Liu et al, 2005; Samaha et al, 2002; Samaha and Robinson, 2005; Wakabayashi et al, 2010). Repeated drug use can trigger the development of pathological motivation for drug, leading to a state where drugs become progressively harder to resist, and are compulsively pursued in the face of rising costs (Kalivas and Volkow, 2005; Robinson and Berridge, 1993). This progressive increase in the time and effort spent to obtain drugs is a hallmark of drug addiction (DSM-IV). This important aspect of the addiction process was not addressed in Wakabayashi et al (2010). It has been shown previously that breakpoints maintained by cocaine under a progressive ratio (PR) schedule more readily sensitize over time with rapid drug delivery (Liu et al, 2005). However, animals were tested following particularly limited cocaine exposure, and it is increasingly recognized that animal models of addiction must include extended periods of drug taking (Ahmed and Koob, 1998; Belin et al, 2009; Deroche-Gamonet et al, 2004; Vanderschuren and Everitt, 2004).

Here we determined how a history of extended access to rapidly delivered cocaine alters the motivation for the drug. To begin to examine potential neurobiological mechanisms, we quantified the effects of the speed of cocaine delivery on D2 receptor number and function. Reduced striatal D2 receptor availability (Martinez et al, 2004; Volkow et al, 1990) and increased D2 receptor function (Self et al, 1996; Ujike et al, 1990) have each been implicated in the susceptibility to cocaine addiction. Work with nicotine also shows that D2 receptors differentially regulate the SA of rapid vs slower drug injections (Sorge and Clarke, 2009).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

One hundred and ten male Wistar rats (225–250 g; Charles River Laboratories, St Constant, QC) were housed individually in a climate-controlled colony room maintained on a reverse 12 h/12 h light/dark cycle (lights off at 0800 hours) Experiments were conducted during the dark phase of the rats’ circadian cycle. Food and water were available ad libitum unless noted otherwise. The animal care committee of the Université de Montréal approved all procedures. Sixty-eight animals completed the study. Attrition was due to loss of catheter patency (21 rats), failure to meet criteria for stable cocaine SA behavior (9 rats), ill heath (5 rats), death during surgery (3 rats), cocaine overdose due to experimental error (3 rats), and exclusion due to abnormally aggressive behavior (1 rat).

Drugs

Cocaine hydrochloride (Medisca Pharmaceutique, St Laurent, QC) was dissolved in saline and filtered with 25-mm Acrodisc syringe filters (0.2 μm GHP membrane; VWR, Town of Mount-Royal, QC).

Apparatus

Rats were trained and tested in standard operant chambers (Med Associates, St Albans, VT), as described in Samaha et al (2011). Operant chambers were located in a testing room separate from the animals’ housing room. 3.33-RPM syringe pump motors delivered cocaine (0.5 or 0.25 mg/kg/infusion) over 5 s at a rate of 29.23 μl/s, and 0.1-RPM motors delivered 90-s infusions (0.85 μl/s). A computer running Med Associates Med-PC version IV software controlled the chambers and collected data.

Operant Food Training

Figure 1 illustrates the experimental events. Rats were trained to lever-press for 45-mg, banana-flavored pellets (grain-based; Cedarlane Laboratories, Burlington, ON) on a continuous reinforcement schedule (fixed ratio 1; FR1). Sessions began with illumination of the house light and insertion of the left lever into the cage (right levers were non-retractable). Each press on the left lever produced one pellet. Sessions ended following 100 pellets or 30 min. Following two sessions, animals that had not received 10 pellets/session were retrained overnight (16 rats). During the food-training phase, animals received two pellets of standard rat chow after testing. Thereafter, rats received 5–7 standard food pellets/day.

Figure 1
figure 1

Timeline of behavioral training and self-administration testing.

PowerPoint slide

Implantation of Jugular Catheters

Following operant food training, rats were assigned to one of three groups; a control group (to serve as control in the D2 assays), or groups that would be allowed to self-administer 0.25 or 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine. Catheters were constructed and implanted into the jugular vein of rats in the self-administration groups, as in Samaha et al (2011). Control rats underwent sham surgery (incisions and suturing). Thereafter, catheters were flushed on alternate days with either 0.1 ml physiological saline or a solution containing 0.2 mg/ml Heparin (Sigma-Aldrich, Oakville, ON) and 2 mg/ml Baytril (CDMV, St Hyacinthe, QC). Rats were allowed to recover for 4–10 days before testing. Catheter patency was verified on the day before PR testing by intravenous injection of 0.2 ml of a sodium thiopental solution (20 mg/ml in sterile water; Vétoquinol, Lavaltrie, QC). Rats that failed to become ataxic within 5 s were excluded from the data analysis.

Self-administration Training

Rats in the two self-administration groups could self-administer 0.25 or 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine injected over 5 s in daily, 1-h sessions under FR1. Sessions began with illumination of the house light and insertion of the left lever. Pressing on the left (active) lever activated the infusion pump and illuminated the cue light above the lever, signalling a 20-s timeout. Pressing on the right (inactive) lever had no programmed consequences.

Self-administration training parameters were adapted from Wakabayashi et al (2010) and consisted of FR1 with a 20-s timeout, then FR2 with the timeout increasing across sessions to 45, 65, and 85 s. We ultimately wished to compare rats self-administering cocaine delivered over 5 or 90 s. An 85-s timeout in the 5-s condition allowed all groups to take one injection every 90 s. Criteria to progress across training steps were 2 injections/session and twice as many active vs inactive lever presses, for two consecutive sessions.

Starting here in the experiment, control rats were transported to the experimental room and placed in Plexiglas cages for 1–6 h/session, for an average of 30 days. Thus, control animals were subjected to similar operant food training, surgical, handling, and transportation procedures, as well as being exposed to novel test cages for sessions that were comparable in number and duration to the self-administration sessions experienced in parallel by animals self-administering cocaine.

Self-administration During Short Access (ShA) Sessions

Following training, four groups were formed such that the mean number of infusions over the last two training days was equivalent across groups. For three 1-h daily sessions, the groups self-administered 0.25 or 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine, delivered either over 5 s with an 85-s timeout or over 90 s with no timeout, under FR2. Thus, the groups were: 0.25 mg–5 s, 0.25 mg–90 s, 0.5 mg–5 s, and 0.5 mg–90 s. In the 5-s groups, the cue light above the lever was illuminated during each 5-s infusion and remained illuminated during the 85-s timeout. In the 90-s condition, the cue light was illuminated during each 90-s infusion. As such, regardless of condition, each cue presentation lasted for 90 s.

Self-administration During Long Access (LgA) Sessions

Next, rats could self-administer cocaine as during the ShA phase, except that sessions now lasted 6 h, for a total of 12 sessions. Animals then remained in their home cages for 4–5 days before PR testing.

The Motivation for Cocaine

Self-administration under PR as a function of dose

Animals self-administered cocaine under PR, where response requirements increased according to the following formula (5 e(injection number × 0.2))−5, yielding the following ratio progression 1, 2, 4, 6, 9, 12, 15, 20, 25, 32, 40, 50, 62, etc. (Richardson and Roberts, 1996). For the 0.25-mg/kg groups, doses of 0.25, 0.125, and 0.063 mg/kg were tested (2 sessions/dose). For the 0.5-mg/kg groups, doses of 0.5, 0.25, and 0.125 mg/kg were tested. Doses were given in descending order. Cocaine was injected over 5 s for the 5-s groups and over 90 s for the 90-s groups. Sessions ended when 1 h elapsed since the last infusion or after 5 h.

Self-administration under PR as a function of speed

During the preceding PR sessions, any group differences in self-administration behavior could be due either to variations in the speed of cocaine delivery in the past (during ShA and LgA sessions) or variations in the speed of delivery during PR testing. To dissociate these two influences, all the groups were given additional PR sessions during which cocaine was delivered over 5 and 90 s (2–3 sessions/speed). These additional PR tests were given immediately following the preceding dose-response sessions.

D2 Receptor Number and Function

Fourteen days after the last PR session, 6–7 rats from the 0.25 mg-5 s and 0.25 mg-90 s groups and the 9 control rats were killed. All brains were extracted, plunged into isopentane (−50 °C), and stored at −80 °C until processing. Coronal brain sections (12 μm) were cut on a cryostat and mounted onto Snowcoat X-tra slides (Surgipath, Winnipeg, MA, Canada).

[125I]iodosulpiride binding

D2 receptor autoradiography was adapted from Maheux et al (2012). Brain sections were pre-incubated (15 min) in a 50 mM Tris-HCl buffer (pH 7.4) containing 120 mM NaCl, 5 mM KCl, 1 mM CaCl2, 1 mM MgCl2, 5.7 mM ascorbic acid, and 10 mM hydroxyquinoline. Slides were then incubated (1 h) in the same buffer containing 0.2 nM [125I]iodosulpride (specific activity 2200 Ci/mmol; PerkinElmer, Woodbridge, ON, Canada). Non-specific binding was evaluated in the presence of 2 μM raclopride. Slides were then washed (2 × 5 min) in ice-cold Tris-HCl buffer. Sections were dipped for 10 s in cold water, dried, and apposed to BioMaxMR film (Kodak, New Haven, CT). Autoradiograms were generated after 24 h of film exposure.

Quinpirole-stimulated [ 35 S]GTPγS binding

Agonist-stimulated [35 S]guanylyl-5′-O-(γ-thio)-triphosphate ([35 S]GTPγS)-binding autoradiography was adapted from Sim et al (1995) and Sovago et al (2005). Slides were pre-incubated (30 min) in buffer containing 50 mM HEPES (pH 7.4), 5 mM MgCl2, 0.2 mM EGTA, 150 mM NaCl, 75 mM KCl 1 mM DL-dithiothreitol, and 2 mM guanosine 5′-diphosphate (GDP; Sigma-Aldrich, St Louis, MO) at 37 °C. Basal and agonist-stimulated activities were determined by incubating the slides with 0.05 nM [35 S]GTPγS (specific activity, 1250 Ci/mmol; PerkinElmer) with or without the D2 agonist quinpirole (10 μM) in the same assay buffer at 37 °C for 1.5 h. In preliminary experiments, D2 receptor specificity of quinpirole-stimulated [35 S]GTPγS binding was examined by addition of raclopride (10 μM). Non-specific [35 S]GTPγS binding was assessed in the presence of 10 μM unlabeled GTPγS (Sigma-Aldrich, St Louis, MO). Slides were then rinsed (2 × 10 min) in ice-cold buffer (50 mM HEPES, pH 7.4 at 37 °C) and immersed in ice-cold distilled water. Slides were dried overnight and apposed to BioMaxMR film. Autoradiograms were generated after 36 h of film exposure.

Quantification

An experimenter blind to condition translated optical densities from autoradiograms into μCi/g of tissue using [14C] radioactivity standards (ARC 146A–14C; American Radiolabeled Chemicals, St Louis, MO) and ImageJ software (NIH, Bethesda, MD). Agonist-stimulated [35 S]GTPγS activity (net stimulation) was calculated for each rat by subtracting the optical density in basal brain sections (incubated with GDP alone) from that in quinpirole-stimulated sections. Results are expressed as percentage of change from net stimulation in controls. Optical densities were quantified in caudate-putamen quadrants (1.6, 1.2, 0.8, and 0.4 mm from Bregma, 1 section/level), the nucleus accumbens (2.0, 1.6, 1.2, and 0.8 mm), and prelimbic, infralimbic (both at 2.6 mm), and orbitofrontal (3.0 mm) cortices, according to Paxinos and Watson, (1986). For the [35 S]GTPγS binding assay, we also quantified optical densities in the frontal and parietal cortices (both at 1.6 and 0.8 mm).

Statistics

Behavioral data during ShA and LgA sessions were analyzed using one-way and two-way (group × 2-session block) ANOVA, respectively. Infusions under PR were analyzed using two-way ANOVA (group × PR dose or PR speed). Significant interaction effects under PR were investigated with two-tailed unpaired t-tests. D2 data were analyzed using two-way ANOVA (group × rostro-caudal level) except in regions where only one rostro-caudal level was analyzed (orbitofrontal and prefrontal cortices), in which case one-way ANOVA was used.

RESULTS

Self-administration During ShA Sessions

Figure 2 illustrates SA behavior averaged over the last two of the three ShA sessions. Cocaine delivery speed had no effect on the average number of cocaine infusions (a) or inter-infusion interval ((b); all P’s>0.05).

Figure 2
figure 2

The speed of intravenous cocaine delivery does not influence self-administration behavior when access to the drug is limited (1 h/session). Number of infusions earned (a) and inter-infusion interval (b) in animals self-administering 0.5 or 0.25 mg/kg/infusion cocaine delivered over 5 or 90 s. (c) Cumulative response records for self-administered infusions in a representative rat from each group during the last of the three ShA sessions. Data represent mean±SEM. n=11–16/group.

PowerPoint slide

Self-administration During LgA Sessions

Figure 3 shows self-administration behavior during LgA sessions. The ShA sessions shown in Figure 2 are also included in Figure 3a–f for comparison. When sessions were extended from 1 (ShA) to 6 h (Figure 3b and d), all the groups increased their total drug intake (ShA vs first 2-session block; main effect of session; (b) F(1,22)=71.76; (d): F(1,30)=146.94; all P’s<0.0001). However, despite having equal opportunity to self-administer cocaine (ie, all rats could only take one injection every 90 s), the 5-s groups took significantly more drug in the first hour as well as after 6 h during the LgA sessions (main effect of group; (a) F(1,22)=7.55; (b) F(1,22)=11.87; (c) F(1,30)=28.72; (d) F(1,30)=38.41; all P’s<0.02).

Figure 3
figure 3

When access to cocaine is extended to 6 h per session, rats self-administering rapid cocaine injections (delivered over 5 vs 90 s) take more drug. Number of infusions earned during the first hour of the 6-h sessions (a, c) or during the entire 6-h session (b, d) by rats self-administrating 0.25 or 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine delivered over 5 or 90 s. The inter-infusion interval in rats self-administering 0.25 (e) or 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine (f) delivered over 5 or 90 s. (g) Cumulative cocaine intake over the 12 6-h sessions as a function of group. The number of infusions averaged over the last two ShA sessions is included for comparison in all of the panels except for panel g. All values are mean±SEM. n=11–16/group. ShA, short access. *P<0.05.

PowerPoint slide

Figure 3e and f show the inter-infusion interval as a function of group. During LgA sessions, the 90-s rats waited longer between each self-administered infusion as compared with the 5-s rats (main effect of group; (e) F(1,22)=5.67, P=0.03; (f) F(1,30)=6.40, P=0.02). Inspection of Figure 3e and f also reveals that the self-imposed inter-infusion interval in the 5-s rats was considerably longer than the experimenter-imposed 90-s timeout interval in these groups. This suggests that the 90-s timeout in the 5-s groups did not artificially constrain self-administration behavior.

As shown in Figure 3g, cumulative drug intake was greatest in the 0.5 mg/kg–5-s group (main effect of group; vs 0.5 mg/kg–90-s, F(1,30)=30.08; vs 0.25 mg/kg–5-s, F(1,27)=22.48; vs 0.25 mg/kg–90-s, F(1,25)=61; all P’s<0.0001). Also, at the 0.25 mg/kg dose, the 5-s group took more cocaine than the 90-s group (F(1,22)=12.23, P=0.002). No other comparisons were significant.

Inactive lever presses, unreinforced active lever presses, and within-session variability in the inter-infusion interval are illustrated in Supplementary Figure S1. Unreinforced active lever presses were defined as active lever presses made either during the 5-s infusion and 85-s timeout period (5-s groups) or during the 90-s infusion (90-s groups). Within-session variability in the inter-infusion interval was calculated using the root mean square of successive differences (rMSSD). During LgA sessions, the speed of cocaine delivery did not influence inactive lever presses (main effect of group; (a) F(1,22)=2.37; (b) F(1,30)=1.73; All P's>0.05) or unreinforced active lever presses ((c) F(1,22)=1.04; (d) F(1,30)=1.41; All P’s>0. 05). In animals self-administering 0.25 mg/kg/infusion cocaine, rMSSD over the 12 LgA sessions was greater in the 90- vs the 5-s group (main effect of group; (e) F(1,22)=8.01; P<0.01). There was a tendency towards a similar effect in animals self-administering 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine ((f) F(1,30)=4.12, P=0.0513). Thus, animals self-administering slower cocaine injections (delivered over 90 vs 5 s) evidence a more irregular pattern of SA during each LgA session.

The Motivation for Cocaine

Figure 4 shows cocaine intake under a PR schedule of reinforcement. As during the previous FR self-administration sessions, all rats could only take one injection every 90 s during the PR tests. In Figure 4a and b, cocaine was delivered over 5 s for the 5-s groups and over 90 s for the 90-s groups. Across all cocaine doses tested, animals in the 5-s groups self-administered more drug than animals in the 90-s groups (main effect of group; (a) F(1,22)=26.72, (b) F(1,30)=16.40; all P’s<0.0004). Also, there were qualitative differences in self-administration behavior under PR. Animals in the 5-s groups increased their drug intake with increasing dose (group × dose interaction effect; (b) F(2,60)=7.52, P=0.001; and a similar tendency in (a), F(2,44)=2.92, P=0.06). In contrast, animals in the 90-s groups took comparable amounts of cocaine at all of the doses tested (Figure 4a and b; all P’s>0.05).

Figure 4
figure 4

Animals that have self-administered rapid cocaine injections (delivered over 5 vs 90 s) are more motivated to obtain the drug. In panels a and b, different doses of cocaine were delivered over 5 s for the 5-s groups and over 90 s for the 90-s groups, under a progressive ratio schedule. In panels c and d, cocaine was delivered over 5 and 90 s during progressive ratio testing in rats that had previously self-administered 0.25 mg/kg/infusion or 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine delivered over 5 or 90 s. Values are mean±SEM of 2–3 sessions/dose or injection speed. n=11–16/group. PR 5 s, cocaine was delivered over 5 s during progressive ratio testing. PR 90 s, cocaine was delivered over 90 s during progressive ratio testing. *P<0.05 compared with the 90-s group. #P<0.0001 compared with the 90-s group.

PowerPoint slide

Figure 4c and d show the effects of delivering cocaine over 5 or 90 s during PR testing in rats that had previously self-administered cocaine delivered over 5 or 90 s. Thus, during these PR tests, the speed of drug delivery was the same for all rats. In Figure 4c, there was a significant main effect of group (F(1,22)=4.90, P=0.04) and a significant group × PR speed interaction effect (F(1,22)=4.97, P=0.04). Investigation of this interaction revealed that when cocaine was delivered over 90 s during PR testing, rats that had previously self-administered 5-s cocaine infusions took more drug than rats that had previously self-administered 90-s cocaine infusions (t(22)=3.17, P=0.004). In Figure 4d, rats that had previously self-administered cocaine delivered over 5 s took more drug than their 90-s counterparts, regardless of whether cocaine was delivered over 5 or 90 s during PR testing (main effect of group; F(1,30)=6.26, P=0.018).

D2 Receptor Number and Function

There were no group differences in D2 receptor density in the nucleus accumbens (Figure 5; two-way ANOVA; main effect of group; (e), F(2,18)=0.20; (f), F(2,18)=0.05; all P’s>0.05) or in any of the cortical areas quantified (data not shown; all P’s>0.05). As shown in Figure 5, the speed of cocaine delivery altered D2 receptor density in the caudate-putamen (two-way ANOVA on levels 1.2–0.4 mm relative to Bregma; main effect of group; DM, F(2,19)=4.18; DL, F(2,19)=7.76; VL, F(2,19)=5.02; all P’s<0.05; with the exception of the VM, P=0.11). D2 receptor density was decreased in the 90-s group as compared with either the control or the 5-s group (DM, 90 s vs Control, F(1,14)=7.91, 90 vs 5 s, F(1,11)=8.02; DL, 90 s vs Control, F(1,14)=14.52, 90 vs 5 s, F(1,11)=13.68; VL, 90 s vs Control, F(1,14)=8.93, 90 vs 5 s, F(1,11)=8.60; VM, 90 s vs Control, F(1,14)=5.49; but not vs 5 s, F(1,11)=3.28, P=0.10; all other P’s<0.05). There were no significant differences in D2 receptor levels between the 5-s and the control groups (all P’s>0.05).

Figure 5
figure 5

A history of self-administering slow (delivered over 90 s) but not rapid cocaine injections (5 s) decreases D2 receptor density in the caudate-putamen. D2 receptor density in the dorsomedial (a), dorsolateral (b), ventromedial (c), and ventrolateral (d) quadrants of the caudate-putamen and in the core (e) and shell (f) of the nucleus accumbens 14 days after the last self-administration session in animals that had self-administered cocaine delivered over 5 or 90 s and in control rats. Receptor density was measured by autoradiographic binding with (125I) iodosulpiride. μCi/g, microcurie per gram of tissue. C, nucleus accumbens core. S, nucleus accumbens shell. Data represent mean±SEM. n=6–9/group. *P<0.05 90-s group compared with the control group at levels 1.2–0.4. #P<0.05 90-s group compared with the 5-s group at levels 1.2–0.4.

PowerPoint slide

With regard to D2 receptor function, there were group differences in the basal levels of [35S]GTPγS binding (data not shown; two-way ANOVA on 1.6–0.4 mm; DM, F(2, 19)=6.12; DL, F(2, 18)=4.89; VM, F(2, 19)=7.68; VL, F(2, 19)=7.11; all P’s<0.02). Relative to both the 5-s and control groups, the 90-s group had lower basal levels of [35S]GTPγS binding in the caudate-putamen (main effect of group; DM, 90 s vs Control, F(1,14)=10.27, 90 vs 5 s, F(1,11)=13.9; DL, 90 s vs Control, F(1,14)=8.07, 90 vs 5 s, F(1,10)=17.39; VM, 90 s vs Control, F(1,14)=13.94, 90 vs 5 s, F(1,11)=8.94; VL, 90 s vs Control, F(1,14)=12.59, 90 vs 5 s, F(1,11)=11.21; all P’s<0.02). Basal [35S]GTPγS activity in the caudate-putamen was comparable between the 5-s and the control groups (all P’s>0.05).

In addition, there were significant group differences in agonist-stimulated [35S]GTPγS activity in the caudate-putamen (Figure 6 illustrates these data as the percentage of change from net stimulation in control animals; overall two-way ANOVA on 1.2–0.8 mm; DM, F(2, 19)=9.00; DL, F(2, 19)=9.60; VM, F(2, 19)=11.52; VL (2, 19)=11.74; all P’s<0.002). Relative to controls, the 5-s group had greater levels of quinpirole-stimulated [35S]GTPγS activity in ventral aspects of the caudate-putamen (main effect of group; VM, F(1, 13)=6.57; VL, F(1,13)=5.97; all P’s<0.05; DM and DL, P’s>0.05). The 90-s group was significantly different from controls in all caudate-putamen quadrants (DM, F(1, 14)=19.50; DL, F(1,14)=19.85; VM, F(1, 14)=24.39; VL, F(1,14)=21.33; all P’s<0.05). The 90-s group also had greater levels of quinpirole-stimulated [35S]GTPγS activity in the caudate-putamen relative to the 5-s group (two-way ANOVA on 1.2–0.8 mm; main effect of group; DM, F(1,11)=6.09; DL, F(1,11)=6.55; VL, F(1,11)=5.03; all P’s<0.05; with the exception of VM, P=0.11). In the nucleus accumbens core and shell (Figure 6e and f), there was no effect of the speed of cocaine delivery (all P’s>0.05), but both cocaine groups had increased quinpirole-stimulated [35S]GTPγS binding relative to control animals (overall two-way ANOVA on 1.6 and 1.2 mm relative to Bregma; core, F(2, 15)=6.77; shell, F(2, 15)=9.47; Core, 5 s vs Control, F(1,9)=9.83; 90 s vs Control, F(1,11)=10.09; Shell, 5 s vs Control, F(1,9)=10.50; 90 s vs Control, F(1,11)=12.13; All P’s<0.02). There were no group differences in any of the cortical areas quantified (data not shown; all P’s>0.05).

Figure 6
figure 6

A history of self-administering slow cocaine injections (delivered over 90 vs 5 s) leads to a greater increase in agonist-stimulated [35S]GTPγS binding in the caudate-putamen. Quinpirole-stimulated [35S]GTPγS activity in the dorsomedial (a), dorsolateral (b), ventromedial (c), and ventrolateral (d) quadrants of the caudate-putamen, as well as in the nucleus accumbens core (e) and shell (f) 14 days after the last self-administration session in animals that had self-administered cocaine delivered over 5 or 90 s and in control rats. In panel (e), the inset shows data at +0.8 mm relative to Bregma. C, nucleus accumbens core; S, nucleus accumbens shell. Data represent mean±SEM. n=6–7/group. *P<0.05 relative to the 5-s group at corresponding rostro-caudal position. #P<0.05 relative to the control group at corresponding rostro-caudal position.

PowerPoint slide

In summary, the largest effects on D2 receptor number and function were observed in the caudate-putamen. Striatal D2 receptor density was decreased only in animals that had chronically self-administered slow cocaine infusions (delivered over 90 s), and D2 agonist-stimulated Gαi/o-protein activation was increased to a greater extent in the 90- vs 5-s rats.

DISCUSSION

Our objective here was to determine whether a history of chronic exposure to rapidly delivered cocaine promotes increased motivation to take the drug. Rats first self-administered cocaine delivered intravenously over 5 or 90 s under a FR schedule of reinforcement during 1- and 6-h sessions. The motivation to self-administer cocaine was then assessed using a PR schedule of reinforcement. The PR procedure measures motivation for drugs by quantifying the maximal amount of operant responding (lever-pressing in our case) that an animal will perform to obtain a single drug infusion before cessation of responding (Richardson and Roberts, 1996). During all self-administration sessions, rats in all of the groups had equal opportunity to take cocaine (one injection was available every 90 s). Consistent with previous work, the speed of cocaine delivery influenced self-administration behaviour when access to cocaine was extended to 6 hours/session but not when it was limited to 1 hour/session (Wakabayashi et al, 2010). When given extended access to the drug, animals self-administering rapid cocaine injections (delivered over 5 s) took almost twice as more drug than rats self-administering more sustained cocaine injections (90 s). It has been proposed that limited access procedures are best suited to study acute reinforcement, whereas extended access is a better model to study the progressive changes in the behavioral and brain effects of drugs that characterize the addiction process (Ahmed, 2012; Roberts et al, 2007). If this is true, then it might not be so surprising that varying drug injection speed between 5 and 100 s does not dramatically alter the acute reinforcing strength of cocaine during limited sessions but determines the susceptibility to increase cocaine intake in a more addiction-relevant, extended access model ((Crombag et al, 2008; Pickens et al, 1969; Wakabayashi et al, 2010) and present findings). Similarly, varying the speed of cocaine delivery over the same range does not alter the acute locomotor response to the drug but does determine the development of psychomotor sensitization to cocaine (Samaha et al, 2002; Samaha et al, 2004).

Animals given extended access to cocaine often escalate their intake of drug over time (Ahmed, 2012). However, we observed no such escalation. Studies giving extended access to doses of cocaine similar to the ones used here do not always report escalated drug intake (Ferrario and Robinson, 2007; Kippin et al, 2006; Mantsch et al, 2004). Escalation is also reported to be more likely at higher doses of cocaine (2 mg/kg/infusion; (Mantsch et al, 2004)). Finally, rats can show significant inter-individual variability in the propensity to escalate their drug intake (Deroche-Gamonet et al, 2004; Wakabayashi et al, 2010). It is also noteworthy that under FR, the rats self-administering 0.5 mg/kg/infusion cocaine, regardless of infusion speed, took a similar number of infusions compared with the rats self-administering 0.25 mg/kg/infusion. The two doses might not have been far enough apart on the dose–response curve to evoke differences in cocaine intake (Mantsch et al, 2001; Wee et al, 2007).

In addition to influencing cocaine intake, a major finding of the current study is that the speed of cocaine delivery determines the subsequent motivation to obtain the drug. Animals that had self-administered rapid cocaine injections worked significantly harder for the drug under a PR schedule and did so across a range of doses and different speeds of cocaine delivery during PR testing. Varying the rate of cocaine delivery led to both quantitative and qualitative differences in cocaine intake under PR conditions. Animals that had previously self-administered rapid cocaine infusions worked harder to obtain the drug across a range of doses. Also, the 5-s rats showed a dose-dependent decrease in cocaine intake under PR, as rats usually do (Roberts et al, 1989). In contrast, the 90-s rats maintained the same level of drug intake regardless of dose. It is not clear why rats in the 90-s condition were insensitive to variations in cocaine dose. However, in typical SA studies drug is injected in <10 s. To our knowledge, this is the first published report of a dose–response curve under a PR schedule in animals self-administering slower cocaine injections. Rats that had self-administered rapid cocaine injections in the past (delivered over 5 vs 90 s) also worked harder for the drug regardless of whether cocaine was delivered over 5 or 90 s during PR testing. This suggests that the motivation for cocaine is determined by the speed of self-administered cocaine injections in the past and not simply by the speed of drug delivery during PR testing (see also (Liu et al, 2005)).

The precise mechanisms underlying the effects of the speed of drug delivery on the motivation for cocaine remain to be identified. However, certain potential explanations must be considered. It could be argued that the 5-s groups demonstrated greater motivation to self-administer cocaine during PR testing because they had previously been exposed to larger amounts of the drug. Indeed, before testing under PR, animals in the 5-s condition had taken more cocaine than animals in the 90-s condition. However, the SA of larger amounts of cocaine does not necessarily lead to increased drug intake under a PR schedule, sometimes it is quite the opposite (Morgan et al, 2006). In addition, in the present study, the amount of cocaine taken in the past was dissociable from drug intake during PR testing. For example, the 0.25 mg–5 s and 0.5 mg–90 s groups had self-administered similar amounts of cocaine before PR testing (see the two superimposed curves in Figure 3g), yet the 0.25 mg–5 s group subsequently took more drug under PR (compare Figure 4a and b). This being said, the possibility that the amount of cocaine consumed by the 5- vs 90-s rats might have influenced breakpoints for the drug cannot be dismissed entirely. Increased cocaine intake can predict increased breakpoints for the drug. For example, compared with rats with a history of limited access to cocaine, rats previously given extended access take much more cocaine and also subsequently show elevated breakpoints for the drug (Paterson and Markou, 2003; Wee et al, 2008). Thus, it remains possible that the rapid delivery of cocaine to the brain might lead to greater motivation to take the drug in part by promoting the intake of increasing amounts of cocaine. Exposing the brain to larger quantities of cocaine, in turn, would presumably promote the neuroplastic changes that underlie excessive motivation for drug. Another potential explanation is that less cocaine reaches the brain when the drug is delivered over 90 s. This is possible, but unlikely. Pharmacokinetic modelling reveals that when cocaine is delivered between 5 and 100 s, brain Cmax does not vary significantly, but the rate of rise of drug levels does (Samaha et al, 2002; Schindler et al, 2009). Brain dopamine levels produced by cocaine are also linearly correlated with brain concentrations of cocaine (Nicolaysen et al, 1988), and varying the speed of intravenous cocaine injection between 5 and 100 s has no effect on total or peak brain dopamine levels (Ferrario et al, 2008). Finally, there is no difference in self-administration behavior between 5-s and 90-s rats during ShA sessions ((Crombag et al, 2008; Wakabayashi et al, 2010) and present findings), suggesting that similar amounts of cocaine are reaching the brain. It appears, therefore, that something about the temporal dynamics of chronic cocaine delivery to the brain determines the subsequent motivation to take the drug.

Several psychological processes are likely to be involved in the ability of rapidly delivered cocaine to enhance drug intake under a PR schedule of reinforcement. For example, the 5-s groups might have taken more cocaine because they developed greater tolerance to the hedonic and rewarding effects of the drug, thus requiring greater amounts of cocaine to obtain the same subjective effects (Ahmed et al, 2002). However, compared with the 90-s groups, the 5-s groups showed an upward shift in the dose–response curve for cocaine during PR testing (Figure 4a and b), as opposed to the downward shift associated with tolerance (Li et al, 1994). The 5-s groups might also have worked harder for cocaine, because they attributed increased motivational value not only to cocaine but also to cocaine-associated stimuli as well. In both the 5- and 90-s groups, each self-administered cocaine infusion was accompanied by illumination of a stimulus light, making this light a cocaine cue. Such drug cues can acquire incentive motivational properties of their own and enhance drug-taking behavior (Panlilio et al, 1996). However, discrete environmental cues paired with either 5- or 90-s cocaine injections acquire similar incentive motivational properties (Samaha et al, 2011). Finally, it has been shown that rapid cocaine infusions more readily induce forms of neural plasticity that are linked to increased incentive motivation for the drug (Liu et al, 2005; Samaha et al, 2002; Samaha et al, 2004; Wakabayashi et al, 2010), and this could account for why animals in the 5-s groups both increase their intake during the LgA sessions and evidence greater motivation to obtain cocaine under PR. For example, increasing the speed of intravenous cocaine delivery between 100 and 5 s (1) more readily induces psychomotor sensitization (Samaha et al, 2002; Samaha et al, 2004, 2) enhances immediate early gene expression in mesocorticolimbic structures (Ferrario et al, 2008; Samaha et al, 2004), and (3) produces more immediate blockade of dopamine reuptake (Samaha et al, 2004; Woolverton and Wang, 2004), dopamine overflow (Ferrario et al, 2008), and heat-producing metabolic activity (Brown and Kiyatkin, 2005) in regions including the nucleus accumbens and ventral tegmental area. Thus, by more readily engaging the neural circuits involved in incentive motivation, rapidly delivered cocaine could promote increased drug consumption as well as increased responding for the drug in the face of rising costs (as modelled here by the PR procedure).

Consistent with the literature cited above, our findings show that variations in the speed of drug delivery alter the neurobiological impact of cocaine. Following 14 days of cocaine withdrawal, the 5-s rats had normal levels of striatal D2 receptors and enhanced quinpirole-induced GTPγS binding to D2 receptors compared with cocaine-naive control animals. This is consistent with the work showing that extended access to rapid cocaine injections (0.4 mg/kg/infusion, over 1.6 s) does not change striatal D2 receptor protein levels but increases the proportion of striatal D2 receptors that are in a high-affinity state for dopamine (Briand et al, 2008). In contrast, a history of chronic self-administration of slower cocaine injections (90 s) downregulates striatal D2 receptors and also enhances agonist-induced GTPγS binding to caudate-putamen D2 receptors to a greater extent than that seen following the self-administration of rapid cocaine injections (5 s). At present, it cannot be determined on which side of the synapse these receptor changes occur, or whether the net effect is a potentiation or a blunting of dopamine-mediated signalling. However, the results suggest that changes in affinity are not simply a consequence of changes in density or vice versa. Changes in receptor density and in agonist affinity did not always occur in the same locations in the caudate-putamen. For example, at +0.4 mm relative to Bregma, the 90-s animals had decreased striatal D2 receptor levels (Figure 5) but no change in quinpirole-induced GTPγS binding to these receptors (Figure 6).

It is important to consider that because the control group used here was not a yoked saline group, we cannot determine how intravenous infusions of fluid delivered at different speeds might contribute to the D2 receptor changes we report. However, just as there were no differences in striatal D2 receptor density between the control vs 5-s rats, others have reported no differences in striatal D2 receptor density following yoked saline vs the SA of cocaine delivered over 5 s (Frankowska et al, 2013). This study also did not include a yoked cocaine group. As such, we cannot exclude the possibility that interaction effects between the speed of cocaine delivery, the pharmacological effects of the drug, and the volitional self-administration of cocaine might have contributed to the observed changes in D2 receptor number and function.

It is not clear how the observed D2 receptor changes might contribute to the effects of the speed of cocaine delivery on the motivation to self-administer the drug. The two effects might not be causally related. Another possibility is that decreases in D2 receptor number and/or increases in D2 agonist affinity in specific caudate-putamen locations modify striatal signalling in ways that protect from excessive motivation to take cocaine. Indeed, we report the novel and surprising finding that striatal D2 receptor levels are decreased and D2 agonist affinity is increased in rats that take only a modest amount of cocaine when given the opportunity to self-administer much more and that are only moderately motivated to lever-press for the drug (ie, the 90-s rats). Changes in D2 density or agonist affinity were either absent or less pronounced in the 5-s rats. As such, it is possible that rapidly injected cocaine leads to greater motivation to obtain the drug in part by preventing forms of D2-related neuroadaptations that protect from compulsive drug use. However, this remains highly speculative, and contradicts a number of findings in the literature. Indeed, reduced striatal D2 receptor availability and increased D2 receptor function are each reported to enhance drug effects that are relevant to addiction. For example, cocaine abusers have reduced striatal D2 receptor availability (Martinez et al, 2004; Volkow et al, 1993; Volkow et al, 1990), and innately low striatal D2 receptor availability is predictive of both increased positive subjective ratings of a psychostimulant such as methylphenidate (Volkow et al, 1999) and enhanced psychomotor sensitization to amphetamine (Tournier et al, 2013). In addition, the D1/D2 receptor agonist pergolide increases craving for cocaine in cocaine abusers (but decreases many of the drug’s positive subjective effects in the same subjects (Haney et al, 1998). D2-like agonists can also trigger the reinstatement of extinguished cocaine-seeking behavior in rats (Self et al, 1996) and evoke a behaviorally sensitized psychomotor response in methamphetamine- or cocaine-treated rats (Ujike et al, 1990).

In conclusion, the present results demonstrate that the rapid delivery of cocaine leads to greater drug intake and greater motivation to expend physical effort to obtain the drug. The self-administration of rapid vs more sustained cocaine infusions also led to distinct neurobiological profiles, such that striatal D2 receptor density and agonist affinity were altered to a greater extent in animals that had been exposed to slower cocaine injections. We suggest that the rapid uptake of cocaine into the brain might facilitate addiction not simply because it increases the reinforcing or subjective pleasurable effects of the drug but because rapid delivery alters brain motivation circuits in ways that increase the willingness to self-administer the drug in the face of rising costs, leading to pathological drug use.

FUNDING AND DISCLOSURE

The authors declare no conflict of interest. This research was supported by grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (http://www.cihr-irsc.gc.ca/e/193.html; Grant No. 97841) and the Canada Foundation for Innovation (http://www.innovation.ca/en; Grant No. 24326) to ANS, and a salary award from the Fonds de la Recherche en Santé du Québec (http://www.frsq.gouv.qc.ca/en/index.shtml; Grant No. 16193) to ANS.