INTRODUCTION

The endocannabinoid (eCB) system is densely distributed in key brain regions that subserve feeding, motivation, and stress. Accordingly, a growing literature highlights its pivotal role in the modulation of food intake, drug abuse, and anxiety (Di Marzo et al, 2011; Gaetani et al, 2008; Kirkham, 2009; Pacher et al, 2006; Patel and Hillard, 2008; Sidhpura and Parsons, 2011; Supplementary ref 1–2). Rimonabant, an antagonist of the type-1 cannabinoid (CB1) receptor, was approved in 2006 in several countries for the treatment of obesity; however, it was withdrawn from the market 2 years thereafter because of adverse psychiatric side effects, which included a negative affective state (Moreira et al, 2009; Supplementary ref 3-6). Here, we provide evidence that rimonabant may precipitate a withdrawal-like state in patients with obesity or eating disorders, analogous to the withdrawal-like state that receptor antagonists of a drug of abuse can precipitate in drug-dependent subjects. Eating disorders and obesity, similarly to drug addiction, can be conceptualized as chronic relapsing conditions with alternating periods of abstinence (eg, dieting) and relapse (compulsive overeating of palatable foods; Corwin and Grigson, 2009; Cottone et al, 2009a; Epstein and Shaham, 2010; Johnson and Kenny, 2010; Parylak et al, 2011). Chronic, intermittent drug use can result in affective dependence over repeated cycles of abuse and abstinence, culminating with the emergence of a negative emotional state (eg, dysphoria, anxiety, irritability) when drug use ceases or is prevented (‘withdrawal’; Koob and Volkow, 2010). For many drugs of abuse, emotional or physical signs of withdrawal also can be triggered by pharmacologically blocking the effects of the reinforcing drug, eliciting a ‘precipitated withdrawal’ syndrome (eg, naltrexone/naloxone-opiate withdrawal; flumazenil-benzodiazepine withdrawal; mecamylamine-nicotine withdrawal; Liu and Schulteis, 2004; Lukas and Griffiths, 1982; Malin et al, 1994; Supplementary ref 7-8). Accordingly, rimonabant administration can precipitate withdrawal symptoms in both cannabinoid and opiate dependence (Navarro et al, 2001; Rodriguez de Fonseca et al, 1997; Valverde et al, 2000). Interestingly, withdrawal-like symptoms can be triggered by pharmacological blockade even when subjects are abstaining from the substance of abuse, putatively reflecting abuse-induced neuroadaptation (Avena et al, 2008; Goldberg and Schuster, 1969; Paterson and Markou, 2004; Rosenberg and Chiu, 1982; Supplementary ref 9-21). The present study tested the hypothesis that rimonabant treatment precipitates a negative emotional state in rats withdrawn from a chronic, intermittent access to a highly palatable diet.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Please see Supplementary Material for additional details.

Subjects

Female Wistar rats (n=229), weighing 126–150 g and 41- to 47-day old upon arrival (Charles River, Wilmington, MA), were housed in wire-topped, plastic cages (27 × 48 × 20 cm) in a 12 : 12 h reverse light cycle (lights off at 1000 hours), in a humidity- (60%) and temperature-controlled (22 °C) vivarium. Rats had access to chow (Harlan Teklad LM-485 Diet 7012 (58% (kcal) carbohydrate, 17% fat, 25% protein 310 cal/100 g); Harlan, Indianapolis, IN) and water ad libitum at all times, unless otherwise stated. Procedures adhered to the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (NIH publication number 85–23, revised 1996) and the Principles of Laboratory Animal Care (http://www.nap.edu/readingroom/books/labrats), and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees of Boston University or Scripps Research Institute. No experimental procedures involved food or water restriction/deprivation.

Drug

Rimonabant (SR141716A, 5-(4-Chlorophenyl)-1-(2,4-dichloro-phenyl)-4-methyl-N-(piperidin-1-yl)-1H-pyrazole-3-carboxamide) HCl was solubilized in an 18 : 1 : 1 mixture of saline/ethanol/cremophor and was administered either intraperitoneally (0, 0.3, 1, 3 mg/kg, 1 ml/kg, 30 min pretreatment) or bilaterally into the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA; 0, 0.5, 1.5 μg/side, 30 min pretreatment). Intraperitoneal and site-specific doses of rimonabant were chosen based on previous literature (Caille and Parsons, 2006; Dore et al, 2013b; Gessa et al, 2006; Roche et al, 2007).

Ad libitum Diet Alternation

Ad libitum diet alternation was performed as previously described (Cottone et al, 2009a; Cottone et al, 2008, 2009b; Iemolo et al, 2012; see scheme in Figure 1a). After acclimation, rats were divided into two groups matched for food intake, body weight and feed efficiency from the previous 4 days. One group was provided a chow diet (‘Chow’) ad libitum 7 days a week (Chow/Chow), and a second group was provided chow ad libitum 5 days a week followed by 2 days of ad libitum access to a highly palatable, chocolate flavored, high-sucrose diet (‘Palatable’; Chow/Palatable). Chow diet was the above-described Harlan Teklad LM-485 Diet 7012. The palatable diet was a nutritionally complete, high-sucrose (50% kcal), AIN-76A-based diet that is comparable in macronutrient proportions and energy density to the chow diet (5TUL: 66.7% (kcal) carbohydrate, 12.7% fat, 20.6% protein, 344 cal/100 g; TestDiet). For brevity, the first 5 days (chow only) and last 2 days (chow or palatable according to experimental group) of each week are referred to in all experiments as C and P phases (see scheme Figure 1a). Palatable diet was provided in GPF20 ‘J’-feeders (Ancare, Bellmore, NY). Diets were never concurrently available. All rats were diet cycled for at least 7 weeks (ranging from 8 to 11) and the number of weeks was equally represented in all the experimental groups. The length of diet cycles (7 days) reduces the likelihood that the estrous cycle (4–5 days) accounts for observed effects and resembles designs used in previous diet-cycling studies of female rats (Cottone et al, 2009b; Hagan and Moss, 1997).

Figure 1
figure 1

Effects of systemic administration of rimonabant (30 min pretreatment, 0, 0.3, 1, 3 mg/kg, i.p.) on anxiety-like behavior, plasma corticosterone, regular chow intake, and body weight change in female Wistar rats (n=94) withdrawn for 4 days from chronic, intermittent access to a highly palatable diet. (a) Schematic representation of the experimental design used in this study. Subjects were divided in two groups: a first group received a standard chow diet ad libitum daily (Chow/Chow), and a second group received 5 days of chow diet followed by 2 days of highly palatable, chocolate-flavored, high-sucrose, diet (Chow/Palatable). For brevity, the first 5 days (chow only) and last 2 days (chow or palatable per diet condition) of each week are referred to as C and P Phases. Rats were diet-cycled for at least 7 weeks and tests were performed on the 4th day of palatable food withdrawal (C Phase, both groups are receiving the chow diet). (b) Chow/Palatable rats did not significantly differ from Chow/Chow rats in the basal level of anxiety-like behavior, measured using the elevated plus-maze test during the 4th day of withdrawal from chronic, intermittent access to the palatable diet (C Phase). Left panel: % open arm time, a measure of anxiety-like behavior; right panel: number of closed arm entries, a measure of locomotor activity. (c) Rimonabant (3 mg/kg) selectively precipitates anxiety-like behavior, measured using a defensive withdrawal test, in Chow/Palatable rats. (d) Rimonabant similarly activates the HPA axis in both Chow/Chow and Chow/Palatable rats, as measured by plasma corticosterone levels. Left panel: time course (ng/ml, % of basal Chow/Chow group); right panel: area under the curve (AUC). Rats were treated (as symbolically represented by the arrow) immediately after the baseline blood collection. (e) Rimonabant more potently precipitates chow anorexia and body weight loss in withdrawn Chow/Palatable rats than in chow controls. (Top panels) 2 h Food intake, (middle panels) 24 h food intake, and (bottom panels) body weight change. Panels represent M±SEM. Symbols denote: *significant difference from vehicle-treated group p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001.

PowerPoint slide

Intracranial Surgeries and Microinfusion Procedure

Surgeries were performed following a previously described procedure (Sabino et al, 2007). The cannula coordinates used were A/P −0.2 mm, M/L±4.1 mm, D/V −6.5 mm. The interaural bar was set at +5 according to the atlas of Pellegrino (Pellegrino, 1979).

Drug was microinfused in the brain of rats as described previously (Blasio et al, 2013; Dore et al, 2013b). Microinfusions were performed in 0.5 μl volume delivered over 2 min. Cannula placement was verified at the conclusion of all testing.

Behavioral Tests

Food intake and body weight measurements

Food intake was measured at 2 h and 24 h later and rats’ body weights at 24 h. Rimonabant was administered intraperitoneally (0, 0.3, 1, 3 mg/kg) or was microinfused within the CeA (0, 0.5 and 1.5 μg/side). Treatments were administered during the first switch to palatable diet (first week, P Phase, 6th day of the cycle), or after 7 weeks of cycling on the 4th day of withdrawal from palatable diet (C phase, 4th day of the cycle), or after 7 weeks of cycling on the first day of renewed access to palatable diet (P Phase, 6th day of the cycle).

Defensive withdrawal test

The defensive withdrawal test was performed as previously described (Cottone et al, 2009b). Rats were diet-cycled for at least 7 weeks and tested during the 4th day of withdrawal from palatable diet (C Phase). Rats were pretreated with either vehicle or rimonabant (0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg, i.p.).

Elevated Plus-maze test

The elevated plus-maze was performed as previously described (Cottone et al, 2009b; Dore et al, 2013a). Rats were diet-cycled for at least 7 weeks and tested on the 4th day of withdrawal from palatable diet (C Phase) for spontaneous anxiety-like behavior or following microinfusion of rimonabant into the CeA (0.5 μg/side).

Tissue eCB Measurement

Chow/Palatable (n=23) rats were diet-cycled for at least 7 weeks. On the 4th day (C Phase) or on the 7th day (P Phase) the rats were killed right after the dark cycle onset and CeA, nucleus accumbens (NAc), and lateral hypothalamus (LH) punches were stored at −80 °C. The brain samples were not pooled together. Each data point represented the eCB level of an individual subject. Anandamide (AEA) and 2-arachidonoylglycerol (2-AG) levels were quantified as previously described (Grimaldi et al, 2009).

Quantitative Real-Time PCR

To quantify CB1 mRNA levels, rats (n=6) were diet-cycled for 7 weeks and then decapitated on the 4th day of the chow phase right after the dark cycle onset. Brains were quickly removed and sliced coronally in a brain matrix. CeA punches were stored at −80 °C. Total RNA was extracted according to manufacturer recommendations (Invitrogen), reverse transcribed in cDNA and analyzed as previously described (Grimaldi et al, 2009).

Western Blotting

A different cohort of rats (n=8) was diet-cycled for at least 7 weeks. On the 4th day of chow phase, rats were sacrificed and CeA punches were collected. Punches were homogenized in lysis buffer (20 mM HEPES, pH=7.4, EDTA 2 mM, 1% SDS, 10% sucrose) by sonication, and centrifuged at 10 000 × g for 20 min at 4 °C. The supernatant was transferred to a new tube, and protein concentration in lysates was determined by the BCA assay (Pierce) using BSA as standard. Subsequently, the samples (70–80 μg of total protein) were boiled for 5 min in Laemmli SDS loading buffer, loaded on 8–10% SDS-polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis, and then transferred to a PVDF membrane. Filters were incubated overnight at 4 °C with monoclonal anti-CB1 (209550 N221/17, dilution 1 : 1000 Calbiochem, Millipore Billerica, MA, USA). The monoclonal anti-tubulin clone B-5-1-2 (dilution, 1 : 5000; Sigma-Aldrich) was used to check for equal protein loading. Reactive bands were detected by chemiluminescence (ECL or ECL-plus; Perkin-Elmer) and the images were analyzed on a Chemi-Doc station with Quantity-one software (Bio-Rad, Segrate MI, Italy).

Plasma Corticosterone RIA

Tail blood samples were collected after the 7th week of cycling from rats (n=24) at four time points on the 4th day of the chow phase right after the dark cycle onset: basal condition and 30 min following rimonabant administration (0, 3 mg/kg, i.p., 30 min pretreatment). Rimonabant was administered immediately after the basal blood collection. Plasma levels of corticosterone-like immunoreactivity levels were quantified as previously described (Cottone et al, 2009a).

Statistical Analysis

Student’s t-tests were used to analyze factors with two levels. ANOVAs were performed to analyze factors with more than two levels. Following significant omnibus effect of ANOVAs (p<0.05), Student–Newman–Keuls post-hoc comparison tests were used. The statistical packages used were Instat 3.0 (GraphPad, San Diego, CA), Systat 11.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL) and Statistica 7 (StatSoft, Tulsa, OK).

RESULTS

Rimonabant, Systemically Administered, Precipitates Anxiety-like Behavior in Rats Withdrawn from Palatable Diet Cycling

To test the hypothesis that rimonabant treatment precipitates anxiety-like behavior during withdrawal from chronic intermittent access to palatable food, female Wistar rats were provided a chow diet ad libitum every day of the week (Chow/Chow) or were provided a chow diet for 5 days (C Phase) followed by a highly palatable, sugary diet for 2 days (P phase; Chow/Palatable; see scheme in Figure 1a; Cottone et al, 2009a; Cottone et al, 2009b). After at least 7 weeks of diet cycling, rats were administered rimonabant (0, 0.3, 1, 3 mg/kg, i.p.) and tested for anxiety-like behavior in the defensive withdrawal test and in the elevated plus-maze test following 4 days of withdrawal from the palatable diet. At this early protracted withdrawal time point, diet-cycled rats no longer show increased spontaneous (ie, non-precipitated) anxiety like-behavior in the elevated plus-maze (ts(17)<1.46, n.s. Figure 1b) or defensive withdrawal test (vehicle condition, Figure 1c). As shown in Figure 1c, two-way ANOVA revealed a main effect of treatment (F(1,59)=5.14, p<0.003); the highest dose of rimonabant (3 mg/kg) increased the time spent in the closed compartment, an anxiogenic-like effect, selectively in Chow/Palatable rats. To further confirm the precipitation of anxiety-like behavior in Chow/Palatable rats, a different cohort of rats was tested in the elevated plus-maze test using the dose which had effectively induced anxiety-like behavior in the defensive withdrawal test. A two-way ANOVA showed a main effect of treatment (F(1,37)=13.47, p<0.001, Supplementary Figure 1A). Post hoc analysis revealed a significant decrease in percentage of time spent in the open arms in the rimonabant-treated Chow/Palatable group as compared with vehicle condition. No significant effect was observed in Chow/Chow control rats. No effect in the closed arm entries was observed (F(1,37)=1.92, n.s.; Supplementary Figure 1B). Thus, rimonabant, systemically administered, precipitated anxiety-like behavior in rats withdrawn from chronic, intermittent access to palatable food.

Effects of Systemic Administration of Rimonabant on the Adrenocortical Activation in Rats withdrawn from Palatable Diet Cycling

Because rimonabant can activate the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Patel and Hillard, 2008), we tested the hypothesis that rimonabant-induced anxiety-like behavior in diet-cycled rats was related to differential activation of the HPA axis in Chow/Palatable vs Chow/Chow rats. Three-way ANOVA analysis showed a main effect of drug treatment (F(1,20)=4.32, p0.05; Figure 1d) and rimonabant increased corticosterone levels equally in both Chow/Palatable and Chow/Chow rats (Figure 1d). Thus, rimonabant-precipitated anxiety-like behavior in rats withdrawn from chronic, intermittent access to palatable food was independent from the degree of adrenocortical activation.

Rimonabant Precipitates Anorexia and Body Weight Loss in rats Withdrawn from Palatable Diet Cycling

Withdrawal from many drugs of abuse can induce anorexia and body weight loss (Aceto et al, 2001; Koga and Inukai, 1981; Tsuda et al, 1998; Supplementary ref 22–28). To determine whether rimonabant induced anorexia and body weight loss more potently in rats withdrawn from palatable food, we administered the CB1 receptor antagonist during the 4th day of abstinence (when precipitated anxiety like-behavior was observed). Two-way ANOVA revealed that rimonabant dose-dependently reduced food intake and body weight (2 h: F(3,62)= 14.62, p<0.001; 24 h: F(3,62)=16.42, p<0.001; body weight change: F(3,62)=18.00, p<0.001; Figure 1e and Supplementary Figure 2) with a minimum effective dose in Chow/Chow rats of 1 mg/kg. However, the lowest dose of the CB1 receptor antagonist (0.3 mg/kg), known to be ineffective at reducing food intake and body weight in literature (Freedland et al, 2000) and the present study of Chow/Chow rats, significantly reduced 2-h chow intake by 50% in Chow/Palatable rats. Cumulative anorexia persisted through 24 h, by which time the lowest rimonabant dose (0.3 mg/kg) had induced weight loss comparable in magnitude to the body weight lost following treatment with the highest dose (3 mg/kg). Thus, systemic rimonabant treatment dramatically reduced food intake and body weight in diet-cycled rats withdrawn from palatable food at a dose which does not reduce food intake in control conditions.

Effects of Systemic Administration of Rimonabant on Compulsive Eating of Palatable Food

Next, we sought to determine the effects of systemic administration of rimonabant on the compulsive eating of highly palatable food. For this purpose, a different cohort of rats was diet cycled for at least 7 weeks, and rimonabant was administered 30 min before renewing access to the highly palatable diet (C→P). Two-way ANOVA analysis showed that rimonabant dose-dependently reduced food intake and body weight, as expected (2 h: F(3,36)=5.30, p<0.003; 24 h:F(3,36)=8.91, p<0.001; weight change: F(3,36)=8.43, p<0.001; Figure 2a and Supplementary Figures 3A and 4A). However, although Chow/Chow rats appropriately responded to the effects of rimonabant, drug treatment did not significantly affect food intake upon renewed access to the palatable diet in Chow/Palatable rats. Thus, contrary to what observed during withdrawal from palatable food, rimonabant showed decreased anorectic potency to reduce palatable diet intake in rats with a history of chronic, intermittent access to palatable food.

Figure 2
figure 2

Effects of rimonabant (30 min pretreatment, 0, 0.3, 1, 3 mg/kg, i.p.) on food intake in female Wistar rats (n=48) during day 6th (P phase, when Chow/Chow rats are fed chow diet and Chow/Palatable rats are fed the highly palatable diet). (a) Rimonabant fails to reduce 2-h intake of the highly palatable diet. (b) Rimonabant reduces 2-h food intake during the first access to the palatable diet in Chow/Palatable rats. Panels represent M±SEM. Symbols denote: *significant difference from vehicle condition p<0.05, ***p<0.001.

PowerPoint slide

Effects of Systemic Administration of Rimonabant on Intake of Palatable Food During the First Access

To evaluate whether the lack of sensitivity to the anorectic effects of rimonabant in diet-cycled rats refeeding on palatable diet was determined by the diet cycling (as opposed to the diet itself), a different cohort of rats was challenged with the 3 mg/kg of rimonabant 30 min before their very first access to the palatable food, with chow-fed rats tested concurrently. As shown by one-way ANOVA analysis, rimonabant maintained its acute anorectic activity in rats that underwent a single switch to the highly palatable diet (F(1,30)=28.45, p<0.001; Figure 2b and Supplementary Figures 3B and 4B).Thus, the decreased anorectic potency of rimonabant in reducing palatable diet intake in rats with a history of chronic, intermittent access to palatable food, was determined by diet cycling and not by the diet itself.

eCBs Levels in the CeA of Rats Withdrawn from Palatable Diet Cycling

The increased sensitivity to the anxiogenic-like and anorectic effects of rimonabant in Chow/Palatable rats withdrawn from palatable food was suggestive of heightened CB1 receptor signaling. To investigate whether increased eCB tone occurred during palatable food withdrawal, we measured tissue levels of AEA and 2-AG in the CeA, the NAc, and the LH of a separate group of rats that underwent the same diet schedules described above (Figure 1a). As shown in Figure 3a, withdrawal from the palatable diet increased the levels of 2-AG in the CeA of Chow/Palatable rats as compared with Chow/Chow rats. Levels of 2-AG returned to control-like levels upon renewed access to palatable food (F(2,21)=5.028, p<0.02). No significant group differences were observed in AEA levels within the CeA, suggesting specificity of the 2-AG effect. In addition, neither 2-AG nor AEA levels of Chow/Palatable rats differed in the other brain areas analyzed when compared with Chow/Chow rats (2-AG: NAc, F(2,20)=0.58, n.s.; LH, F(2,20)=0.43, n.s.; AEA: NAc, F(2,20)=1.89, n.s.; CeA, F(2,20)=0.45, n.s.; LH F(2,20)=0.58, n.s.; Supplementary Figure 5). Thus, withdrawal from intermittent access to highly palatable food increased 2-AG levels in the CeA.

Figure 3
figure 3

Effects of palatable diet alternation on (a) (left panel) anandamide (AEA) and (right panel) 2-arachidonoylglycerol (2-AG) levels (n=16), (b) CB1 receptor gene expression (n=6) and (c) protein expression (n=8) in the CeA. (d) Drawing of rat brain coronal slices used for mass spectrometry and quantitative RT-PCR. Circles show brain regions that were punched out. Rats were diet-cycled for 7 weeks, and CeA punches were collected during the 4th day of palatable food withdrawal (C Phase). 2-AG levels in the CeA of Chow/Palatable rats increased when palatable diet was withdrawn (day 4) and returned to chow-fed control levels upon renewing the access to the palatable diet (day 7). Values are expressed as % of Chow/Chow group (see Supplementary Figure 4 for absolute levels). Panels represent M±SEM. Symbols denote: *significant difference from Chow/Chow group p<0.05, **p<0.01.

PowerPoint slide

CB1 mRNA and Protein Expression in the CeA of Rats Withdrawn from Palatable Diet Cycling

Given the increased 2-AG content seen in the CeA during withdrawal from palatable food, we investigated the gene expression of CB1 receptor in the same area using quantitative real-time PCR in a different cohort of rats. As shown in Figure 3b, Chow/Palatable rats withdrawn from palatable food exhibited three-fold higher levels of CB1 mRNA expression in the CeA as compared with Chow/Chow rats (t(4)=3.55, p<0.02; Figure 3b). To confirm the results obtained with the mRNA expression, the CB1 protein levels in the CeA were also measured using western blotting. Figure 3c shows a significant increase in the CB1 receptor in Chow/Palatable rats withdrawn from palatable food (t(6)=2.77, p<0.03). Thus, withdrawal from intermittent access to highly palatable food, increased both the CB1 receptor gene and protein expression in the CeA.

Microinfusion of Rimonabant in the CeA Precipitates Anxiety-like Behavior in Rats Withdrawn from Palatable Diet Cycling

Next, we tested the hypothesis that rimonabant could site-specifically precipitate anxiogenic-like behavior when locally microinfused to interrupt the increased CeA CB1 signaling of rats withdrawn from palatable food. We bilaterally implanted diet-cycled rats with cannula targeting the CeA and then tested rats in the elevated plus-maze following microinfusion of rimonabant (0.5 μg/μl) during the 4th day of withdrawal. As shown in Figure 4a, intra-CeA rimonabant microinfusion reduced the percentage of time spent in the open arms, an anxiogenic-like effect, selectively in Chow/Palatable rats (F(1,36)=5.27, p<0.02). No significant group difference was detected in the number of closed arm entries, an index of locomotor activity (F(1,36)=0.26, n.s.). Confirming the specificity of the effects for CB1 receptors in the CeA, no effect was observed in the elevated plus-maze of subjects with misplaced cannulae (F(1,17)=0.02, n.s. and F(1,17)=1.87, n.s., % open arm time and closed arm entries, respectively. Therefore, site-specific antagonism of CB1 receptors in the CeA is sufficient to precipitate anxiety-like behavior in rats withdrawn from chronic, intermittent access to a highly palatable diet. The results support the hypothesis that the anxiogenic-like state elicited by systemic rimonabant in diet-cycled animals withdrawn from palatable food is mediated by the CeA.

Figure 4
figure 4

Effects of intra-CeA injection of rimonabant (30 min pretreatment, 0, 0.5, 1.5 μg/side) on anxiety-like behavior and food intake in diet-cycled female Wistar rats (n=46). (a) Rimonabant, microinfused within the CeA during the 4th day of withdrawal from palatable food (C Phase), selectively precipitates anxiety-like behavior, measured using an elevated plus-maze test, in Chow/Palatable rats. (Top panels) % open arm time, a measure of anxiety-like behavior; (bottom panels) number of closed arm entries, a measure of locomotor activity. (b) Rimonabant, microinfused into the CeA during the 4th day of withdrawal from palatable food (C Phase), selectively precipitates chow anorexia; in contrast, microinfusion during the 6th day (P Phase), does not affect intake of the highly palatable diet in Chow/Palatable rats. (c) Drawing of coronal rats’ brain slices. Dots represent the injection sites in the CeA included in the data analysis. Panels represent M±SEM. Symbols denote: *significant difference from vehicle condition p<0.05, **p<0.01.

PowerPoint slide

Microinfusion of Rimonabant in the CeA Precipitates Anorexia in Rats Withdrawn from Palatable Diet Cycling

We then tested the hypothesis that intra-CeA administration of rimonabant was sufficient to reproduce the precipitated anorectic effect of systemic rimonabant in withdrawn Chow/Palatable rats. Two-way ANOVA revealed a diet × treatment interaction (F(4,52)=4.39 p<0.003; Figure 4b). As hypothesized, the highest dose of rimonabant locally injected into the CeA (1.5 μg/side) selectively decreased chow intake in Chow/Palatable rats during withdrawal from palatable food, without altering the intake of Chow/Chow rats (Figure 4b). Furthermore, site-specific microinfusion of rimonabant within the CeA did not affect food intake in palatable diet-fed Chow/Palatable rats. Once again, analysis of subjects with misplaced cannulae showed no rimonabant effect on food intake (F(2,42)=0.94, n.s.), confirming the specificity of the effects for the CeA. The findings support the hypothesis that CeA CB1 receptors mediate the precipitated anorectic effects of rimonabant in rats withdrawn from chronic, intermittent access to a highly palatable diet.

DISCUSSION

The present study provides functional evidence that rimonabant precipitates a negative emotional state in rats withdrawn from chronic, intermittent access to a highly palatable diet and that blockade of CB1 receptor signaling in the CeA mediates these effects. Specifically, we demonstrate that systemic administration of rimonabant precipitates anxiety-like behavior and more potently induces anorexia and weight loss in rats during early protracted withdrawal from a palatable high-sucrose diet compared with chow-fed controls. In addition, at a time when diet-cycled rats no longer showed the increased spontaneous anxiety-like behavior elicited by acute withdrawal from this diet (Cottone et al, 2009a; Cottone et al, 2009b), they still exhibited increased vulnerability to the anxiogenic-like effects of rimonabant (Haller et al, 2004). These results are consistent with the hypothesis that rats withdrawn from chronic, palatable diet cycling become vulnerable to the pharmacological effects of rimonabant. Indeed, spontaneous or precipitated withdrawal from many drugs of abuse can induce not only anorexia and body weight loss (Aceto et al, 2001; Koga and Inukai, 1981; Tsuda et al, 1998) (Supplementary ref 22–28), but also anxiety-like behavior (Basso et al, 1999; George et al, 2007; Rodriguez de Fonseca et al, 1997; Schulteis et al, 1998). Also noteworthy, is the apparent discrepancy between the systemic lowest effective doses in the food intake and anxiety-like behavior experiments, which is very likely determined by the additional contribution of peripheral mechanisms to the anorectic effects of rimonabant (Di Marzo and Matias, 2005).

Interestingly, rimonabant treatment did not affect anxiety-like behavior in control rats, confirming the suggested role for CB1 receptors in modulating anxiety only in conditions of heightened stress (either external or internal; Haller et al, 2004). In addition, rimonabant-precipitated anxiety-like behavior in rats withdrawn from chronic, intermittent access to palatable food was independent of the degree of adrenocortical activation. This finding confirms the previous observation that the negative emotional state in rats withdrawn from chronic, intermittent access to a highly palatable diet is independent of HPA axis activation (Cottone et al, 2009a).

The increased sensitivity to the anxiogenic-like and anorectic effects of rimonabant in diet-cycled rats withdrawn from palatable food was accompanied by molecular evidence of increased 2-AG-CB1 signaling in the CeA, a brain area involved in emotional processing and in the emergence of negative affect during abstinence from substances of abuse (Cottone et al, 2009a; Koob and Volkow, 2010). Consistent with a key functional role for the CeA in mediating the rimonabant-induced negative emotional state, microinfusion of rimonabant into the CeA was sufficient to precipitate both anxiety-like behavior and chow anorexia selectively in diet-cycled rats withdrawn from the palatable diet and not in chow controls.

Interestingly, in this study, rimonabant failed to reduce compulsive eating upon renewed access to the highly palatable diet. The reduced effectiveness of CB1 receptor blockade on excessive palatable food intake is consistent with what we previously published (Parylak et al, 2012). Compulsive eating observed here is analogous to the behavior observed in other animal models of drug dependence, which is characterized by the relief of anxiety by performing the compulsive behavior (Cottone et al, 2009b; Iemolo et al, 2013; Koob, 2009; Koob and Volkow, 2010). However, it is important to mention that in preclinical research compulsive behavior is also defined as a behavior, which occurs in spite of negative consequences (Belin et al, 2008; Davis et al, 2010; Dore et al, 2013b; Johnson and Kenny, 2010).

Collectively, the biochemical and pharmacological data suggest that the 2-AG-CB1 signaling in the CeA is increased during abstinence from palatable food, perhaps as a compensatory mechanism to oppose the negative emotional state produced by withdrawal. Acute (5–9 h) withdrawal from chronic, intermittent access to the highly palatable diet used in the present study is known to produce a stress-like withdrawal state, which is characterized by anxiety-like behavior, hypophagia of the otherwise acceptable chow diet, and body weight loss (Cottone et al, 2009a; Cottone et al, 2008, 2009b; Iemolo et al, 2012). This palatable food withdrawal-dependent behavioral outcome is accompanied by the recruitment of the amygdaloid CRF-CRF1 stress system in the CeA (Cottone et al, 2009a; Koob and Volkow, 2010). Therefore, consistent with the proposed anxiolytic role of eCBs (Patel and Hillard, 2008), the neuroadaptive elevation of 2-AG-CB1 levels seen during early protracted withdrawal may serve to counteract the acute withdrawal-like state. Accordingly, blockade of CB1 signaling with rimonabant would prevent the compensatory, anxiolytic-like effects of 2-AG and, thereby, unmask the underlying anxiogenic-like withdrawal syndrome.

The hypothesis proposed here resembles the conceptualization of naloxone precipitated opiate withdrawal-like symptoms during abstinence (Rothwell et al, 2012) and the view that amygdaloid 2-AG is recruited during chronic stressful events to promote habituation to stress (Patel and Hillard, 2008). Indeed, repeated but not single homotypic restraint stress has been demonstrated to increase the amygdaloid 2-AG, consistent with a mechanism which opposes the behavioral and neuronal responses to aversive stimuli and contributes to the expression of habituation (Patel et al, 2005). Moreover, the present results are consistent with the ability of rimonabant to precipitate negative emotional behavior during chronic exposure to cannabinoids and opiates (Navarro et al, 2001; Rodriguez de Fonseca et al, 1997; Valverde et al, 2000). Indeed, rimonabant precipitated an opiate-like withdrawal syndrome as shown by increased somatic signs of withdrawal and increased place aversion in morphine-dependent rats, without inducing any intrinsic effect in non-dependent subjects (Navarro et al, 2001). In addition, rimonabant treatment precipitated somatic signs of withdrawal in mice made dependent on chronic administration of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (Valverde et al, 2000).

It is notable that, the role of CB1 antagonism in regulating anxiety-like behavior in relation to other neurotransmitters has been shown to vary as a function of the system studied. For example, although antagonism of CB1 receptor precipitates withdrawal from opiates and cannabinoids (Navarro et al, 2001; Rodriguez de Fonseca et al, 1997; Valverde et al, 2000), it has also been shown, at low doses, to reduce anxiety-like responses induced by chronic cocaine and CRF injections (Kupferschmidt et al, 2012b). Furthermore, although CB1 receptor antagonism blocked forced swim-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior (Vaughn et al, 2012), it did not affect reinstatement of cocaine seeking by footshock stress, an effect known to be mediated by CRF (De Vries et al, 2001; Kupferschmidt et al, 2012a).

Importantly, neither systemic nor intra-CeA administration of rimonabant potentiated anorexia when rats had ongoing access to the palatable diet. The lack of precipitated anorexia is consistent with the observation that elevations in 2-AG-CB1 levels were seen selectively during protracted withdrawal from the palatable diet and not when rats had ongoing access, further supporting the proposed mechanism of action. Rather, systemic rimonabant less potently reduced palatable food intake in diet-cycled rats. However, when rimonabant was administered during the first access to the palatable food, drug treatment was indeed able to decrease food intake and body weight in rats without a history of diet cycling, suggesting that chronic, intermittent access to palatable food attenuates the anorectic potency of CB1 antagonist to reduce palatable food intake.

In conclusion, our findings suggest an addiction-like, neuroadaptive functional mechanism for the emergence of severe psychiatric side-effects following rimonabant treatment in obese patients. We propose that rimonabant may precipitate a withdrawal-like syndrome in a subpopulation of obese individuals ‘withdrawing’ from palatable food as they attempt to lose weight (eg, by dieting). Rimonabant may do so by preventing the anti-stress/anti-withdrawal effects of heightened 2-AG-CB1 signaling in the CeA during early protracted withdrawal, leading to the emergence of negative emotional symptoms. The present findings support the hypothesis that recently developed, peripherally-restricted CB1 receptor antagonists/inverse agonists (Engeli, 2012) could be a suitable pharmacologic strategy to treat obesity, by avoiding the adverse CeA-mediated psychiatric side-effects of blood–brain barrier penetrating CB1 ligands like rimonabant.

FUNDING AND DISCLOSURE

This publication was made possible by grant numbers DA023680, DA030425, MH091945, MH093650, AA016731, AA06420, and DK070118 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA), the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA), and the National Institute on Diabetes, Digestive and Kidney Disorders, by the Peter Paul Career Development Professorship (P.C.) and by Boston University’s Undergraduate Research Opportunities Program (UROP). This research was also supported by the NIH Intramural Research Programs of the National Institute on Drug Abuse, and the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, NIH, DHHS. The authors declare no conflict of interest.