INTRODUCTION

Motivational salience of a stimulus refers to the property of capturing attention and driving behavior because of previous associations with reward or punishment. According to the aberrant-salience hypothesis, abnormal attribution of motivational salience is one of the main contributors to both positive and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Based on converging pharmacological and imaging findings, this theory states that the degradation of the signal-to-noise ratio in the striatum results in an impaired representation of motivational salience (Heinz and Schlagenhauf, 2010). As a consequence, patients fail to recognize the predictive property of a stimulus that has previously been associated with reward or punishment, and thus suffer from a reduced ability to discriminate between salient and neutral stimuli. This not only leads to misinterpretation of actually encountered stimuli (ie, false alarms), which might promote the formation of delusions, but also impairs the accurate anticipation of reward and punishment (ie, misses), which might be related to negative symptoms, such as anhedonia and blunted affect (Ziauddeen and Murray, 2010).

Cortical electrophysiological responses in humans and animals suggest the modulation of both early and late components of event-related potentials by emotional and motivationally salient stimuli, as demonstrated by studies using incentive or aversive conditioned stimuli (CS; Baas et al, 2002; Quirk et al, 1995; Tang et al, 2003). Early effects, such as the potentiation of the P1–N1 complex, are exogenously triggered and might reflect modality-specific tuning of the underlying sensory cortex, which allows enhanced encoding of the salient stimulus (Weinberger, 2004). Late components are associated with higher-order, supra-sensory processes. A frontal negative slow wave, which is referred to as stimulus preceding negativity (SPN), is associated with the anticipation of reward and punishment in humans (Brunia et al, 2011), and can thus be regarded as an electrophysiological index of motivational salience. No corresponding late component has been described in rodents so far. However, a cortical slow wave was reported in the auditory cortex of rats, which was interpreted as CS anticipation (Quirk et al, 1997).

This study investigates the electrophysiological correlates of motivational salience and their alteration by a schizophrenia-like state in mice. It has repeatedly been shown that electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings in mice show similarities to the human EEG, which makes it attractive to be used in animal models of diseases (Amann et al, 2010). To isolate the specific impact of motivational as compared with perceptual salience (change in stimulus characteristics, eg, 6 vs 9 kHz) on neural processes, a complex environment of salience was generated using an oddball paradigm. Perceptual salience was implemented as the pop-out effect of a rare tone embedded in a series of standard tones. Motivational salience was induced by previous coupling of the rare tone (CS) with an aversive event (foot shock; unconditioned stimulus, US; Figure 1). EEG was then recorded in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), which is defined as the prelimbic subdivision of the mouse prefrontal cortex (DeVito et al, 2010; Guldin et al, 1981). To investigate the cortical response to motivational salience as a function of time, recordings were performed immediately after conditioning and following a 4-week delay. In another condition, a subset of mice was additionally treated with the N-methyl-D-aspartic acid receptor (NMDAR) antagonist MK-801 to pharmacologically induce a schizophrenia-like state during EEG recordings (Kantrowitz and Javitt, 2010). We hypothesized that mice would show a similar EEG pattern compared with humans in response to salient stimuli and can thus be used as a model to study salience attribution in schizophrenia. More precisely, we expected a specific impact of motivationally salient stimuli on late auditory evoked potential (AEP) components, analogously to the human SPN response. We also anticipated that the induction of a schizophrenia-like state by NMDAR disruption should mimic aberrant salience processing in schizophrenia, with reduced response to motivationally salient stimuli, and aberrantly increased response to neutral stimuli.

Figure 1
figure 1

(a) Training and test sessions of the cued conditioning paradigm consisted of an acclimation phase, followed by a baseline phase, an event phase during which shocks and/or tones were delivered, and post-event phase. Mice assigned to the conditioned group (n=9) received tones paired with shocks in the morning session, but were placed into the chamber without any stimulation in the afternoon session. Nonconditioned mice (n=9) received only tones in the morning, and only shocks in the afternoon session. (b) Behavioral testing was performed in a modified context. For this purpose, grids were covered by an evenly textured, hard plastic floor. Both floor and walls were reconstructed by card-stock paper, transforming the chamber into a white-colored semi-circle. The olfactory context was changed using cotton balls soaked with orange concentrate (McCormick, Sparks, MD). (c) Perceptual salience was modeled as the pop-out effect of a deviant tone (9 kHz) in a series of standard tones (6 kHz). Previous aversive conditioning endowed the deviant with additional motivational salience.

PowerPoint slide

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Eighteen male C57BL/6Hsd (BL6) mice were obtained at 7–8 weeks of age from Jackson Labs (Bar Harbor, ME). After a 7-day acclimation phase, mice underwent surgery for stereotactic electrode implantation, and were single housed for the remainder of the study. All subjects were maintained in a standard 12 h light/dark cycle with free access to food and water. The experimental protocols were performed in accordance with the University Laboratory Animal Resources guidelines and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Experiments were conducted during the light phase from 0800 to 0000 hours and 1500 to 1900 hours.

Surgery

Electrode assemblies (PlasticsOne, Roanoke, VA) for non-anesthetized recording of AEPs were stereotactically implanted as previously reported (Amann et al, 2009). Unipolar recording electrodes (PlasticsOne) were placed into the right mPFC −2.3 mm anterior, 0.5 mm lateral, and 1.8 mm deep to Bregma. Negative and ground electrodes were placed in transversal direction (1.5 and 2.5 mm lateral, respectively) onto the surface of the ipsilateral neocortex (0.8 mm deep). Signals recorded with this electrode configuration will emphasis neocortical activity in the medial portion of the PFC (Franklin and Paxinos, 1997). The electrode pedestal was secured to the skull using ethyl cyanoacrylate (Loctite, Henkel, Germany) and dental cement (Ortho Jet, Lang Dental, Wheeling, Illinois). All animals were allowed to recover 1 week.

Cued Fear Conditioning

Behavioral training and test

The experimental protocol started on day 1, when mice were carefully handled and familiarized to the testing and recording environments on three consecutive days (day 1–3, Table 1). On days 4–6, conditioning was performed in ventilated, sound-attenuated chambers (Med Associates, St Albans, VT, USA), which consisted of a metal grid floor, two stainless steel side walls, and a Plexiglas door, back, and ceiling. Training was performed twice a day on day 4 and 6 (Table 1), with explicit pairing of tones and shocks in the experimental group, and explicit un-pairing in nonconditioned mice (Figure 1a). Auditory stimuli (n=90) were delivered by wall-mounted speakers and consisted of 9000 Hz tones of 10 ms duration and mean interstimulus interval (ISI) of 10 s (8.5–11.5 s, jittered in 500 ms steps). In the paired condition, tones were pseudorandomly interspersed with scrambled 65-mA foot shocks (n=9) of 500 ms duration, which were delivered 500 ms after the onset of the preceding tone. For nonconditioned mice, the only-tone and only-shock conditions consisted of identical sequences of either tones or shocks. Equal perception of shocks in both groups was verified by comparing freezing levels during and following presentation of shocks in the first training session on day 4 (p>0.05). On day 5, mice were tested for conditional freezing in response to the CS in a modified context, including different shape, color, texture, and smell of the chamber (Figure 1b). Each animal was tested in a different chamber than their individual training chamber, and exposed to the identical ‘only-tone’ session as used during training in nonconditioned mice. Mice were videotaped throughout all sessions, and freezing was assessed as percent time spent immobile.

Table 1 Experimental Protocol

Behavioral analysis

Freezing was quantified in naive mice before any stimulation (‘pre-shock baseline’), ie, baseline of session1, day 4, and in trained mice during the test session in the modified context prior (‘baseline’), during (‘event phase’), and following (‘post-event phase’) the presentation of the cue. Increased freezing to tones in the modified context reflects CS-dependent fear in conditioned mice. Freezing values were entered into a 2 × 2-way repeated measures analysis of variance (rmANOVA), with within-subject factor ‘time point’ (pre-shock baseline vs baseline vs event phase vs post-event phase) and between-subject factor ‘group’ (conditioned vs nonconditioned). Post-hoc tests were calculated as matched, two-sample t-tests for within-subject comparisons, and as independent samples t-tests for between-subject comparisons (only within conditions). Degrees of freedom are reported in subscript to each t statistic. The Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was applied, with αcrit resulting from division of α=0.05 by the number of calculated, independent tests. Effect sizes (Cohen’s d) were calculated for all post-hoc tests. Statistical calculations were performed with SPSS 17.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL).

Electrophysiological Recordings

Recording

Electrophysiological data were assessed before (day 2 and 3) and following fear conditioning (day 7–9, and after a 4-week delay, Table 1) to quantify effects of motivational salience on AEPs. Recordings were performed inside a Faraday electrical isolation cage with background white noise of 70 dB. Electrode pedestals were connected to a 30-cm tripolar electrode cable, which allowed unrestrained movement in the recording chamber. EEG recording and stimulus generation were controlled by Micro1401 hardware and Spike 5 software (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). EEG data were digitized at a rate of 1667 Hz with a 1–500-Hz bandpass filter. Tones were delivered through speakers attached to the cage top and the sound pressure was calibrated to 85 dB inside the cage. Each recording session was preceded by a 10-min acclimation phase.

Pre-exposure to the standard tones

During the morning session of day 2 and 3, mice were exposed to a series of 6 kHz tones (n=1000, duration=10 ms, ISI=500 ms). This pre-exposure was expected to induce latent inhibition, which prevents generalization of the conditioned fear response (Lubow, 1973). Thus, only deviant tones (ie, CS) should be associated with electric shocks (US) in the oddball recordings following fear conditioning. In addition, peak amplitudes of AEPs recorded on day 3 were compared between groups and equal auditory processing was confirmed (see supplementary material). All pre-exposure recordings were performed in the animal’s home cage.

Recording of AEPs during the oddball paradigm

The oddball paradigm consisted of a series of 10-ms 6 kHz standard tones interspersed with 10-ms 9 kHz deviant tones. The ISI between two consecutive tones was fixed at 500 ms and thus identical to the presentation rate of standards during the pre-exposure. In total, 70 deviant tones were presented at a mean interval of 10 s (8.5–11.5 s, jittered in 500 ms steps), which is identical to the presentation rate of the CS used during fear conditioning. On day 5 and 6, a subset of mice (n=12) were injected with either 0.25 mg/kg MK-801 (Sigma-Aldrich, St Louis, MO) dissolved in saline, or an equal amount of saline 10 min before recording. Injections of drug or saline were counter-balanced across groups and days. Owing to an expected drug-induced increase in EEG signal variance, the number of deviant tones was increased to 225. All oddball recordings were preformed in a neutral context (ie, not in the animal's home cage) to exclude potential contextual confounds.

Preprocessing of electrophysiological data and parameter extraction

EEG data were analyzed using EEGLAB (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). Preprocessing encompassed condition-wise epoching around stimulus onset at t=0 from −200 to 400 ms, baseline correction, and application of an individually adjusted epoch rejection criterion (ie, rejection of epochs surpassing two times the SD of the individual amplitude distribution). Regarding the oddball paradigm, only standards preceding a deviant were included to create averages with an equal number of trials in each condition. Owing to poor signal quality, three mice were excluded from EEG analysis of the oddball without pharmacological intervention (day 7 and 4 weeks later). Two mice were excluded from the oddball with pharmacological intervention (day 8 and 9). For AEP analysis, condition-wise average waveforms were calculated for each mouse, and amplitude and latency values were extracted as the minimum or maximum value for the P20 (10–30 ms), N40 (30–60 ms), P3 (50–200 ms), and the late negative component (late frontal negativity, (LFN); 200–400 ms).

Statistical analysis of electrophysiological data

To analyze the effects of salience as a function of time, each ERP measure obtained from the oddball paradigm without pharmacological intervention was entered into a 2 × 2 × 2-way rmANOVA, with within-subject factors ‘tone’ (deviant vs standard) and ‘time’ (EEG III vs EEG VI), and between-subject factor ‘group’ (conditioned vs nonconditioned). For analyzing the effect of NMDAR dysfunction on salience processing, ERP measures obtained from the oddball paradigm with pharmacological intervention were analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 2-way rmANOVA, with within-subject factors ‘tone’ (deviant vs standard) and ‘drug’ (MK-801 vs saline), and between-subject factor ‘group’ (conditioned vs nonconditioned). As group differences were hypothesized in response to the deviant tone, analyses focused on group × tone interactions. Post-hoc tests were performed as outlined above. All mean values are presented in Supplementary Table S1.

RESULTS

Freezing Behavior

Analysis of freezing revealed a highly significant group × time point interaction (F[3, 48]=17.120, p<0.001; Supplementary Figure S1). Post-hoc analyses (αcrit=0.003) confirm CS-induced freezing in the conditioned group, which is significant for the between-groups (conditioned vs nonconditioned: t[16]=3.934, p<0.001, d=1.94) as well as within-group comparisons (pre-shock baseline vs event phase: t[8]=7.582, p<0.001, d=3.25; baseline vs event phase: t[8]=9.486, p<0.001, d=3.02; post-event vs event phase: t[8]=12.840, p=<0.001, d=3.61). Crucially, nonconditioned mice demonstrated no differences in freezing across time points.

ERP Results—Salience as a Function of Time

An overview of rmANOVA results for all peaks is given in Table 2. A significant effect of motivational salience as indicated by a group × tone interaction was found for LFN amplitude (F[1,13]=20.231, p=0.001). In contrast, no peak yielded a three-way interaction of the factors ‘tone’, ‘group’, and ‘time’. Post-hoc tests (αcrit=0.012) were therefore calculated over peak values collapsed over both sessions, and corresponding waveforms are depicted in Figure 2. Amplitude of the LFN was significantly increased in conditioned mice subsequent to presentation of the deviant tone in both between- (t[13]=4.485, p=0.001, d=2.37) and within-group comparisons (t[7]=6.155, p<0.001, d=2.56). These differential effects were absent in nonconditioned mice (LFN amplitude: t[6]=0.875, p=0.42, d=0.43).

Table 2 Overview of Three-way rmANOVA Results Calculated for Each ERP Measure of the Oddball Paradigms with and without Pharmacological Intervention
Figure 2
figure 2

Coding of motivational salience by late ERP components. (a) Schematic depiction of the region for intracranial recording in the right mPFC (−2.3 mm anterior, 0.5 mm lateral and 1.8 mm deep to Bregma). (b) Average waveforms and (c) mean LFN peak amplitudes (±SD) in response to standard (std) and deviant (dev) tones (onset at t=0 ms) in conditioned (n=8) and nonconditioned mice (n=7) suggest a differential response to the deviant tone (b). Owing to jittering of the latency of individual mean amplitudes in the immediate and delay recording session, the grand average of the LFN amplitude is attenuated in the average waveform depiction. *pcorr<0.05

PowerPoint slide

ERP Results—Salience as a Function of NMDA Integrity

Following administration of MK-801, the effect of NMDAR-mediated glutamate disruption on salience attribution is revealed as three-way interaction of the factors ‘tone’, ‘drug’, and ‘group’. This effect was present for the amplitude of the LFN (F[1, 8]=8.508, p=0.019; Figure 3). A main effect of ‘drug’ was evident in all ERP measures except for P3 amplitude and N40 latency, indicating an overall adverse effect of NMDAR disruption (Table 2). Consistent with results of the former analysis, post-hoc comparisons (αcrit=0.004) on LFN amplitude following saline injection showed an increased response to the deviant in conditioned mice (t[4]=6.089, p=0.004, d=1.93), which, however, did not survive correction for multiple comparisons in the between-group comparison (t[8]=3.250, p=0.012, d=2.05). Selectivity for the deviant tone was abolished following injection of MK-801 (between: t[8]=1.190, p=0.268, d=0.7; within: t[4]=1.926, p=0.126, d=1.18). Within-group comparisons in nonconditioned mice indicated the emergence of an increased response to deviants after MK-801 (t[4]=2.720, p=0.053, d=2.01; not surviving correction for multiple comparisons), which mimicked the ‘normal’ response of conditioned mice after saline.

Figure 3
figure 3

Disruption of salience attribution by NMDAR antagonism. Average waveforms (a) in response to deviants (dev) and standards (std) in conditioned (n=5) and nonconditioned mice (n=5) following injection of saline or 0.25 mg/kg MK-801. Note the resemblance of the late component (LFN) in conditioned mice treated with saline (sal) and the LFN in nonconditioned mice treated with MK-801. Mean amplitude (±SD) of the LFN shows decreased discrimination between standard and deviant in conditioned mice, but aberrantly increased discrimination in nonconditioned mice following administration of MK-801 (drug). *puncorr<0.05

PowerPoint slide

DISCUSSION

In this study, the impact of motivational salience on electrophysiological measures was investigated in normal mice and in the MK-801 model of schizophrenia. Motivational salience was successfully generated using cued fear conditioning of an auditory stimulus. Subsequent prefrontal EEG recordings revealed selective modulation of the LFN, a late frontal slow wave, by motivational salience, an effect that was stable over a 4-week delay. NMDAR disruption following administration of the pharmacological agent MK-801 led to a disruption of the salience-specific LFN effect. More precisely, the LFN response in conditioned mice was highly reduced, leading to a loss of differential response to motivationally salient and neutral tones. In contrast, the LFN response in nonconditioned mice mimicked LFN responses of normal conditioned mice, with significantly increased LFN amplitudes to a motivationally neutral, but perceptually salient tone. Under the assumption that the LFN corresponds to an index of motivational salience, this pattern is consistent with the aberrant-salience hypothesis of schizophrenia, as the induction of a schizophrenia-like state resulted in ‘false-negatives’ and ‘false-positives’ in conditioned mice and nonconditioned mice, respectively.

LFN and Stimulus-preceding Negativity

Using an oddball paradigm with conditioned and nonconditioned deviant tones, we aimed at separating the effects of motivational salience from those of perceptual salience. While the latter primarily relies on bottom-up processing of perceptually outstanding features, the former mainly involves top–down processing and is inherently associated with the activation of appetitive or inhibitory motivational systems (Wynn et al, 2010). The present data are consistent with this notion as only a late frontal slow wave was modulated by motivational salience. The absence of a significant impact of time suggests that there is a robust memory for motivational salience, similar to what has been reported for conditioned responses in general (Bouton, 1994). Although direct comparisons across species are difficult, this slow wave bears intriguing resemblance to a cortical slow wave reported in threat-of-shock experiments in humans, in which subjects anticipated the possible occurrence of an electric shock (Baas et al, 2002). This so-called SPN is reliably elicited in anticipation of motivationally relevant stimuli, such as evocative photos (Poli et al, 2007), aversive noise (Kotani et al, 2001), monetary rewards, (Ohgami et al, 2006) and electrical shocks (Babiloni et al, 2007; Bocker et al, 2001). Cortical downstream targets of the ventral attention and reward system, including mPFC and ACC, have been suggested as generators of the human SPN (van Boxtel and Böcker, 2004). It has even been argued that the SPN is the ultimate cortical consequence of the proposed temporal difference error (TDE; Brunia et al, 2011), which is generated in tegmental neurons in response to motivationally salient stimuli (Schultz et al, 1997). Independent of its generators, the SPN is thought to represent the EEG-recorded manifestation of the lowering of threshold in wide-spread cortical areas to prepare processing of the anticipated reward or punishment (Brunia, 1993). Under the assumption that basic signaling principles associated with reward and motivation are shared across species, such as the TDE (Rolls et al, 2008; Schultz et al, 2000) or corticostriatolimbic circuitries (Berridge and Kringelbach, 2008; Haber and Knutson, 2010), the late negativity measured in the mPFC of conditioned mice might reflect analogous anticipatory processes in response to motivational salience.

Motivational Salience as a Function of NMDAR Signaling Integrity

Administration of NMDAR antagonists is a common approach to model schizophrenia-like symptoms in animals (Xi et al, 2009) and humans (Kantrowitz and Javitt, 2010). According to the glutamate hypothesis of schizophrenia, NMDAR hypofunction results in an imbalance of glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmission, which leads to wide-ranging sensory, behavioral, and cognitive disturbances. The mPFC has been shown to be one of the primary targets of systemic NMDAR antagonist administration in animals (Jackson et al, 2004) and humans (Breier et al, 1997), which seems to be mediated by subcortical thalamic and limbic structures (Kiss et al, 2011). Another study showed a widespread thalamic and cortical activation after NMDA-R blockade (Santana et al, 2011). In addition to thalamic inputs, NMDA-R blockade in PFC may also contribute (Homayoun and Moghaddam, 2007). The dysregulation of the prefrontal cortex interferes with various cortical-striatal circuits, which directly (Javitt, 2007) and indirectly (Murase et al, 1993; Sesack and Carr, 2002) affect dopamine signaling in the striatum, leading to disturbed salience attribution. Our results support the hypothesis that MK-801 alters processing of motivational salience as reflected by amplitude changes of the LFN, with attenuated responses to motivationally salient events in conditioned mice (‘false negatives’), and exaggerated responses to perceptually salient, but motivationally neutral events in nonconditioned mice (‘false positives’). This finding is all the more meaningful as it occurs on a background of an overall reduction of AEP amplitudes, which is a common effect of NMDA antagonists on auditory ERPs (Saunders et al, 2012). If the LFN represents anticipatory and preparatory processes, our findings imply that MK-801 ultimately led to inadequate anticipation of the aversive US, ie, aberrant salience attribution. This is consistent with observations of impaired anticipation of emotionally relevant stimuli in patients with schizophrenia. A well-known example is the dissociation between impaired anticipation of reward (ie, anticipatory pleasure) and relatively intact hedonic experience (ie, consummatory pleasure), a phenomenon that has been linked to anhedonia (Gard et al, 2006). Numerous imaging studies reported abnormal neural responses in the reward system of patients with schizophrenia during learning and reward anticipation, with blunted response to salient, and aberrantly increased response to neutral stimuli (Ziauddeen and Murray, 2010). The same applies to the limbic system and emotional stimuli (Habel et al, 2010; Seiferth et al, 2009). The functional significance of aberrant salience attribution is reflected by the correlation with positive (Roiser et al, 2009; Schlagenhauf et al, 2009) and negative (Waltz et al, 2009) symptoms, and the partial reversal by antipsychotic medication (Juckel et al, 2006). Complementary to these findings, EEG studies have shown intact early- and mid-latency ERP responses during affective processing, but a reduced SPN response in patients with schizophrenia during anticipation of reward or punishment (Horan et al, 2010; Wynn et al, 2010). An alternate explanation of the effects of MK-801 on processing of motivational salience is interference with mnemonic processes (Amann et al, 2009). In this study, injection of MK-801 might have led to an inability to retrieve salience information in conditioned mice, which resulted in a reduced LFN response. Although interference effects with memory retrieval are more difficult to reconcile with the finding of an increased LFN response in nonconditioned mice, mnemonic alterations might contribute to abnormal salience processing in our MK-801 treated mice, and more generally in patients with schizophrenia. The present results on motivational salience attribution in mice raise the question about their potential impact for gaining a better understanding of diverse symptoms relating to aberrant salience attribution in schizophrenia, such as anhedonia, motivational deficits, or delusional thinking. As our data suggest that the mouse LFN is an electrophysiological marker of motivational salience, comparable to the human SPN, animal models offer a possibility to study a wide range of environmental and genetic determinants of normal and disturbed salience attribution. These findings can in turn inform the development and evaluation of therapeutic strategies in schizophrenia for abnormalities in salience attribution, which is not addressed by current therapies as evidenced by persistent negative symptoms.

Limitations

A major limitation of this study is the small sample size of n=5 mice per group when investigating effects of MK-801 (ie, EEG recording with pharmacological intervention). Along with a relatively high dose of MK-801, effects might have been obscured by increased variance of the electrophysiological signal. However, as the reduction of statistical power bears the risk of type II errors, this drawback does not invalidate our main finding of a reversed SPN response in conditioned and nonconditioned mice. In addition, inbred mouse strains are highly homogenous, which allow the generalization of findings obtained in smaller samples. Finally, effect sizes reported in this study, which range from d=0.7 in between-group to d=2.05 in within-group comparisons for EEG recordings with MK-801, are comparable to, and even higher than effect sizes reported in human EEG studies. For instance, the comparison of P3 amplitude, which is another instance of long-latency endogenous AEP components, between patients with schizophrenia (n=23) and matched controls (n=22) showed an effect size of 0.63 for P3a and 1.26 for P3b, respectively (Turetsky et al, 2009). Similarly, the between-group comparison of SPN responses between patients with schizophrenia (n=34) and matched controls (n=36) yielded an effect size of 0.78 (Wynn et al, 2010).

A second limitation of the current findings is the lack of control conditions, which unambiguously link the LFN to a subsequent motivational stimulus, as it is the case for the human SPN. The waveforms suggest that the LFN response returns to zero at the time of the expected shock delivery (ie, 500 ms after stimulus onset). The use of increasing time intervals between CS and US might answer the question about the anticipatory nature in future studies. In addition, the use of stimuli, which have been associated with non-motivational events, might allow a better differentiation between motivational and non-motivational expectation (eg, Engel and Fries, 2010). However, even if the LFN is more a modulation of a CS evoked component than anticipatory to the US, it can still be interpreted as an electrophysiological signature of motivational salience in mice.

Conclusion

The combined interpretation of the presented data points to similarities between human and mouse electrophysiological coding of motivational salience. More precisely, a frontal negative slow wave bears temporal, functional, and pharmacological correspondence to the human SPN, and is suggestive for a mouse homology of human salience attribution. Although conclusions based on comparison between rodent EEG and human EEG must remain tentative in view of considerable differences between species, these similarities of phenotypes are not only attractive for further exploration of underlying mechanisms, but can also be used for the study and evaluation of potential therapeutic strategies in disease.