INTRODUCTION

Addiction is caused, in part, by powerful and long-lasting memories of the drug experience, and the long-term changes in chemical synaptic action in response to drug administration are believed to be candidate mechanisms (Hyman and Malenka, 2001; Hyman et al, 2006; Kauer and Malenka, 2007). It has been well documented that exposure to abused drugs can induce long-term changes in excitatory synapses. Emerging evidence indicates that exposure to opioids, by the activation of μ-opioid receptors (MORs) also induced long-term changes in inhibitory GABA (γ-aminobutyric acid)-releasing synapses (LTPGABA) on dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area (VTA), a brain region critical for reward and drug abuse (Nugent et al, 2007). The MORs in VTA are mostly expressed in GABAergic neurons (Mansour et al, 1995; Steffensen et al, 1998; Garzon and Pickel, 2001). Activation of MORs hyperpolarizes and inhibits VTA GABAergic neurons (Di Chiara and North, 1992; Johnson and North, 1992b; Margolis et al, 2003).

Recent electrophysiological evidence indicates that both in vivo and in vitro exposure to ethanol profoundly altered GABAergic transmission to dopamine neurons in the VTA (Gallegos et al, 1999; Melis et al, 2002; Xiao et al, 2007b; Theile et al, 2008, 2009; Xiao and Ye, 2008). Several lines of evidence indicate that ethanol's action on neurons in the VTA involves MORs. Ethanol enhances the release of β-endorphin, which activates MORs in the VTA and several other brain regions (Stein, 1993; Herz, 1997; Mendez et al, 2003; Marinelli et al, 2004; Lam et al, 2008; Jarjour et al, 2009). Selective MOR antagonists reduce ethanol consumption (Krishnan-Sarin et al, 1998; Kim et al, 2000; Hyytia and Kiianmaa, 2001; Margolis et al, 2008). Naloxone, a MOR antagonist strongly attenuated ethanol-induced inhibition of VTA GABAergic neurons and ethanol-induced excitation of VTA dopamine neurons (Xiao et al, 2007b; Xiao and Ye, 2008). However, there are no studies directly examining ethanol's effects on LTPGABA. The objective of current study was to determine whether in vitro and in vivo exposure to ethanol affects LTPGABA on dopamine neurons in the VTA.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

All experiments were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, and they were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey (Newark, NJ). The experiments were carried out on Sprague–Dawley rats.

Slice Preparation

The midbrain slices were prepared as described previously (Xiao and Ye, 2008). Briefly, Sprague–Dawley rats (21–35 days old) were anesthetized and then killed. Coronal midbrain slices (200–250 μm thick) were cut using a VF-200 slicer (Precisionary Instruments, Greenville, NC). They were prepared in an ice-cold glycerol-based artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) containing 250 mM glycerol, 1.6 mM KCl, 1.2 mM NaH2PO4, 1.2 mM MgCl2, 2.4 mM CaCl2, 25 mM NaHCO3, and 11 mM glucose, and saturated with 95%O2/5%CO2 (carbogen) (Ye et al, 2006). Slices were allowed to recover for at least 1 h in a holding chamber at 32°C in carbogen-saturated regular ACSF, which has the same composition as glycerol-based ACSF, except that glycerol was replaced by 125 mM NaCl.

Electrophysiological Recordings

Electrical signals were obtained in whole-cell configurations using MultiClamp 700A (Molecular Devices, Union City, CA), Digidata 1320A A/D converter (Molecular Devices), and pCLAMP 9.2 software (Molecular Devices). The patch electrodes had a resistance of 2–5 MΩ when filled with the pipette solution containing 125 mM KCl, 2.8 mM NaCl, 2 mM MgCl2, 0.6 mM EGTA, 10 mM HEPES, 2 mM ATP-Na, and 0.3 mM GTP-Na. The pH was adjusted to 7.2 with Tris buffer base and osmolarity to 300 mOsmol/l with sucrose. A single slice was transferred into a 0.4 ml recording chamber, where it was held down by a platinum ring. Warm carbogenated ACSF flowed through the bath (1.5–2.0 ml/min). All recordings were carried out at 32°C, maintained by an automatic temperature controller (Warner Instruments, Hamden, CT).

Under infrared video microscopy (E600FN; Nikon Corporation, Tokyo, Japan), the VTA was identified medial to the accessory optic tract and lateral to the fasciculus retroflexus. Currents were recorded using whole-cell mode. Experiments were carried out only after series resistance had stabilized. Series resistance and input resistance were monitored continuously online with a −4 mV hyperpolarizing step (50 ms), which was given after every afferent stimulus, and experiments were discarded if these values changed by 20% during the experiment. Dopamine neurons were identified by the presence of a large Ih current (Johnson and North, 1992b) that was assayed immediately after break-in, using a series of incremental 10 mV hyperpolarizing steps from a holding potential of −50 mV. Specifically, if the steady-state h-current was greater than 60 pA during a step from −50 to −100 mV, the neuron was considered a dopamine neuron. A recent study showed that expression of Ih alone is not sufficient to identify dopamine cells unequivocally (Margolis et al, 2006), however, refer the review by Chen et al (2008). Therefore, in each set of our experiments, a subset of the neurons recorded from and reported here are possibly nondopaminergic neurons (Nugent et al, 2009).

GABAergic IPSCs were recorded from the dopamine neurons in the presence of 6,7-dinitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (DNQX, 10 μM), an AMPA receptor antagonist, and strychnine (1 μM), a glycine receptor antagonist. Neurons were voltage clamped at a membrane potential of −70 mV except where noted. GABAAR-mediated IPSCs were stimulated at 0.1 Hz using a bipolar stainless steel-stimulating electrode placed 200–400 μm away from the recording site in VTA. LTPGABA was induced by stimulating afferents at 100 Hz for 1 s, the train was repeated twice, 20 s apart (high-frequency stimulation; HFS). After recording the baseline currents, during the drug application and washout, synaptic stimulation was stopped and the recorded neuron was taken from voltage-clamp into bridge mode. The HFS trains were also delivered under bridge mode.

Data were filtered at 1 kHz, sampled at 5 kHz, collected online using pCLAMP 9.2 software (Molecular Devices), stored on a computer, and analyzed offline. The amplitudes of IPSCs were calculated by taking the mean of a 2–4 ms window around the peak and comparing this with the mean of a 2–8 ms window immediately before the stimulation artifact using Clampfit 9.2 software (Molecular Devices).

Chemicals and Applications

Drugs were added to the superfusate at final concentrations. Most of the chemicals were obtained from Sigma-Aldrich Chemical Company (St Louis, MO). Ethanol (95% (v/v), prepared from grain and stored in glass bottles) was from Pharmco products (Brookfield, CT). Ethanol and other chemicals were applied to the recorded neurons at the stated concentrations through bath perfusion.

Data Analysis

Data are presented as means±SEM. Significance was determined using a Student's unpaired t-test with a significance level of P<0.05. Levels of LTP are reported as averaged IPSC amplitudes for 5 min just before LTP induction compared with averaged IPSC amplitudes during the 5 min period from 20 to 25 min after HFS using a Student's t-test. Paired-pulse ratios (PPRs) (50 ms inter-stimulus interval) were measured over 5 min epochs of 30 IPSCs each, as previously described (Ye et al, 2004; Xiao et al, 2007a).

In Vivo Ethanol Treatment

Rats (21–35 days old) were maintained on a 12 h light/dark cycle, and provided food and water ad libitum. For the groups receiving in vivo treatments, rats were injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) with either 2 g/kg ethanol or a comparable volume of saline, placed in a new cage for 2 h, and then returned to the home cage. They were killed for brain slice preparation, 24 h after injection.

RESULTS

In Vitro Exposure to Ethanol Blocks LTPGABA in VTA Dopamine Neurons by the Activation of MORs

Evoked IPSCs were recorded from VTA dopamine neurons in midbrain slices in the presence of DNQX (10 μM) and strychnine (1 μM) at a holding potential of −70 mV. The IPSCs recorded under these experimental conditions were blocked by 10 μM gabazine, a GABAA receptor antagonist (not illustrated), indicating that they were mediated by GABAA receptors. Consistent with previous reports (Nugent et al, 2007, 2009), HFS induced long-term potentiation of IPSC (LTPGABA, Figure 1a). LTPGABA was associated with a decrease in the PPR (Figure 1a, insets), suggesting that LTPGABA is maintained by persistently increased GABA release, which is probably caused by high-frequency firing of the presynaptic GABAergic afferents (Nugent et al, 2007).

Figure 1
figure 1

In vitro exposure to ethanol blocks LTPGABA in VTA dopamine neurons involving μ-opioid receptors. (a) LTPGABA in a dopamine neuron using whole-cell recording methods. High-frequency stimulation (HFS) was delivered at the point specified by the arrow. Inset: averaged IPSCs before (gray) and 20 min after HFS (black). In this and all other figures, 10 consecutive IPSCs from each condition were averaged for illustration. The dashed line in this and other figures is an extrapolation of the mean response before HFS. (b) Bath-applied ethanol blocks LTPGABA. After recording the baseline IPSCs in normal ACSF for 10 min, ethanol (EtOH, 40 mM) was bath applied for 15 min and then washed out (for 6 min) before the delivery of HFS. Inset: averaged IPSCs before (gray) and 20 min after HFS (black). (c) Average of 11 experiments from dopamine neurons (Control LTPGABA, open circles; ethanol cells, filled circles). (d) Normalized PPR at 20 min after HFS under a variety of experimental conditions. The PPR at 20 min was normalized to the pre-HFS PPR values. (*P<0.05, Student's t-test for PPR after HFS compared with pre-HFS values). (e) Bath-applied naloxone blocked the inhibition of LTPGABA induced by bath-applied EtOH. After recording the baseline IPSCs in normal ACSF for 10 min, naloxone (5 μM) was bath applied for 5 min before the application of the mixture containing naloxone (5 μM) and ethanol (40 mM) for 15 min. HFS was delivered 6 min after washout of the mixture (at the arrow). Inset: averaged IPSCs before (gray) and 20 min after HFS (black). (f) Average of 9 experiments from dopamine neurons (Naloxone+EtOH, open circles; EtOH alone, filled circles). (g) Bath-applied DAMGO blocks LTPGABA. After recording the baseline IPSCs in normal ACSF for 10 min, DAMGO (1 μM) was bath applied for 15 min and then washed out (for 6 min) before the delivery of HFS. Inset: averaged IPSCs before (gray) and 20 min after HFS (black). (h) Average of six experiments from dopamine neurons.

PowerPoint slide

We then examined the effects of ethanol on LTPGABA. Ethanol at a pharmacologically relevant concentration (40 mM) was bath applied for 15 min and then washed out for 6 min, which was followed with HFS. HFS failed to induce LTPGABA under such experimental conditions (Figure 1b and c). In 11 experiments, in the absence of ethanol, the averaged peak amplitude of IPSC (IPSC1) elicited by the first stimulus of the paired pulse measured 20–25 min after HFS was 347±57% of pre-HFS values (Figure 1c, open circles). After brief in vitro exposure to ethanol, it was 99±13% of pre-HFS values (Figure 1c, filled circles). Ethanol also reversed the changes in PPR (in ACSF, PPR=0.61±0.10 of pre-HFS values: from 1.17±0.13 in baseline to 0.71±0.14 after HFS; P<0.05; in ethanol, PPR=0.98±0.13 of pre-HFS values: from 1.23±0.14 in baseline to 1.20±0.13 after HFS, P>0.05; Figure 1d). As mentioned, much evidence links ethanol's effect on VTA neurons with MORs. To determine whether activation of MORs during ethanol exposure contributes to ethanol-induced blockade of LTPGABA, naloxone (5 μM), a MOR competitive antagonist, was bath applied for 5 min before the application of the mixture containing naloxone (5 μM) and ethanol (40 mM) for 15 min. HFS was delivered 6 min after washout of the mixture. Under these experimental conditions, the cells exhibited normal LTPGABA (Figure 1e). In nine experiments, the averaged peak amplitude of IPSC1 measured 20–25 min after HFS was 219±66% of pre-HFS values in cells exposed to ethanol plus naloxone (Figure 1f, open circles) vs 92±35% of pre-HFS values in ethanol alone (filled circles, Figure 1f). The PPR was 0.68±0.12 of pre-HFS values (from 1.22±0.14 in baseline to 0.83±0.12 after HFS) in cells exposed to ethanol plus naloxone. In further tests of the involvement of MORs, we applied the MOR agonist DAMGO ([D-Ala2, N-MePhe4, Gly-ol]-enkephalin; 1 μM) to the bath for 15 min. HFS was delivered 6 min after washout of the DAMGO solution. Under these experimental conditions, HFS failed to induce LTPGABA, consistent with a previous report (Nugent et al, 2007) (Figure 1g). In six experiments, the averaged peak amplitude of IPSC1 measured 20–25 min after HFS was 102±11% of pre-HFS values in cells exposed to DAMGO (Figure 1h). Together, these results provide strong support for the idea that suppression of LTPGABA by ethanol was mediated by MORs at the level of the presynaptic terminals.

In Vivo Exposure to Ethanol Blocks LTPGABA in VTA Dopamine Neurons by the Activation of MORs

A single in vivo ethanol exposure induced a long-lasting potentiation of VTA GABAergic synapses in VTA dopamine neurons of mice (Melis et al, 2002). We therefore asked whether in vivo exposure to ethanol also alters LTPGABA of rats. Sprague–Dawley rats (21–35 days old) were treated with either ethanol (2 g/kg, i.p.) or saline (i.p.). After HFS in slices 24 h after ethanol injection, there was no LTPGABA (Figure 2a). In 14 experiments, the averaged peak amplitude of IPSC1 was 93±21% of pre-HFS values (Figure 2b, filled circles; Figure 2e, EtOH, P>0.05 compared with pre-HFS values) and PPR was 1.02±0.18 of pre-HFS values (from 1.04±0.16 in baseline to 1.06±0.10 after HFS, Figure 2f, EtOH, P>0.05, compared with pre-HFS values). Conversely, slices from saline-treated animals exhibited normal LTPGABA. In six experiments, the averaged peak amplitude of IPSC1 was 266±35% of pre-HFS values (Figure 2b, open circles; Figure 2e, saline) and PPR was 0.65±0.13 of pre-HFS values (from 1.20±0.09 in baseline to 0.78±0.11 after HFS, P<0.05, Figure 2f, saline). Interestingly, the basal IPSCs from ethanol and saline-treated rats had indistinguishable PPR values (1.20±0.05 in saline-treated vs 1.09±0.05 in EtOH-treated rats, P=0.14; Figure 2g). These data provide direct evidence that inhibitory synapses in the VTA dopamine neurons can no longer be potentiated by HFS 24 h after a single in vivo exposure to ethanol.

Figure 2
figure 2

In vivo exposure to ethanol blocks LTPGABA involving the activation of MORs. (a) LTPGABA was blocked in dopamine neurons in slices prepared 24 h after a single in vivo ethanol exposure. Insets: averaged IPSCs before (gray) and 20 min after HFS (black). (b) Average of six experiments from slices prepared 24 h after either saline (open circles) or ethanol (filled circles) treatment. (c) In vivo exposure to naloxone prevents in vivo ethanol-induced blockade of LTPGABA in slices prepared 24 h later. Insets: averaged IPSCs before (gray) and 20 min after HFS (black). (d) Average of six experiments from slices prepared 24 h after ethanol (filled circles) or naloxone+ethanol (open circles) injection. (e) Bar chart illustrating the magnitude of LTPGABA 20 min after HFS in various experimental conditions (*P<0.05 compared with pre-HFS values). (f) Normalized Paired-pulse ratio (PPR) under a variety of experimental conditions (*P<0.05 compared with pre-HFS values). (g) PPRs are similar in slices 24 h after in vivo injection with either saline (open circles, n=14) or ethanol (filled circles, n=14) treatment. Each symbol represents the PPR value for one animal (mean basal PPR values (black horizontal bars) from saline-treated rats: 1.20±0.05, n=14; ethanol-treated rats: 1.09±0.05, n=14, P=0.14).

PowerPoint slide

We then tested the effects of naloxone. Naloxone (10 mg/kg, i.p.) was injected 15 min before ethanol (2 g/kg, i.p). After HFS in slices cut 24 h after naloxone plus ethanol treatment, the cells exhibited normal LTPGABA (Figure 2c). In nine experiments, the averaged peak amplitude of IPSC1 was 188±36% of pre-HFS values and PPR was 0.73±0.13 of pre-HFS values (from 1.11±0.21 in baseline to 0.81±0.16 after HFS, P<0.05; Figure 2d, e and f, Naloxone+EtOH). This result suggests that in vivo ethanol-induced suppression of LTPGABA was also mediated by MORs.

Sp-cAMPS and pCPT-cGMP Potentiate GABAA-Mediated IPSCs in Slices from Ethanol-Treated Rats

As mentioned, a single in vivo ethanol treatment significantly increases GABA release onto VTA dopamine neurons in slices of mice cut 24 h later (Melis et al, 2002). Thus, the failure of HFS to induce LTPGABA in slices prepared 24 h after in vivo ethanol treatment might be due to saturation of GABAergic synapses, a ceiling effect, which precludes further potentiation. To test this possibility, we examined the effects of Sp-cAMPS, a stable cAMP analog, which has been shown to increase GABAergic IPSCs in many different preparations, including the VTA (Bonci and Williams, 1997). Ethanol (2 g/kg, i.p.) was injected in rats and the brain slices were prepared 24 h later. In 16 experiments, bath-applied Sp-cAMPS (20 μM) further significantly increased the peak amplitude of IPSC1 (189±33% of pre-drug value; P<0.05) measured 20 min after application of Sp-cAMPS (Figure 3a, b and e). This was accompanied by a reduction of PPR (79±6% of pre-drug level, from 1.12±0.11 in pre-drug to 0.89±0.08 in Sp-cAMPS, P<0.05, Figure 3a, insets; Figure 3f, Sp-cAMPS). These data indicate that Sp-cAMPS act on the presynaptic site increasing GABA release, and that in vivo ethanol exposure did not maximally increase GABA release, instead, ethanol produced a long-lasting inability to generate LTPGABA.

Figure 3
figure 3

Sp-cAMPS and pCPT-cGMP further potentiate GABAergic IPSCs in slices prepared 24 h after in vivo ethanol treatment (2 g/kg, i.p.). (a) Bath-applied Sp-cAMPS (20 μM) increased the amplitude and reduced the paired-pulse ratio (PPR) of GABAergic IPSCs on dopamine neurons in slices from ethanol-treated rats. Insets: averaged IPSCs before (gray) and 20 min after Sp-cAMPS (black). (b) Average of 16 experiments from slices prepared 24 h after ethanol treatment. Bath-applied Sp-cAMPS (20 μM) significantly potentiated GABAergic IPSCs (P<0.05). (c) Bath-applied pCPT-cGMP (100 μM) increased the amplitude and reduced the PPR of GABAergic IPSCs on dopamine neurons in slices from ethanol-treated rats. Insets: averaged IPSCs before (gray) and 20 min after pCPT-cGMP (black) treatment. (d) Average of eight experiments from slices prepared 24 h after ethanol treatment. Bath-applied pCPT-cGMP (100 μM) increased the amplitude of GABAergic IPSCs. (e) The bar chart shows that at 20 min after the application of Sp-cAMPS, or pCPT-cGMP, the amplitude of IPSCs recorded from slices prepared 24 h after ethanol treatment was significantly increased (*P<0.05, compared with baseline before application of Sp-cAMPS, n=16, or pCPT-cGMP, n=8). (f) At 20 min after application of Sp-cAMPS, or pCPT-cGMP, the PPR of IPSCs recorded from slices prepared 24 h after ethanol treatment was significantly increased (*P<0.05, compared with baseline before application of Sp-cAMPS, n=16, or pCPT-cGMP, n=8).

PowerPoint slide

A previous study showed that bath-applied pCPT-cGMP, a cGMP analog, robustly increases GABAergic synaptic currents even after in vivo morphine exposure (Nugent et al, 2007). To determine whether cGMP-dependent protein kinases (PKG) signaling is involved in the blockade of LTPGABA induced by in vivo ethanol exposure, pCPT-cGMP was bath applied to slices prepared from rats 24 h after ethanol exposure (2 g/kg, i.p.). In eight experiments, pCPT-cGMP (100 μM) increased the peak amplitude of IPSC1 (165±27% of pre-drug value; P<0.05, Figure 3c, d and e) and reduced the PPR (83±7% of pre-drug values; from 1.13±0.13 in pre-drug to 0.94±0.15 after pCPT-cGMP, P>0.05, Figure 3c, insets; Figure 3e and f).

DISCUSSION

Our major finding is that exposure to ethanol, either in vivo or in vitro, blocks long-term potentiation of the GABAA-medicated synaptic transmission (LTPGABA) on dopamine neurons in the VTA of rat brains, and that ethanol-induced blockade involves the activation of MORs. We hypothesize that these neuroadaptations to ethanol provide a long-lasting mechanism by which ethanol enhances the excitability of dopamine neurons which may contribute to the reinforcing effect of ethanol.

Our recordings of HFS-induced LTPGABA on VTA dopamine neurons are in keeping with previous reports (Nugent et al, 2007, 2009; Nugent and Kauer, 2008). LTPGABA is heterosynaptic, requiring postsynaptic NMDA (N-methyl-D-aspartate) receptor activation at glutamate synapses, but resulting from increased GABA release at neighboring inhibitory nerve terminals (Nugent et al, 2007). An increased presynaptic GABA release in response to HFS was indicated by the significant reduction in the mean PPR of GABAergic synaptic currents associated with LTPGABA. The paired pulse stimulation is typically used as an electrophysiological protocol to test for changes in probability of transmitter release (Melis et al, 2002; Zucker and Regehr, 2002; Ye et al, 2004).

We then showed that a 15 min bath application of 40 mM ethanol before HFS prevented LTPGABA. There is much evidence that a change in GABAergic synaptic transmission is of great importance for the behavioral and cognitive effects of acute ethanol (Mihic, 1999; Siggins et al, 2005; Weiner and Valenzuela, 2006). However, previous in vitro studies of ethanol on IPSCs in several brain regions generated controversial results (Siggins et al, 2005; Weiner and Valenzuela, 2006). Our present study is the first to show that acute ethanol blocks LTPGABA, which is consistent with our recent work showing that acute ethanol decreases IPSCs (Xiao and Ye, 2008); however, see Theile et al (2008, 2009), who reported ethanol potentiation of IPSCs on VTA dopamine neurons. The cellular mechanism by which acute application of ethanol blocks LTPGABA is not yet clear. Ethanol could inhibit GABA neurons directly, for example, by activating K+ channels (such as μ-opioids (Johnson and North, 1992b)) or Cl channels (such as GABA, (Suzdak et al, 1986)), or indirectly through NMDA receptors (Stobbs et al, 2004). However, our data showed that ethanol-induced suppression of LTPGABA may involve MORs. This is in line with the evidence that acute i.p. injections of ethanol increase the extracellular levels of β-endorphins in the nucleus accumbens, in the VTA and in the central amygdala (Olive et al, 2001; Marinelli et al, 2004; Lam et al, 2008; Jarjour et al, 2009). In the VTA, MORs are expressed on the GABA neurons, but not on the dopamine neurons (Svingos et al, 2001). It is well documented that activation of MORs inhibits GABA neurons (Johnson and North, 1992a; Garzon and Pickel, 2001; Bergevin et al, 2002; Xiao et al, 2007b; Xiao and Ye, 2008; Chefer et al, 2009). Thus, the inhibition of GABA neurons by MORs may contribute to the inhibition of LTPGABA. This idea is further supported by the observation that a brief exposure to DAMGO prevents LTPGABA (Figure 1g and h, and Nugent et al, 2007). Numerous lines of evidence indicate that ethanol reinforcement mechanisms involve, at least partially, the ethanol-induced activation of the endogenous opioid system. Ethanol may alter opioidergic transmission at different levels, including the biosynthesis, release, and degradation of opioid peptides, as well as binding of endogenous ligands to opioid receptors (Mendez and Morales-Mulia, 2008). Our data suggest that a 15 min perfusion of ethanol in brain slices has altered opioidergic transmission. Future studies are warranted to determine which levels have been altered by the brief application of ethanol.

We further showed that a single in vivo ethanol exposure blocked LTPGABA in slices prepared 24 h later, and that ethanol-induced blockade was reversed by in vivo naloxone treatment, indicating again the involvement of activation of MORs. In vivo activation of MORs has been shown to inhibit LTPGABA (Nugent et al, 2007; Nugent and Kauer, 2008). These results suggest an important similarity between opiate action and that of ethanol.

Previous studies have shown that bath-applied cGMP or S-nitroso-N-acetylpenicillamine, the NO donor, significantly potentiated GABAergic IPSCs recorded from VTA dopamine neurons of rats even after in vivo morphine exposure (Nugent et al, 2007, 2009). In the current study, we showed that bath-applied pCPT-cGMP increased the peak amplitude of GABAergic IPSCs and reduced the PPR on slices prepared from rats 24 h after ethanol exposure. These data further suggest some of the similarities between the effects of morphine and ethanol. Interestingly, under our experimental conditions, the basal PPR of GABAA IPSCs in slices prepared from rats 24 h after in vivo ethanol exposure was indistinguishable from those treated with saline. Furthermore, bath-applied Sp-cAMPS (and pCPT-cGMP) further increased the peak amplitude of GABAergic IPSCs in slices of ethanol-treated rats, indicating that the GABAergic synapses are not saturated. Conversely, a previous study by Melis and colleagues showed that in slices prepared from mice 24 h after in vivo ethanol exposure, the PPR of GABAA IPSCs was significantly smaller than those treated with saline. In addition, forskolin, an activator of adenylyl cyclase failed to potentiate GABAergic IPSCs in slices of ethanol-treated mice, indicating that the GABAergic synapses are saturated (Melis et al, 2002). The mechanism underlying the apparent difference between our results and that of Melis's is unclear. A simplest explanation is that rats are different from mice regarding their responses to a in vivo ethanol treatment. In support of this possibility, a previous study has shown significant differences in ethanol response between mice and rats. Mice show rapid rise and elimination rates and higher peak blood ethanol concentrations. Rats show more gradual profiles, but retain the ethanol in their bloodstream for longer periods (Livy et al, 2003). Intriguingly, our data are consistent with a recent report that, although the cAMP-PKA system potentiates GABAA synapses, synaptically induced LTPGABA is independent of PKA activation (Nugent et al, 2009).

As mentioned in the Introduction, evidence implicating the endogenous opioid system in the development and maintenance of alcoholism is growing. The currently available literature suggests that ethanol increases opioid neurotransmission and that this activation is part of the mechanism responsible for its reinforcing effects (Oswald and Wand, 2004). In keeping with this idea, in the current study, we showed that naloxone, a MOR antagonist, reversed ethanol-induced block of LTPGABA. This is also consistent with our recent finding that ethanol suppressed VTA GABA neurons through the activation of MORs (Xiao et al, 2007b; Xiao and Ye, 2008). Our results show that similar to opioids, exposure to ethanol also blocks LTPGABA.

Acute ethanol exposure increases glutamate transmission to VTA dopamine neurons (Deng et al, 2009; Xiao et al, 2009). The loss of normal inhibitory control, coupled with potentiation of excitatory synapses, may represent neuroadaptations that increase the addictive properties of ethanol. Our current work provides an additional example of drug-induced adaptation of GABAergic synapses in response to in vivo and in vitro drug exposure. These findings support the hypothesis that changes in synaptic plasticity may be critical for the development of addiction. Uncovering the cellular mechanisms implicated in particular forms of synaptic plasticity in the VTA, a brain region related to addiction, could advance our knowledge of drug addiction.