Abstract
Increasing evidence suggests that the activation of medial A10 neurons mediates positive affective encoding. However, little is known about the functions of the inhibition of midbrain dopamine neurons. Here we show evidence suggesting that the inhibition of medial A10 neurons mediates a negative affective state, leading to negative affective encoding, whereas blunting the activation of medial A10 neurons disrupts positive affective encoding involving food reward. We used a microinjection procedure, in which the D2 dopamine receptor agonist quinpirole was administered into the cell body region of the dopamine neurons, a procedure that reduces dopamine cell firing. Microinjections of quinpirole into the posteromedial ventral tegmental area, but not its more lateral counterparts, led to conditioned place aversion. Quinpirole administration to this site also decreased food intake and basal dopamine concentration in the ventromedial striatum, a major projection area of medial A10 neurons. In addition, moderate quinpirole doses that did not lead to conditioned place aversion or disrupt food intake abolished food-conditioned place preference, suggesting that blunting dopamine impulse activity in response to food reward disrupts positive affective encoding in associated external stimuli. Our data support the hypothesis that activation of medial A10 dopamine neurons mediates a positive affective state, leading to positive affective encoding, while their inhibition mediates a negative affective state, leading to negative affective encoding. Together with previous findings, we propose that medial A10 neurons are an important component of the mechanism via which animals learn to avoid negative incentive stimuli.
Similar content being viewed by others
INTRODUCTION
Midbrain dopamine neurons localized adjacent to the brain's midline appear to play an important role in stimulus-incentive learning, in which an external stimulus that occurs closely in time and contingently with an incentive stimulus (eg food, drug administration, or any conditioned stimulus) acquires the same incentive-motivational (or affective) properties as those possessed by the incentive stimulus (Bolles, 1972; Bindra, 1978). This hypothesis is supported by converging evidence from electrophysiological, anatomical, and behavioral investigations.
Electrophysiological work, which has characterized how dopamine neurons respond to reward-related events, provides important insight on these neurons' functional roles. Recent data suggest that the impulse activities of midbrain dopamine neurons encode discrepancies between the affective event that is predicted and the affective event that actually occurs (Schultz et al, 1995, 1997; Montague et al, 1996). Importantly, such discrepancies (or reward prediction errors) have long been used to model associative learning including stimulus-incentive learning (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972; Mackintosh, 1975; Pearce and Hall, 1980; Sutton and Barto, 1981). However, to fully understand dopamine's functions, it is important to make a distinction between what dopaminergic signals reflect and how various target regions use such signals in adaptive behavior. Indeed, while dopamine neurons respond to reward-related events similarly regardless of their locations in A8, A9, and A10, dopaminergic signals have different functional consequences depending on their target region. Of those neurons projecting to the striatum, medial A10 neurons preferentially project to the ventromedial striatum, including medial accumbens shell, lateral A10 neurons to the ventrolateral striatum, including accumbens core (Ikemoto, 2007), and A9 neurons to the dorsal striatum (Fallon and Moore, 1978). Psychopharmacological and lesion data suggest that these projections are involved in different functions such as stimulus-outcome, response-outcome, and stimulus-response learning (Everitt and Robbins, 2005; Yin and Knowlton, 2006; Ikemoto, 2007). In particular, dopamine neurons projecting from the medial A10 to the ventromedial striatum appear to be important for the affective component of stimulus-outcome learning or stimulus-incentive learning.
Activation of medial A10 neurons appears to elicit positive affective states, which lead to stimulus-positive incentive learning (or positive affective encoding; Ikemoto, 2007). Local administration of psychomotor stimulants such as amphetamine or cocaine into the ventral striatum, or administration of drugs like carbachol or opiates into the A10 area, increases dopamine concentration in the ventral striatum (Carboni et al, 1989; Devine et al, 1993; Westerink et al, 1996) and somatomotor activity (Ikemoto, 2002; Zangen et al, 2002; Ikemoto et al, 2003) and leads to conditioned place preference (Carr and White, 1986; Ikemoto and Wise, 2002; Zangen et al, 2002; Ikemoto, 2003). Conditioned place preference has been shown to depend on stimulus-positive incentive learning (Perks and Clifton, 1997; Yin and Knowlton, 2002). In addition, rats learn to lever-press for administration of cocaine or amphetamine into the ventromedial striatum (Ikemoto, 2003; Ikemoto et al, 2005), and various other drugs into medial A10 (Ikemoto and Wise, 2002; Zangen et al, 2002, 2006; Rodd et al, 2004; Ikemoto et al, 2006). Further, blockade of dopamine receptors or lesions of dopaminergic terminals in the ventromedial striatum, but not the ventrolateral striatum, disrupts the establishment of conditioned place preference induced by systemic administration of cocaine, amphetamine, nicotine, or morphine (Sellings and Clarke, 2003; Fenu et al, 2006; Sellings et al, 2006a, 2006b; Spina et al, 2006).
Little is known about the functions of dopamine neuron inhibition. Midbrain dopamine neurons appear to be actively inhibited by aversive stimuli including noxious stimuli (Ungless et al, 2004), the absence of expected food rewards (or reward omission) (Waelti et al, 2001; Tobler et al, 2003; Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2007), drug withdrawal (Diana et al, 1993), conditioned stimuli associated with lithium-induced sickness (Mark et al, 1991), or reward omission (Waelti et al, 2001; Tobler et al, 2003; Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2007). These data are consistent with the idea that dopamine neuron inhibition encodes prediction errors involving negative incentive stimuli. If excitatory signals of medial A10 neurons are used to create a positive affective state, leading to stimulus-positive incentive learning, then the inhibitory signals of medial A10 neurons may elicit a negative affective state, leading to stimulus-negative incentive learning. This hypothesis is consistent with the finding that pairing external stimuli with high-dose injections of D1 receptor antagonists into the nucleus accumbens leads to conditioned aversion for paired stimuli (Shippenberg et al, 1991). In addition, conditioned place aversion results from systemic or intraventricular administration of high doses of dopamine receptor antagonists (Shippenberg et al, 1991) or a dopamine release inhibitor (Calcagnetti and Schechter, 1991; Schechter and Meechan, 1994).
The aims of the present study were to determine whether the inhibition of dopamine neurons leads to conditioned aversion and whether this effect is selective to medial A10, but not lateral A10 or A9, neurons. In addition, we examined whether moderate disruption of dopamine neuron activity leads to deficit in stimulus-positive incentive learning involving food. We tested these using pharmacological tools that stimulate somatodendritic autoreceptors and, in turn, inhibit basal firings of midbrain dopamine neurons (Aghajanian and Bunney, 1977; Beckstead et al, 2004).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Subjects
A total of 236 male Wistar rats (280–350 g at the time of surgery; Harlan, Dublin, VA) were used. They were housed individually after surgery and maintained under a reversed 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 9 p.m.). Colony and experimental rooms were kept at a constant temperature (21°C). Water and laboratory chow (Zeigler Rodent NIH-07 22.5–5, Zeigler Bros Inc., Gardners, PA) were freely available. Starting 7 days after surgery, however, each rat received a daily ration of 13 g chow, and this regimen was maintained till the end of the experiments. The experiments began after 5 days on this feeding regimen; the rats received food during or after daily experimental procedures. These procedures were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the National Institute on Drug Abuse Intramural Research Program and were in accordance with the guidelines of National Institutes of Health.
Surgery
Under sodium pentobarbital (31 mg/kg, i.p.) and chloral hydrate (142 mg/kg, i.p.) anesthesia, all rats were implanted with bilateral 24-gauge guide cannulas for microinjection in the ventral midbrain. Some rats were also implanted with a unilateral guide cannula for microdialysis probes (CMA/Microdialysis, North Chelmsford, MA) in the ventromedial striatum (medial nucleus accumbens shell). Guide tips for microinjection and microdialysis ended 1.0 and 2.0 mm, respectively, above the target sites. We studied two regions within the ventral tegmental area (VTA) because of previous functional studies (Ikemoto et al, 1997, 1998; Carlezon et al, 2000; Rodd-Henricks et al, 2000; Ikemoto and Wise, 2002; Zangen et al, 2002) that divided the VTA into the anterior and posterior portions. The present study divided the VTA into anterolateral and posteromedial portions, because recent neuron tracer data suggest that the cell bodies of dopaminergic neurons projecting to the ventral striatum distribute from the posteromedial to anterolateral dimensions at an approximate 45° angle to the anteroposterior axis (Ikemoto, 2007). In other words, the majority of neurons in the posteromedial VTA project to the ventromedial striatum, while the majority of anterolateral VTA neurons project to the ventrolateral striatum. Accordingly, guide cannulas for medial and lateral A10 injections were aimed at the posteromedial and anterolateral VTA, respectively. The coordinates were 5.8 mm posterior to bregma, 2.0 mm lateral to the midline, and 8.1 mm ventral to the skull surface with a 10° angle toward midline for the posteromedial VTA; 4.9 posterior, 2.3 lateral, and 7.9 ventral with a 10° angle for the anterolateral VTA; 4.8 posterior, 3.5 lateral, and 7.8 ventral with a 10° angle for the substantia nigra; 2.0 anterior, 1.2 lateral, and 5.3 ventral with a 0° angle for the ventromedial striatum. These surgeries were performed with the ‘flat-skull’ method, in which bregma and lambda were set at the same dorsal-ventral level. The experiments described below commenced 12 days after surgery.
Chemical Substances and Microinjection Procedure
The dopamine D2 agonist quinpirole hydrochloride and the antagonist S(−)-raclopride(+)-tartrate salt were purchased from Sigma (St Louis, MO) and were dissolved in artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) consisting of 148 mM NaCl, 2.7 mM KCl, 1.2 mM CaCl2, and 0.85 mM MgCl2 (pH adjusted to 6.5–7.8). One of the following doses and combinations of quinpirole and raclopride in a volume of 0.3 μl (per hemisphere) was injected into the ventral midbrain of each rat: 0.03, 0.1, 0.3, 1, and 3 μg quinpirole (per hemisphere) and the doses of quinpirole 0.3 and 3 μg mixed with 3 μg raclopride.
Injection cannulas, connected to 10-μl Hamilton syringes with polyethylene tubes, were bilaterally inserted into the guide cannulas. The rats were left unrestrained inside a 30-cm diameter cylinder, while a syringe pump delivered a volume of 0.3 μl into the target regions over 60 s. The injection cannulas were removed after an additional period of 30 s. Immediately after the microinjections, the rats were placed in the test chambers.
Place Conditioning, Food Intake, Somatomotor Activity, and Injection Treatments
Each of four place conditioning chambers consisted of two compartments (21 × 21 × 28 cm3) and an area (21 × 21 × 12 cm3) connecting the compartments; a guillotine door separated each compartment from the connecting area. Each compartment was equipped with four pairs of photocells, used to assess time spent and somatomotor activity in the compartment. One compartment differed from the other by wall color (black vs white), floor type (net vs grid), and lighting; the amount of light was modulated in each compartment so that the rats would not prefer one compartment to the other prior to place conditioning. In session 1, each rat was placed in the place-conditioning chamber for 15 min without any injections or food reward; the rat had access to both compartments and the time spent in each compartment was recorded. Place preference scores were obtained by subtracting the time spent in the compartment that would be associated with no food from the time spent in the compartment that would be associated with food. In sessions 2–5, each rat was confined in one of the two compartments for 30 min per session; one contained 13 g of the laboratory chow and the other contained no food. Rats were placed in the compartment containing food immediately after microinjections, whereas they were placed in the compartment containing no food when no injections were given. Their general activity levels were assessed by counting numbers of beam interruptions in each compartment. The amount of food consumed in the compartment was measured after each food session. Leftover food from the session was then placed in the home cage. The pairings of one compartment with food and the other with no food were alternatively repeated twice over four sessions. In session 6, each rat was placed in the chamber without any injections or food; the rat had access to both compartments and the time spent in each compartment was recorded for 15 min. Sessions were separated by 24 h. The order of these treatments and the assignment of the two compartments for treatments were counterbalanced among rats in each group.
Microdialysis Procedure
Concentrations of extracellular dopamine in the medial accumbens shell (a major target region of medial A10) were determined before and after food access as a function of VTA injections. Rats were food-restricted as described above and placed in a dialysis chamber (40 × 40 × 30 cm3) in which aCSF was perfused overnight through a dialysis probe. Then, four consecutive dialysate samples were collected in 15-min bins to determine basal levels of dopamine. Each rat then received bilateral injections of vehicle, 0.3 or 3 μg quinpirole into posteromedial VTA. Immediately after the injections, 13 g of food was placed in the chamber, and four additional dialysate samples were collected. A group of rats that received 3 μg quinpirole into posteromedial VTA did not receive food immediately after injections. For this group, two dialysate samples were collected before their receiving food, and then four additional samples were collected.
Dopamine was measured with HPLC coupled to the Coulochem II Detector (model 5200; ESA, Chelmsford, MA) with a dual-electrode microdialysis cell and an ESA model 501 data station. Samples were injected manually onto the column (3 μm particle size, 3 × 150 mm; Analytical MD-150; ESA). The mobile phase for dopamine separation consisted of 75 mM NaH2PO4, 1.5 mM N-1-octanesulfonic acid, 10 μM EDTA, and 8% acetonitrile (pH 3.0 adjusted with H3PO4). Dopamine was quantified on both reducing (−250 mV) and oxidizing (350 mV) electrodes. The limit of detection for dopamine was ∼5 fmol per injection.
Histology
When each rat completed the experimental procedure, it was deeply anesthetized with a mixture of sodium pentobarbital (31 mg/kg) and chloral hydrate (142 mg/kg) and its brain was removed and placed in a 10% formalin solution. Coronal sections (40 μm) at the microinjection site and microdialysis site were cut with a cryostat. Sections were subsequently stained with cresyl violet. The placements of the injection cannulas and dialysis probes were confirmed by microscopic examination.
Statistical Analyses
Details of the statistical analyses are provided in ‘Results’ section and figure legends. Generally, ANOVAs were used followed by appropriate post hoc tests. For food intake and somatomotor activity, mean scores of each rat from two sessions, taken after the microinjections, were processed for one-way between-subjects ANOVAs with six doses. Dialysis data were analyzed using raw values (nM), but not percent values.
RESULTS
Figure 1 depicts the injection sites of the rats examined for food place conditioning described in Figure 2. Each rat received bilateral injections into one of three sites: posteromedial VTA (medial A10), anterolateral VTA (lateral A10), or substantia nigra (A9). Regardless of the injection site, quinpirole injections reduced somatomotor activity (Figure 2a). This observation was confirmed by significant main dose effects (one-way between-subjects ANOVAs with six doses), F5,54=2.92, P<0.05; F5,53=8.227, P<0.0001; and F5,36=6.55, P<0.0005 for the posteromedial VTA, anterolateral VTA, and substantia nigra, respectively. It should be noted that motor activity levels in the other compartment, following no microinjection, did not differ among different dose groups receiving injections in any of the injection sites (data not shown). In addition, quinpirole injections were generally not effective in reducing the amount of food consumed (Figure 2b). An exception is that the highest dose (3 μg) of quinpirole injected into the posteromedial VTA reliably decreased food intake as revealed by a significant main effect of one-way ANOVA with six doses (F5,54=3.12, P<0.05), followed by a Dunnett's post hoc test.
Figure 2c shows place preference scores between before and after food conditioning as a function of quinpirole dose. Before conditioning, rats did not show reliable preference for one compartment over the other. After conditioning, those that received vehicle injections spent significantly more time in the compartment paired with food. Marked disruption of place conditioning was found following injections of quinpirole into the posteromedial VTA. Medium doses (0.1 and 0.3 μg) of quinpirole into the posteromedial VTA abolished food-conditioned place preference (Supplementary Figure S1a). Higher doses (3 μg) of quinpirole injected into the posteromedial VTA led to conditioned place aversion (Supplementary Figure S1b). These observations were confirmed by two-way mixed ANOVAs with dose (between-subjects factor; 6) and conditioning (within-subjects factor; before vs after). Injections into the posteromedial VTA had a significant dose-by-conditioning interaction (F5,54=4.79, P<0.01). To evaluate significant difference in place preference score between before and after conditioning at each dose, Bonferroni-corrected post hoc t-tests (six comparisons) were conducted. Significant differences were found at 0 and 3 μg, but not at other, in-between doses. Quinpirole injections into the anterolateral VTA or substantia nigra resulted in significant main conditioning effects (F1,53=10.18, P<0.005 and F1,36=51.86, P<0.0001, respectively), but not in a significant main dose effect or dose-by-conditioning interaction.
Additional experiments were conducted to help interpret the effects of quinpirole injections into the posteromedial VTA. The rats in the following experiments were treated exactly the same as those in the above experiment except that they had no food in either compartment during conditioning. The data involving the 0.3 μg dose in the experiment described above are consistent with the explanation that this dose disrupted stimulus-incentive learning involving food, because the dose completely disrupted the establishment of food-conditioned place preference, while it did not affect food intake. However, an alternative explanation is that administration of this dose may have been aversive, an effect that could counteract the positive affective state induced by food reward. Pairing a compartment containing no food with injections of 0.3 μg quinpirole did not lead to conditioned place aversion or preference (t9=0.74; Figure 3). In addition, injections of the 3 μg dose into the posteromedial VTA in the above experiment led to conditioned place aversion. The simplest explanation is that the administration of this dose elicited a negative affective state, thereby leading to conditioned place aversion and reduced food intake. However, the aversive effect of this dose could have resulted from an interaction with food intake; quinpirole injections may have altered perception of food, leading to an aversive effect. Again, pairing a compartment containing no food with injections of 3 μg quinpirole led to conditioned aversion to the paired compartment (t10=2.48, P<0.05).
The following two experiments support the hypothesis that the effects of posteromedial VTA quinpirole on place conditioning were mediated by dopamine neurons. First, we examined whether the effects of quinpirole on place conditioning were reversed by coadministration of the dopamine D2 receptor antagonist raclopride (3 μg). As shown in Figure 4a, coadministration of raclopride with quinpirole reversed the effects of quinpirole on place conditioning. This observation was confirmed by a three-way mixed ANOVA on place preference data including those shown in Figure 1c, with raclopride treatment (between-subjects factor; 0 and 3 μg), quinpirole treatment (between-subjects factor; 0, 0.3, and 3 μg), and conditioning (within-subjects factor; before and after). A significant three-way interaction between raclopride, quinpirole, and conditioning was found (F2,52=4.50, P<0.05; Figure 4a). It should be noted that injections of raclopride alone into the posteromedial VTA appeared to have an effect. Raclopride injections alone tended to disrupt food-conditioned place preference, as suggested by a large error bar at 0 μg quinpirole dose. Injections of raclopride alone, without quinpirole, reliably decreased food intake (Figure 4b). Interestingly, the combination of raclopride and the high dose of quinpirole did not have an additive effect on food intake. These observations on food intake were confirmed by a significant interaction between raclopride (0 and 3 μg) and quinpirole (0, 0.3, and 3 μg) in a two-way ANOVA on food intake (F2,50=4.66, P<0.05).
A microdialysis experiment was conducted not only to confirm that the quinpirole treatments modulated the activity of dopamine neurons, but also to determine how the quinpirole treatments affected extracellular levels of dopamine in the ventromedial striatum. The access to food reward after vehicle injections into the posteromedial VTA significantly increased extracellular dopamine levels in the ventromedial striatum by 75% over basal levels (Figure 5a). The injection of 0.3 μg quinpirole, however, prevented dopamine levels from increasing. On the other hand, injections of the high dose (3 μg) of quinpirole significantly decreased extracellular levels of dopamine. These observations were confirmed by a three-way mixed ANOVA on dopamine concentration with quinpirole dose (between-subjects factor; 0, 0.3, and 3 μg), food access (within-subjects factor; before and after), and block (within-subjects factor; 4). A significant interaction between quinpirole dose and food access was found (F2,20=14.39, P<0.0005). Consistent with the above findings, injections of the high dose (3 μg) decreased the amount of food consumed, whereas injections of the medium dose (0.3 μg) did not reliably decrease food intake (Figure 5b). This observation was confirmed by a significant dose effect on food intake (F2,20=5.01, P<0.05) by a one-way ANOVA followed by a Newman–Keuls post hoc test.
Because dialysis data obtained with the highest dose of quinpirole (3 μg) were confounded with the presentation of food, an additional experiment was conducted to determine if injections of 3 μg quinpirole alone without food reduce dopamine concentration in the ventromedial striatum lower than basal concentration. Injections of 3 μg quinpirole into the posteromedial VTA significantly decreased dopamine concentration as indicated by a significant main effect in phase (basal, postinjection without food, and postinjection with food; F2,8=10.28, P<0.01) in a two-way within-subjects ANOVA with phase (3) and block (two 15-min blocks within each phase), followed by a Newman–Keuls test (Figure 6). The access to food, which became available 30 min after the injections, did not significantly reverse decreased dopamine concentration induced by quinpirole injections. These rats consumed the mean food amount of 5.4 g (0.9, SEM) in 1-h food access started 30 min after injections. This value is comparable to the amount consumed following 3 μg injections in the above-described experiment.
DISCUSSION
We argue that the inhibition of dopamine neurons has active functional consequences. Specifically, our findings support the hypothesis that the inhibition of medial A10 neurons elicits a negative affective state, leading to negative affective encoding. In addition, blunting the activation of medial A10 neurons appears to disrupt the induction of a positive affective state involving food reward, leading to no affective encoding. In the following subsections, we first argue that medial A10 neurons rather than their lateral counterparts are closely involved in stimulus-incentive learning, and discuss how medial A10 neurons mediate stimulus-incentive learning. We then discuss the implications of our findings on food intake and aversive control of behavior, followed by their clinical implications.
Medial A10 Neurons are More Important for Stimulus-Incentive Learning than their Lateral Counterparts
Place preference scores were altered by lower doses of quinpirole injections into the posteromedial VTA than into its lateral counterparts, suggesting that quinpirole acts in the vicinity of the medial A10. It is also likely that injections of quinpirole into the anterolateral VTA or substantia nigra influenced dopamine neuron activity, since they decreased motor activity counts as effectively as injections into the posteromedial VTA. Although the electrophysiological observation that dopamine neurons across the mediolateral axis (A10, A9, and A8) respond similarly to reward-related events, these data, along with many previous findings, suggest that dopaminergic signals have different functional consequences depending on their target region. As we argue in the subsequent subsections, dopaminergic signals emitted by medial A10 neurons appear to be used for altering affective states, leading to stimulus-incentive learning. Although the present data do not shed new light on the roles played by lateral A10 and A9 neurons, previous findings suggest that they are important for other types of associative learning, including stimulus-response and action-outcome learning (Everitt and Robbins, 2005; Yin and Knowlton, 2006; Ikemoto, 2007).
How do Medial A10 Neurons Mediate Stimulus-Incentive Learning?
Converging evidence suggests that tonic activation of medial A10 neurons projecting to the ventromedial striatum elicits a positive affective state, leading to stimulus-positive incentive learning that associates external stimuli with an induced internal positive state (for an extensive review, see Section 4 of Ikemoto, 2007). Our present findings support this hypothesis. Medium doses of quinpirole, including 0.3 μg, prevented the establishment of food-conditioned place preference but did not reduce food intake, suggesting that the treatment effects on conditioning were not due to the lack of food intake. In addition, the 0.3 μg dose prevented tonic elevation of dopamine concentration in the ventromedial striatum following food reward. It should be noted that conditioned place preference not only depends on affective memories but also relational (or declarative) memories, which encode spatial relationships among external stimuli (eg compartments and food) and factual events (eg food intake in one of the compartments). While it may seem possible that medium doses of quinpirole into the medial A10 area impaired relational encoding, this hypothesis is not a viable one because the high dose (3 μg) of quinpirole injected into the same site led to conditioned place aversion, an effect that depends on relational memories.
The findings from the highest quinpirole dose support the hypothesis that tonic inhibition of medial A10 neurons elicits a negative affective state, which then leads to stimulus-negative incentive learning that associates external stimuli with an induced internal negative state. Pairing external stimuli with injections of the high 3 μg dose of quinpirole into the posteromedial VTA, in the presence of either food or no food, led to conditioned place aversion to paired stimuli. In addition, the finding that the administration of this dose into the medial A10 decreased food intake in hungry rats supports the interpretation that this treatment elicited a negative affective state. Further, with or without the presence of food, this manipulation significantly decreased extracellular dopamine in the ventromedial striatum. Thus, the inhibition of medial A10 neurons appeared to elicit a negative affective state, leading to negative affective encoding. This understanding is consistent with the previous finding that the blockade of accumbens dopamine receptors in opiate-dependent rats elicits somatic withdrawal symptoms (Harris and Aston-Jones, 1994) as well as the aforementioned finding that administration of high doses of dopamine antagonists into the nucleus accumbens leads to conditioned place aversion (Shippenberg et al, 1991).
Previous findings suggest that accumbens dopamine is involved in memory consolidation. Studies have shown that post-training injections of dopamine receptor agonists and antagonists into the nucleus accumbens, respectively, facilitate and disrupt associative learning involving both positive and negative incentive stimuli (Setlow and McGaugh, 1998; Dalley et al, 2005; LaLumiere et al, 2005). Our data suggest that rats can learn conditioned place aversion under severe inhibition of medial A10 neurons, bringing into question the idea that dopamine, at least in the terminal regions of these neurons, is required for or facilitates learning per se. Additional information is needed to understand how dopamine modulates learning and memory.
Implications on Food Intake
Increases in dopaminergic transmission or impulse activity after access to food do not appear to be important for regulating food intake. The administration of medium doses of quinpirole, including 0.3 μg, into the posteromedial VTA abolished increases in dopamine concentration induced by food, but had no detectable effect on food intake. However, food intake decreased after administration of the highest dose of quinpirole, which decreased dopamine concentration to below-baseline levels. Moreover, food intake decreased slightly after injections of a high dose of raclopride into the posteromedial VTA. Taken together, medial A10 neurons appear to participate in food intake in a subtle or indirect manner. These data are consistent with the hypothesis that mesolimbic dopamine neurons play more important roles in reward-seeking than reward-consummatory processes (Ikemoto and Panksepp, 1996, 1999; Baldo and Kelley, 2007).
Implications on Aversive Control of Behavior
Part of what makes aversive stimuli aversive may be their capacity to inhibit medial A10 neurons. Previous data suggest that dopamine neurons are inhibited by aversive stimuli, including foot shock, tail pinch, whole-body restraint, lithium-induced sickness, and conditioned stimuli predicting such aversive stimuli. Electrophysiological data (Mirenowicz and Schultz, 1996) suggest that aversive stimuli inhibit firing of midbrain dopamine neurons (for a review, see Ungless, 2004); unequivocal biochemical markers have confirmed that the neurons inhibited by noxious stimuli are dopaminergic (Ungless et al, 2004). In light of our finding that pharmacological inhibition of medial A10 neurons leads to conditioned aversion, the role of phasic inhibitions of medial A10 neurons in response to aversive stimuli may be to elicit negative affective states, leading to stimulus-negative incentive learning.
In addition, the omission of expected rewards or the stimulus predicting reward omission triggers phasic inhibitions of dopamine neurons and leads to conditioned suppression of responding in instrumental discrimination learning (Waelti et al, 2001; Tobler et al, 2003; Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2007). Indeed, behavioral work has long suggested that animals display similar affective reactions to the omission of expected rewards as to the presentation of noxious stimuli (Amsel, 1958; Wagner, 1969). In light of our data, we argue that stimuli predicting reward omission may encode negative affect via inhibitions of medial A10 neurons, a component of the mechanisms that enable animals not to respond upon the presentation of such stimuli. Recent data suggest that major inhibitory inputs to the midbrain dopamine neurons come from lateral habenula neurons that are activated by aversive events (Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2007).
Paradoxically, negative incentive stimuli not only decrease but also increase extracellular dopamine concentrations in the ventral striatum (Mark et al, 1991; Puglisi-Allegra et al, 1991; Diana et al, 1993; Young et al, 1993; Kalivas and Duffy, 1995; Bassareo et al, 2002). In addition, pharmacological disruption of dopaminergic activity such as blockade of dopamine receptors prevents the formation of conditioned responding induced by aversive stimuli (for a review, see Salamone, 1994). These data cast potential doubt on the notion that the activation of dopamine neurons mediates positive affective states and then positive affective encoding. Therefore, it is important to consider how the activation of dopaminergic systems triggered by aversive stimuli relates to the activation triggered by positive incentive stimuli. Aversive situations appear to be more complicated than appetitive situations, recruiting multiple motivational processes (Masterson and Crawford, 1982; Gray, 1987). Ikemoto and Panksepp (1999) suggested that increased accumbens dopamine transmission in response to aversive events is important for learning about the relationships between external stimuli and ‘safety’ from aversive events, ie a form of stimulus-positive incentive learning. In light of our data, there may be two types of dopamine-mediated avoidance behaviors: one mediated by inhibition of dopamine systems (withdrawal-type avoidance) and the other mediated by activation of dopamine systems (approach-type avoidance). We speculate that inhibition of medial A10 neurons in response to aversive events is involved in stimulus-negative incentive learning and conditioned withdrawal, while their activation is involved in avoidance learning that associates external stimuli with safety from aversive stimuli and conditioned approach.
This hypothesis is not mutually exclusive with one pharmacological explanation. Disruption of aversive learning by inactivation of the dopamine is explained if pharmacological manipulations, such as administration of dopamine receptor antagonists into the nucleus accumbens or D2 agonists into the VTA, blunt not only phasic activation dopamine signals for stimulus-positive incentive learning, but also phasic inhibitory dopamine signals for stimulus-negative incentive learning. Microinjections of quinpirole into the VTA lead to the disruption of second-order associative learning between neutral stimuli and conditioned stimuli previously paired with foot shock (Nader and LeDoux, 1999). This effect does not appear to result from secondary effects of the aversive state induced by intra-VTA quinpirole, because these experiments employed a medium quinpirole dose range (0.1–1.0 μg), which did not induce conditioned place aversion (but did disrupt food-conditioned place preference) in our study. Similarly, low-dose injections (25–50 ng per side) of SCH23390 into the ventromedial striatum disrupt stimulus-negative incentive learning between lithium-induced sickness and taste stimuli associated with sickness (Fenu et al, 2001), whereas higher dose injections (500–1000 ng per side) of SCH23390 into the nucleus accumbens are needed to lead to conditioned place aversion (Shippenberg et al, 1991). Therefore, if lower doses of quinpirole administered into the VTA or lower doses of SCH23390 administered into the ventromedial striatum blunt phasic inhibitory signals of dopamine neurons in response to aversive stimuli, this explains why such pharmacological manipulations are so effective in disrupting behavior controlled by aversive stimuli.
Clinical Implications
The loss of dopaminergic neurons in the ventral midbrain leads to devastating movement disorders including Parkinson's disease. Intriguingly, electrophysiological findings suggest that midbrain dopamine neurons respond to incentive stimuli rather than movement (Schultz, 2002). Many investigators now believe that these neurons play an important role in motor learning in relation to external events (Hikosaka, 1991; Wickens et al, 2003; Graybiel, 2005). In light of our data, patients with the loss of medial A10 dopamine neurons, which may be prevalent in Parkinson's patients (Uhl, 1985), may have affective encoding deficits in which external stimuli no longer acquire appropriate motivational signals for initiating movements. Therefore, the loss of medial A10 neurons could contribute to movement-related disorders. This idea fits with the suggestion that motivational signals processed by the ventromedial portion of the basal ganglia interact with motor signals processed by the dorsolateral counterpart for adaptive behavior (Nauta and Domesick, 1978; Mogenson et al, 1980).
In addition, the therapeutic effects of psychoactive drug treatments for mood disorders and schizophrenia emerge gradually over days and weeks, despite the fact that these drugs exert full pharmacological effects only hours after administration. If it is correct to assume that a set of brain mechanisms including medial A10 neurons mediates affective encoding, the delay in improvement of affective symptoms following administration of psychoactive drugs partly reflects the time needed for individuals to re-acquire adequate affective codes in their central representation of external stimuli and events, which, in turn, control their affect, thoughts, and behavior.
References
Aghajanian GK, Bunney BS (1977). Pharmacological characterization of dopamine ‘autoreceptors’ by microiontophoretic single-cell recording studies. In: Costa E, Gessa GL (eds). Adv Biochem Psychopharmacol. Raven Press: New York. Vol 16, pp 433–438.
Amsel A (1958). The role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuous reward situations. Psychol Bull 55: 102–119.
Baldo BA, Kelley AE (2007). Discrete neurochemical coding of distinguishable motivational processes: insights from nucleus accumbens control of feeding. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 191: 439–459.
Bassareo V, De Luca MA, Di Chiara G (2002). Differential expression of motivational stimulus properties by dopamine in nucleus accumbens shell vs core and prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 22: 4709–4719.
Beckstead MJ, Grandy DK, Wickman K, Williams JT (2004). Vesicular dopamine release elicits an inhibitory postsynaptic current in midbrain dopamine neurons. Neuron 42: 939–946.
Bindra D (1978). How adaptive behavior is produced: a perceptual-motivational alternative to response-reinforcement. Behav Brain Sci 1: 41–91.
Bolles RC (1972). Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning. Psychol Rev 79: 394–409.
Calcagnetti DJ, Schechter MD (1991). Conditioned place aversion following the central administration of a novel dopamine release inhibitor CGS 10746B. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 40: 255–259.
Carboni E, Imperato A, Perezzani L, Di Chiara G (1989). Amphetamine, cocaine, phencyclidine and nomifensine increases extracellular dopamine concentrations preferentially in the nucleus accumbens of freely moving rats. Neuroscience 28: 653–661.
Carlezon Jr WA, Haile CN, Coppersmith R, Hayashi Y, Malinow R, Neve RL et al (2000). Distinct sites of opiate reward and aversion within the midbrain identified using a herpes simplex virus vector expressing GluR1. J Neurosci 20: RC62.
Carr GD, White NM (1986). Anatomical disassociation of amphetamine's rewarding and aversive effects: an intracranial microinjection study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 89: 340–346.
Dalley JW, Laane K, Theobald DE, Armstrong HC, Corlett PR, Chudasama Y et al (2005). Time-limited modulation of appetitive Pavlovian memory by D1 and NMDA receptors in the nucleus accumbens. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102: 6189–6194.
Devine DP, Leone P, Wise RA (1993). Mesolimbic dopamine neurotransmission is increased by administration of mu-opioid receptor antagonists. Eur J Pharmacol 243: 55–64.
Diana M, Pistis M, Carboni S, Gessa GL, Rossetti ZL (1993). Profound decrement of mesolimbic dopaminergic neuronal activity during ethanol withdrawal syndrome in rats: electrophysiological and biochemical evidence. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90: 7966–7969.
Everitt BJ, Robbins TW (2005). Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci 8: 1481–1489.
Fallon JH, Moore RY (1978). Catecholamine innervation of the basal forebrain. IV. Topography of the dopamine projection to the basal forebrain and neostriatum. J Comp Neurol 180: 545–580.
Fenu S, Bassareo V, Di Chiara G (2001). A role for dopamine D1 receptors of the nucleus accumbens shell in conditioned taste aversion learning. J Neurosci 21: 6897–6904.
Fenu S, Spina L, Rivas E, Longoni R, Di Chiara G (2006). Morphine-conditioned single-trial place preference: role of nucleus accumbens shell dopamine receptors in acquisition, but not expression. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 187: 143–153.
Gray JA (1987). The Psychology of Fear and Stress, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge. 422pp.
Graybiel AM (2005). The basal ganglia: learning new tricks and loving it. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15: 638–644.
Harris GC, Aston-Jones G (1994). Involvement of D2 dopamine receptors in the nucleus accumbens in the opiate withdrawal syndrome. Nature 371: 155–157.
Hikosaka O (1991). Basal ganglia—possible role in motor coordination and learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1: 638–643.
Ikemoto S (2002). Ventral striatal anatomy of locomotor activity induced by cocaine, d-amphetamine, dopamine and D1/D2 agonists. Neuroscience 113: 939–955.
Ikemoto S (2003). Involvement of the olfactory tubercle in cocaine reward: intracranial self-administration studies. J Neurosci 23: 9305–9311.
Ikemoto S (2007). Dopamine reward circuitry: two projection systems from the ventral midbrain to the nucleus accumbens-olfactory tubercle complex. Brain Res Rev 56: 27–78.
Ikemoto S, Murphy JM, McBride WJ (1997). Self-infusion of GABAA antagonists directly into the ventral tegmental area and adjacent regions. Behav Neurosci 111: 369–380.
Ikemoto S, Murphy JM, McBride WJ (1998). Regional differences within the rat ventral tegmental area for muscimol self-infusions. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 61: 87–92.
Ikemoto S, Panksepp J (1996). Dissociations between appetitive and consummatory responses by pharmacological manipulations of reward-relevant brain regions. Behav Neurosci 110: 331–345.
Ikemoto S, Panksepp J (1999). The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: a unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Res Rev 31: 6–41.
Ikemoto S, Qin M, Liu ZH (2005). The functional divide for primary reinforcement of D-amphetamine lies between the medial and lateral ventral striatum: is the division of the accumbens core, shell and olfactory tubercle valid? J Neurosci 25: 5061–5065.
Ikemoto S, Qin M, Liu ZH (2006). Primary reinforcing effects of nicotine are triggered from multiple regions both inside and outside the ventral tegmental area. J Neurosci 26: 723–730.
Ikemoto S, Wise RA (2002). Rewarding effects of the cholinergic agents carbachol and neostigmine in the posterior ventral tegmental area. J Neurosci 22: 9895–9904.
Ikemoto S, Witkin BM, Morales M (2003). Rewarding injections of the cholinergic agonist carbachol into the ventral tegmental area induce locomotion and c-Fos expression in the retrosplenial area and supramammillary nucleus. Brain Res 969: 78–87.
Kalivas PW, Duffy P (1995). Selective activation of dopamine transmission in the shell of the nucleus accumbens by stress. Brain Res 675: 325–328.
LaLumiere RT, Nawar EM, McGaugh JL (2005). Modulation of memory consolidation by the basolateral amygdala or nucleus accumbens shell requires concurrent dopamine receptor activation in both brain regions. Learn Mem 12: 296–301.
Mackintosh NJ (1975). A theory of attention: variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychol Rev 82: 276–298.
Mark GP, Blander DS, Hoebel BG (1991). A conditioned stimulus decreases extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens after the development of a learned taste aversion. Brain Res 551: 308–310.
Masterson FA, Crawford M (1982). The defense motivation system: a theory of avoidance behavior. Behav Brain Sci 5: 661–696.
Matsumoto M, Hikosaka O (2007). Lateral habenula as a source of negative reward signals in dopamine neurons. Nature 447: 1111–1115.
Mirenowicz J, Schultz W (1996). Preferential activation of midbrain dopamine neurons by appetitive rather than aversive stimuli. Nature 379: 449–451.
Mogenson G, Jones D, Yim CY (1980). From motivation to action: functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system. Prog Neurobiol 14: 69–97.
Montague PR, Dayan P, Sejnowski TJ (1996). A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning. J Neurosci 16: 1936–1947.
Nader K, LeDoux JE (1999). Inhibition of the mesoamygdala dopaminergic pathway impairs the retrieval of conditioned fear associations. Behav Neurosci 113: 891–901.
Nauta WJH, Domesick VB (1978). Crossroads of limbic and striatal circuitry: hypothalamo-nigral connections. In: Livingston KE, Hornykiewicz O (eds). Limbic Mechanisms: The Continuing Evolution of the Limbic System Concept. Plenum: New York, pp 75–93.
Paxinos G, Watson C (1997). The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, 3rd edn. Academic Press: San Diego. 78pp.
Pearce JM, Hall G (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychol Rev 87: 532–552.
Perks SM, Clifton PG (1997). Reinforcer revaluation and conditioned place preference. Physiol Behav 61: 1–5.
Puglisi-Allegra S, Imperato A, Angelucci L, Cabib S (1991). Acute stress induces time-dependent responses in dopamine mesolimbic system. Brain Res 554: 217–222.
Rescorla RA, Wagner AR (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Black AH, Prokasy WF (eds). Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York. pp 64–99.
Rodd ZA, Melendez RI, Bell RL, Kuc KA, Zhang Y, Murphy JM et al (2004). Intracranial self-administration of ethanol within the ventral tegmental area of male Wistar rats: evidence for involvement of dopamine neurons. J Neurosci 24: 1050–1057.
Rodd-Henricks ZA, McKinzie DL, Crile RS, Murphy JM, McBride WJ (2000). Regional heterogeneity for the intracranial self-administration of ethanol within the ventral tegmental area of female Wistar rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 149: 217–224.
Salamone JD (1994). The involvement of nucleus accumbens dopamine in appetitive and aversive motivation. Behav Brain Res 61: 117–133.
Schechter MD, Meechan SM (1994). Conditioned place aversion produced by dopamine release inhibition. Eur J Pharmacol 260: 133–137.
Schultz W (2002). Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron 36: 241–263.
Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275: 1593–1599.
Schultz W, Romo R, Ljungberg T, Mirenowicz J, Hollerman JR, Dickinson A (1995). Reward-related signals carried by dopamine neurons. In: Houk JR, Davis JL, Beiser D (eds). Models of Information Processing in the Basal Ganglia. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, pp 233–248.
Sellings LH, Clarke PB (2003). Segregation of amphetamine reward and locomotor stimulation between nucleus accumbens medial shell and core. J Neurosci 23: 6295–6303.
Sellings LH, McQuade LE, Clarke PB (2006a). Characterization of dopamine-dependent rewarding and locomotor stimulant effects of intravenously-administered methylphenidate in rats. Neuroscience 141: 1457–1468.
Sellings LH, McQuade LE, Clarke PB (2006b). Evidence for multiple sites within rat ventral striatum mediating cocaine conditioned place preference and locomotor activation. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 317: 1178–1187.
Setlow B, McGaugh JL (1998). Sulpiride infused into the nucleus accumbens posttraining impairs memory of spatial water maze training. Behav Neurosci 112: 603–610.
Shippenberg TTS, Bals-Kubik RR, Huber AA, Herz AA (1991). Neuroanatomical substrates mediating the aversive effects of D-1 dopamine receptor antagonists. Psychopharmacology 103: 209–214.
Spina L, Fenu S, Longoni R, Rivas E, Di Chiara G (2006). Nicotine-conditioned single-trial place preference: selective role of nucleus accumbens shell dopamine D1 receptors in acquisition. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 184: 447–455.
Sutton RS, Barto AG (1981). Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: expectation and prediction. Psychol Rev 88: 135–170.
Tobler PN, Dickinson A, Schultz W (2003). Coding of predicted reward omission by dopamine neurons in a conditioned inhibition paradigm. J Neurosci 23: 10402–10410.
Uhl GR (1985). Parkinson's disease: loss of neurons from the ventral tegmental area contralateral to therapeutic surgical lesions. Neurology 35: 1215–1218.
Ungless MA (2004). Dopamine: the salient issue. Trends Neurosci 27: 702–706.
Ungless MA, Magill PJ, Bolam JP (2004). Uniform inhibition of dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area by aversive stimuli. Science 303: 2040–2042.
Waelti P, Dickinson A, Schultz W (2001). Dopamine responses comply with basic assumptions of formal learning theory. Nature 412: 43–48.
Wagner AR (1969). Frustrative nonreward: a variety of punishment. In: Cambell BA, Church RM (eds). Punishment and Aversive Behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, pp 157–181.
Westerink BH, Kwint HF, deVries JB (1996). The pharmacology of mesolimbic dopamine neurons: a dual-probe microdialysis study in the ventral tegmental area and nucleus accumbens of the rat brain. J Neurosci 16: 2605–2611.
Wickens JR, Reynolds JN, Hyland BI (2003). Neural mechanisms of reward-related motor learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13: 685–690.
Yin HH, Knowlton BJ (2002). Reinforcer devaluation abolishes conditioned cue preference: evidence for stimulus–stimulus associations. Behav Neurosci 116: 174–177.
Yin HH, Knowlton BJ (2006). The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nat Rev Neurosci 7: 464–476.
Young AMJ, Joseph MH, Gray JA (1993). Latent inhibition of conditioned dopamine release in rat nucleus accumbens. Neuroscience 54: 5–9.
Zangen A, Ikemoto S, Zadina JE, Wise RA (2002). Rewarding and psychomotor stimulant effects of endomorphin-1: anteroposterior differences within the ventral tegmental area and lack of effect in nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci 22: 7225–7233.
Zangen A, Solinas M, Ikemoto S, Goldberg SR, Wise RA (2006). Two brain sites for cannabinoid reward. J Neurosci 26: 4901–4907.
Acknowledgements
The present research was supported by the Intramural Research Program of National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institutes of Health. We thank Drs Bin Wang, Roy Wise, and Zhi-Bing You for their help with microdialysis and Mary Pfeiffer and Emily Wentzell for editorial assistance.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
DISCLOSURE/CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors disclose that they have no conflict of interest.
Supplementary Information accompanies the paper on the Neuropsychopharmacology website (http://www.nature.com/npp)
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Liu, ZH., Shin, R. & Ikemoto, S. Dual Role of Medial A10 Dopamine Neurons in Affective Encoding. Neuropsychopharmacol 33, 3010–3020 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2008.4
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2008.4
Keywords
This article is cited by
-
Supramammillary neurons projecting to the septum regulate dopamine and motivation for environmental interaction in mice
Nature Communications (2021)
-
Behavioral and neurobiological mechanisms of punishment: implications for psychiatric disorders
Neuropsychopharmacology (2018)
-
Lateral preoptic and ventral pallidal roles in locomotion and other movements
Brain Structure and Function (2018)
-
Recent Advances in Neuroimaging to Model Eating Disorder Neurobiology
Current Psychiatry Reports (2015)
-
Migraine and Reward System—Or Is It Aversive?
Current Pain and Headache Reports (2014)