Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Using visual direction in three-dimensional motion perception

Abstract

The eyes receive slightly different views of the world, and the differences between their images (binocular disparity) are used to see depth. Several authors have suggested how the brain could exploit this information for three-dimensional (3D) motion perception, but here we consider a simpler strategy. Visual direction is the angle between the direction of an object and the direction that an observer faces. Here we describe human behavioral experiments in which observers use visual direction, rather than binocular information, to estimate an object's 3D motion even though this causes them to make systematic errors. This suggests that recent models of binocular 3D motion perception may not reflect the strategies that human observers actually use.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Schematics of stimulus geometry.
Figure 2: Results and predictions for Experiment 1.
Figure 3: Comparison of Experiments 1 and 2.
Figure 4: Experiment 3 results.
Figure 5: Standard error of the mean for each observer for each trajectory angle.
Figure 6: Experiment 4 results.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Westheimer, G. The spatial sense of the eye. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci 18, 893–912 (1979).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Howard, I.P. & Rogers, B.J. Depth Perception. Seeing in Depth Vol. 2 (I. Porteous, Toronto, 2002).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Cumming, B.G. & Parker, A.J. Binocular mechanisms for detecting motion-in-depth. Vision Res. 34, 483–495 (1994).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Regan, D. Binocular correlates of the direction of motion in depth. Vision Res. 33, 2359–2360 (1993).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Brooks, K.R. Interocular velocity difference contributes to stereomotion speed perception. J. Vis. 2, 218–231 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Harris, J.M. & Dean, P.J.A. Accuracy and precision of binocular 3D motion perception. J. Exp. Psych. Hum. Perc. Perf. 29, 869–881 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Rushton, S.K., Harris, J.M., Lloyd, M.R. & Wann, J.P. Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow. Curr. Biol. 8, 1191–1194 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Wilkie, R.M. & Wann, J.P. Driving as night falls: the contribution of retinal flow and visual direction to the control of steering. Curr. Biol. 12, 2014–2017 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Harris, J.M. & Bonas, W. Optic flow and scene structure do not always contribute to the control of human walking. Vision Res. 42, 1619–1626 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Fajen, B.R. & Warren, W.H. Visual guidance of intercepting a moving target on foot. Perception 33, 689–715 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. McKee, S.P. & Welch, L. The precision of size constancy. Vision Res. 32, 1447–1460 (1992).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Foley, J.M. Binocular distance perception. Psychol. Rev. 87, 411–434 (1980).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Beverley, K.I. & Regan, D. The relation between discrimination and sensitivity in the perception of motion in depth. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 249, 387–398 (1975).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Wann, J.P. & Swapp, D.K. Why you should look where you are going. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 647–648 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Wann, J. & Land, M. Steering with or without the flow: is the retrieval of heading necessary? Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 319–324 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Chardenon, A., Montagne, G., Laurent, M. & Bootsma, R.J. The perceptual control of goal-directed locomotion: a common control architecture for interception and navigation? Exp. Brain Res. 158, 100–108 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Peper, L., Bootsma, R.J., Mestre, D.R. & Bakker, F.C. Catching balls: how to get the hand to the right place at the right time. J. Exp. Psych. Hum. Perc. Perf. 20, 591–612 (1994).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Welchman, A.E., Tuck, V.L. & Harris, J.M. Human observers are biased in judging the angular approach of a projectile. Vision Res. 44, 2027–2042 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank P. Dean for collecting some of the data presented here. The work was funded by an Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council project grant and an EPSRC Advanced Fellowship to J.M.H.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Julie M Harris.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Note 1

Control observers on original task. (PDF 49 kb)

Supplementary Note 2

Control experiment: trajectory discrimination. (PDF 193 kb)

Supplementary Note 3

Individual observer data and predictions. (PDF 107 kb)

Supplementary Note 4

Real 3D motion, target moves along track. (PDF 58 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Harris, J., Drga, V. Using visual direction in three-dimensional motion perception. Nat Neurosci 8, 229–233 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1389

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1389

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing