Article | Published:

The anatomy of conscious vision: an fMRI study of visual hallucinations

Nature Neuroscience volume 1, pages 738742 (1998) | Download Citation

Subjects

Abstract

Despite recent advances in functional neuroimaging, the apparently simple question of how and where we see—the neurobiology of visual consciousness—continues to challenge neuroscientists. Without a method to differentiate neural processing specific to consciousness from unconscious afferent sensory signals, the issue has been difficult to resolve experimentally. Here we use functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to study patients with the Charles Bonnet syndrome, for whom visual perception and sensory input have become dissociated. We found that hallucinations of color, faces, textures and objects correlate with cerebral activity in ventral extrastriate visual cortex, that the content of the hallucinations reflects the functional specializations of the region and that patients who hallucinate have increased ventral extrastriate activity, which persists between hallucinations.

  • Subscribe to Nature Neuroscience for full access:

    $59

    Subscribe

Additional access options:

Already a subscriber?  Log in  now or  Register  for online access.

References

  1. 1.

    & Consciousness and neuroscience. Cereb. Cortex 8, 97–107 (1998).

  2. 2.

    Complex visual hallucinations in the hemianopic field. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 48, 29–38 (1985).

  3. 3.

    Spontaneous visual phenomena with visual loss. Neurology 40, 444–447 (1990).

  4. 4.

    et al. Visual hallucinations in patients with macular degeneration. Am. J. Psychiatry 149, 1701– 1706 (1992).

  5. 5.

    Les automatismes visuels. (Hallucinations visuelles rétrochiasmatiques). Schweiz. Med. Woch. 66, 700– 703 (1936).

  6. 6.

    et al. Visual hallucinations in psychologically normal people: Charles Bonnet's syndrome. Lancet 347, 794 –797 (1996).

  7. 7.

    et al. Cortical responses to exogenous visual stimulation during visual hallucinations. Lancet 345, 70 (1995).

  8. 8.

    et al. A functional neuroanatomy of hallucinations in schizophrenia. Nature 378, 176–179 (1995).

  9. 9.

    et al. Auditory hallucinations and the temporal cortical response to speech in schizophrenia: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 1676– 1682 (1997).

  10. 10.

    , , & Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1. J. Neurosci. 16, 4207– 4221 (1996).

  11. 11.

    et al. Differential sensitivity of human visual cortex to faces, letterstrings, and textures: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 16, 5205–5215 (1996).

  12. 12.

    & ffytche, D. H. The Riddoch syndrome: insights into the neurobiology of conscious vision. Brain 121, 25–45 (1998).

  13. 13.

    et al. A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 11, 641– 649 (1991).

  14. 14.

    & The position and topography of the human colour centre as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Brain 120, 2229– 2242 (1997).

  15. 15.

    & The Cerebral Cortex of Man (Macmillan, New York, 1950).

  16. 16.

    , & The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialised for face perception. J. Neurosci. 17, 4302–4311 (1997).

  17. 17.

    , & Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. Brain 115, 15–36 (1992).

  18. 18.

    , , & Functional imaging of human visual recognition. Cog. Brain Res. 5, 55–67 (1996 ).

  19. 19.

    et al. The functional anatomy of imagining and perceiving colour. Neuroreport 9, 1019–1023 (1998).

  20. 20.

    & Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? Nature 375, 121– 123 (1995).

  21. 21.

    , & Motion specific responses from a blind hemifield. Brain 119, 1971–1982 ( 1996).

  22. 22.

    A Vision of the Brain (Blackwell, Oxford, 1993).

  23. 23.

    & Brain activity related to the perception of illusory contours. Neuroimage 3, 104–108 (1996).

  24. 24.

    , & "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatry Res. 12, 189–198 ( 1975).

  25. 25.

    et al. Dynamic magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity during primary sensory stimulation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 89, 5675–5679 ( 1992).

  26. 26.

    et al. Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magn. Reson. Med. 35, 346–355 (1996).

  27. 27.

    & Co-planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain (Thieme, Stuttgart, 1988).

  28. 28.

    et al. Statistical methods of estimation and inference for functional MR image analysis. Magn. Reson. Med. 35, 261–277 (1996).

  29. 29.

    et al. Generic brain activation mapping in functional magnetic resonance imaging: a nonparametric approach. Magn. Reson. Imaging 15, 763–770 (1997).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Kim Miller, the Buckinghamshire Association for the Blind, T. J. ffytche, D. McHugh and G. Plant for referring their patients and to the staff of the Neuroimaging group at the Institute of Psychiatry. D.H.ff. is a Wellcome Clinician Scientist Fellow.

Author information

Affiliations

  1. Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK

    • D. H. ffytche
    • , R. J. Howard
    • , M. J. Brammer
    • , A. David
    • , P. Woodruff
    •  & S. Williams

Authors

  1. Search for D. H. ffytche in:

  2. Search for R. J. Howard in:

  3. Search for M. J. Brammer in:

  4. Search for A. David in:

  5. Search for P. Woodruff in:

  6. Search for S. Williams in:

Corresponding author

Correspondence to D. H. ffytche.

About this article

Publication history

Received

Accepted

Published

DOI

https://doi.org/10.1038/3738

Rights and permissions

To obtain permission to re-use content from this article visit RightsLink.