Article | Published:

Perception of Fourier and non-Fourier motion by larval zebrafish

Abstract

A moving grating elicits innate optomotor behavior in zebrafish larvae; they swim in the direction of perceived motion. We took advantage of this behavior, using computer-animated displays, to determine what attributes of motion are extracted by the fish visual system. As in humans, first-order (luminance-defined or Fourier) signals dominated motion perception in fish; edges or other features had little or no effect when presented with these signals. Humans can see complex movements that lack first-order cues, an ability that is usually ascribed to higher-level processing in the visual cortex. Here we show that second-order (non-Fourier) motion displays induced optomotor behavior in zebrafish larvae, which do not have a cortex. We suggest that second-order motion is extracted early in the lower vertebrate visual pathway.

Access optionsAccess options

Rent or Buy article

Get time limited or full article access on ReadCube.

from$8.99

All prices are NET prices.

References

  1. 1

    Clark, D. T. Visual responses in developing zebrafish. (Univ. of Oregon Press, Eugene, Oregon, 1981).

  2. 2

    Neuhauss, S. C. et al. Genetic disorders of vision revealed by a behavioral screen of 400 essential loci in zebrafish. J. Neurosci. 19, 8603–8615 (1999).

  3. 3

    Anstis, S. M. Phi movement as a subtraction process. Vision Res. 10, 1411–1430 (1970).

  4. 4

    Adelson, E. H. & Bergen, J. R. Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2, 284–299 (1985).

  5. 5

    Reichardt, W. Autokorrelationsauswertung als funktionsprinzip des zentralnervensystems. Z. Naturforschung 12b, 447–457 (1957).

  6. 6

    Wilson, H. R., Ferrera, V. P. & Yo, C. A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception. Vis. Neurosci. 9, 79–97 (1992).

  7. 7

    Hammett, S. T., Ledgeway, T. & Smith, A. T. Transparent motion from feature- and luminance-based processes. Vision Res. 33, 1119–1122 (1993).

  8. 8

    Ramachandran, V. S., Rao, V. M. & Vidyasagar, T. R. Apparent movement with subjective contours. Vision Res. 13, 1399–1401 (1973).

  9. 9

    Cavanagh, P. & Mather, G. Motion: the long and short of it. Spat. Vis. 4, 103–129 (1989).

  10. 10

    Chubb, C. & Sperling, G. Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 5, 1986–2007 (1988).

  11. 11

    Ohzawa, I. Do animals see what we see? Nat. Neurosci. 2, 586–588 (1999).

  12. 12

    Albright, T. D. Form-cue invariant motion processing in primate visual cortex. Science 255, 1141–1143 (1992).

  13. 13

    Zhou, Y. X. & Baker, C. L. A processing stream in mammalian visual cortex neurons for non-Fourier responses. Science 261, 98–101 (1993).

  14. 14

    Smith, A. T., Greenlee, M. W., Singh, K. D., Kraemer, F. M. & Hennig, J. The processing of first- and second-order motion in human visual cortex assessed by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). J. Neurosci. 18, 3816–3830 (1998).

  15. 15

    Baker, C. L. Central neural mechanisms for detecting second-order motion. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9, 461–466 (1999).

  16. 16

    Easter, S. S. & Nicola, G. N. The development of vision in the zebrafish (Danio rerio). Dev. Biol. 180, 646–663 (1996).

  17. 17

    Anstis, S. M. & Rogers, B. J. Illusory reversal of visual depth and movement during changes of contrast. Vision Res. 15, 957–961 (1975).

  18. 18

    Chubb, C. & Sperling, G. Two motion perception mechanisms revealed by distance driven reversal of apparent motion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 86, 2985–2989 (1989).

  19. 19

    Smith, A. T. & Ledgeway, T. Separate detection of moving luminance and contrast modulations: fact or artifact? Vision Res. 37, 45–62 (1997).

  20. 20

    Scott-Samuel, N. E. & Georgeson, M. A. Does early non-linearity account for second-order motion? Vision Res. 39, 2853–2865 (1999).

  21. 21

    Shapley, R. M. & Victor, J. D. The effect of contrast on the transfer properties of cat retinal ganglion cells. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 285, 275–298 (1978).

  22. 22

    Demb, J. B., Haarsma, L., Freed, M. A. & Sterling, P. Functional circuitry of the retinal ganglion cell's nonlinear receptive field. J. Neurosci. 19, 9756–9767 (1999).

  23. 23

    Nieder, A. & Wagner, H. Perception and neuronal coding of subjective contours in the owl. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 660–663 (1999).

  24. 24

    Derrington, A. M. & Henning, B. G. Linear and non-linear mechanisms in pattern vision. Curr. Biol. 3, 800–803 (1995).

  25. 25

    Brockerhoff, S. E. et al. A behavioral screen for isolating zebrafish mutants with visual system defects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 92, 10545–10549 (1995).

  26. 26

    Baier, H. et al. Genetic dissection of the retinotectal projection. Development 123, 415–425 (1996).

  27. 27

    Haffter, P. et al. The identification of genes with unique and essential functions in the development of the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Development 123, 1–36 (1996).

  28. 28

    Driever, W. et al. A genetic screen for mutations affecting embryogenesis in zebrafish. Development 123, 37–46 (1996).

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Mark Churchland and Nick Priebe for reading the manuscript, and Jonathan Horton and Dan Adams for comments. H.B. is a David and Lucile Packard and an Alfred P. Sloan fellow. This work was supported by the HHMI Research Resources Program and by seed money from the Department of Physiology, UCSF. M.O. and M.S. were supported by predoctoral fellowships from HHMI and NSF, respectively.

Author information

Correspondence to Herwig Baier.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Further reading

Figure 2: Psychophysical tests for the detection of Fourier motion by zebrafish.
Figure 1: The optomotor response of zebrafish larvae does not depend on long integration times or feature displacement.
Figure 3: Contrast-defined second-order motion evokes optomotor behavior in zebrafish.
Figure 4: Flicker-defined and orientation-defined second-order motion evokes optomotor behavior in zebrafish.