Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Broca's area and the language instinct

Abstract

Language acquisition in humans relies on abilities like abstraction and use of syntactic rules, which are absent in other animals. The neural correlate of acquiring new linguistic competence was investigated with two functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies. German native speakers learned a sample of 'real' grammatical rules of different languages (Italian or Japanese), which, although parametrically different, follow the universal principles of grammar (UG). Activity during this task was compared with that during a task that involved learning 'unreal' rules of language. 'Unreal' rules were obtained manipulating the original two languages; they used the same lexicon as Italian or Japanese, but were linguistically illegal, as they violated the principles of UG. Increase of activation over time in Broca's area was specific for 'real' language acquisition only, independent of the kind of language. Thus, in Broca's area, biological constraints and language experience interact to enable linguistic competence for a new language.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Behavioral measurements.
Figure 2: Result of the interaction between performance and type of rule learning (real versus unreal Italian).
Figure 3: Results of the conjunction analysis of the real and unreal Italian learning experiment.
Figure 4: Results of the interaction between performance and type of rule learning (real Italian versus unreal Italian in yellow; real versus unreal Japanese in red) resulting from the random effects analysis are shown on selected slices of the T1 template, thresholded at P < 0.001 (uncorrected) for visualization.
Figure 5: Conjunction analysis of the real-unreal Japanese (red) and Italian (yellow) learning experiment.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Darwin, C.R. Descent of Man (John Murray, London, 1871).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Chomsky, N. Syntactic Structures (Mouton, The Hague, Paris, 1957).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Chomsky, N. Knowledge of Language: its Nature, Origin and Use (Praeger, New York, 1986).

  4. Pinker, S. Talks of genetics and vice versa. Nature 413, 465–466 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Moro, A. Linguistica mendeliana ovvero quali domande su genetica e grammatica. Lingue e Linguaggio 1, 39–58 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  6. Kim, K.H., Relkin, N.R., Lee, K.M. & Hirsch, J. Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature 10, 171–174 (1997).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Illes, J. et al. Convergent cortical representation of semantic processing in bilinguals. Brain Lang. 70, 347–363 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Chomsky, N. Lectures on Government and Binding (Foris, Dordrecht, 1981).

    Google Scholar 

  9. Chomsky, N. The Minimalist Program (MIT press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1995).

    Google Scholar 

  10. Piattelli-Palmarini, M. Language and Learning: the Debate Between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1980).

    Google Scholar 

  11. Anderson, M. Intelligence and Development: a Cognitive Theory (Basil Blackwell, Oxford, 1992).

  12. Moro, A. Dynamic Antisymmetry (MIT press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2000).

    Google Scholar 

  13. Smith, N. & Tsimpli, I.A. The Mind of a Savant (Blackwell Publisher Ltd., Oxford, 1996).

    Google Scholar 

  14. Tettamanti, M. et al. Neural correlates for the acquisition of natural language syntax. Neuroimage 17, 700–709 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Breitenstein, C. & Knecht, S. Development and validation of a language learning model for behavioral and functional-imaging studies. J. Neurosci. Methods 114, 173–179 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Friederici A.D., Steinhauer, K., Pfeifer E. Brain signatures of artificial language processing: evidence challenging the critical period hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 8, 529–534 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Rose M., Haider H., Weiller, C. & Buechel, C. The role of medial temporal lobe structures in implicit learning: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron 36, 1221–1231 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Buckner, R.L. Functional neuroimaging of human memory in The New Cognitive Neuroscience (ed. Gazzaniga, S.) 817–828 (MIT Press, Massachusetts, 2000).

    Google Scholar 

  19. Lapage, M., Ghaffar, O., Nyberg, L. & Tulving, E. Prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval mode. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 97, 506–511 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Owen, A.M. The role of the lateral frontal cortex in mnemonic processing: the contribution of functional neuroimaging. Exp. Brain Res. 133, 33–43 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Shallice, T. et al. Brain regions associated with acquisition and retrieval of verbal episodic memory. Nature 368, 633–635 (1994).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Tulving, E., Kapur, S., Craik F.I.M., Moscovitch, M. & Houle, S. Hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry in episodic memory: Positron emission tomography findings. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91, 2016–2020 (1994).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Allan, K., Dolan, R.J., Fletcher, P.C. & Rugg, M.D. The role of the right anterior prefrontal cortex in episodic retrieval. Neuroimage 11, 217–227 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Petrides, M. The role of the mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in working memory. Exp. Brain Res. 133, 44–54 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Henson, R.N.A., Shallice, T. & Dolan, J.R. Right prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval: a functional fMRI test of the monitoring hypothesis. Brain 122, 1367–1381 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Ullman, M.T. A neurocognitive perspective on language: the declarative/procedural model. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 717–726 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Seger, A.C. Two forms of sequential implicit learning. Conscious. Cogn. 6, 108–131 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Maess, B., Koelsch, S., Gunter, T.C. & Friederici, A.D. Musical syntax is processed in Broca's area: an MEG study. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 540–545 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Hahne, A. & Friederici, A.D. Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: early automatic and late controlled processes. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 194–205 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Friederici, A.D., Meyer, M. & von Cramon, D.Y. Auditory language comprehension: an event-related fMRI study on processing of syntax and lexical information. Brain Lang. 74, 289–300 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Embick, D., Marantz, A., Miyashita, Y., O'Neil, W. & Sakai, K.L. A syntactic specialisation for Broca's area. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 23, 6150–6154 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Ni, W. et al. An event-related neuroimaging study distinguishing form and content in sentence processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 120–133 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Sakai, K.L., Noguchi, Y., Takeuchi, T. & Watanabe, E. Selective priming of syntactic processing by event-related transcranial magnetic stimulation of Broca's area. Neuron 35, 1177–1182 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Patel, A.D., Gibson, E., Ratner, J., Besson, M. & Holcomb, P.J. Processing syntactic relations in language and music: an event-related potential study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 717–733 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Vargha-Khadem, F. et al. Neural basis of an inherited speech and language disorder. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95, 12695–12700 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Gopnik, M. Genetic basis of grammar defect. Nature 347, 715 (1990).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Alcock, K.J., Passingham, R.E., Watkins K. & Vargha-Khadem, F. Pitch and timing abilities in inherited speech and language impairment. Brain Lang. 15, 34–46 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Lai, C.S., Fisher, S.E., Hurst, J.A., Vargha-Khadem, F. & Monaco, A.P. A forkhead- domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature 413, 519–523 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Enard, W. et al. Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature 22, 869–872 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V. & Fogassi, L. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Res. Cogn. 3, 131–141 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Rizzolatti, G. & Arbib, M.A. Language within our grasp. Trends Neurosci. 21, 188–194, (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Jensvold, M.L. & Gardner, R.A. Interactive use of sign language by cross-fostered chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 14, 335–346 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Cantalupo, C., Hopkins, W.D. Asymmetric Broca's area in great apes. Nature 414, 505 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Walker, A.E. A cytoarchitectonical study of the prefrontal area of the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 262, 256–270 (1940).

    Google Scholar 

  45. Petrides, M. & Pandya, D.N. Comparative architectonic analysis of the human and the macaque frontal cortex. in Handbook of Neuropsychology IX (Boller Grafman, Amsterdam, 1994).

    Google Scholar 

  46. Oldfield, R.C. The assessment and analysis of handiness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113 (1971).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Delis, K., Kramer, M., Kaplan, L. & Ober, R. California Verbal Learning Test (The Psychological Corporation, New York, 1987).

    Google Scholar 

  48. Gittler, G., Fay, F. & Trost, G. Intelligenz-Struktur –Analyse ISA (Swets Test Services, Frankfurt, 1998).

    Google Scholar 

  49. Markson, L. & Bloom, P. Evidence against a dedicated system for word learning in children. Nature 27, 813–815 (1997).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Holmes A.P., Friston, K.J. Generalizability, random effects and population interference. Neuroimage 7, S574 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the medical technical assistants of the Radiology Department of the Friedrich Schiller University in Jena as well as all the volunteers and the colleagues of the NeuroImage Nord in Hamburg. Special thanks to S. Michels, C. Donati, I. Mazur, S. Barkowsky, S. Kameyama and L. Wolfram for help with trial design, T. Wolbers and M. Rose for help with statistical analysis, and A. Baumgärtner and D. Gonzalo for proof-reading of the manuscript. This work was supported by a European Union grant (QLRT-1999-2140). C. Büchel is supported by the Volkswagen-Stiftung.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mariacristina Musso.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Fig. 1.

Result of the interaction between performance and type of rule learning (real versus unreal Italian). On the left, the activation specific to real language acquisition resulting from the random effects analysis is displayed on selected slices of the MRI template available in SPM99. The threshold was set at p<0.05 (corrected for multiple comparisons). On the right, plots of changes of BOLD signal in the left inferior frontal gyrus [Brodmann Area (BA) -45, 21 6] (black open squares) and of the individual accuracy within sessions containing "real grammatical" trials (black filled circles) or "unreal grammatical" trials (black open circles) are shown as a function of time. BOLD signal is expressed with arbitrary units; the accuracy of performance as normalised values. The distance between the voxels where each subject showed the nearest activation to the cluster maximum and the voxel maxima derived from the group analysis using a random model is: 28 mm (subject 1), 23 mm (subject 2), 3 mm (subject 3), 24 mm (subject 4), 13 mm (subject 5), 11 mm (subject 6), 23 mm (subjects 7) and 15 mm (subject 8). (JPG 46 kb)

Supplementary Fig. 2.

Results of the conjunction analysis of the "real" and the "unreal" Italian learning experiment. On the left, the common patterns of activation are displayed on slices from the MRI template used for the normalisation. The threshold was set at p<0.05 (corrected for multiple comparisons). On the right, plots of changes in BOLD signal in the right inferior frontal gyrus [BA 51, 6, 30] (black open squares) and of the individual accuracy within sessions containing "real grammatical" trials (black filled circles) or "unreal grammatical" trials (black open circles) are shown as a function of time. BOLD signal is expressed with arbitrary units; the accuracy of performance as normalized values. The distance between single subject activation and main group activation in the conjunction analysis was about: 11 mm (subject 1), 5 mm (subject 2), 8 mm (subject 3), 6 mm (subject 4), 3 mm (subject 5), 7 mm (subjects 6 and 7) and 21 mm (subject 8). (JPG 56 kb)

Supplementary Fig. 3.

Results of the interaction between performance and type of rule learning (real Italian versus unreal Italian, in yellow, and real versus unreal Japanese, in red) resulting from the random effects analysis are displayed on selected slices of the T1 template, thresholded at p< 0.001 (uncorrected) for visualisation. On the right, plots of individual changes in BOLD signal in the left inferior frontal gyrus (black open squares) and of the individual accuracy within sessions containing "real grammatical" trials (black filled circles) or "unreal grammatical" trials (black open circles) are shown as a function of time. BOLD signal is expressed with arbitrary units; the accuracy of performance as normalized values. The distance between individual voxel closest to the cluster maximum and the voxel maxima derived from the group analysis using a random model is: 12 mm (subjects 1), 21 mm (subject 2), 25 mm (subject 3), 9 mm (subjects 4 and 5 and 6), 6 mm (subjects 7) and 11 mm (subject 8). (JPG 52 kb)

Supplementary Fig. 4.

Results of the conjunction analysis of the real-unreal Japanese (in red) and Italian (in yellow) learning experiment. On the left, common patterns of activation are displayed on slices of the MRI template. The threshold was set at p<0.05 (corrected for multiple comparisons). On the right, plots of changes of BOLD signal in the right inferior frontal gyrus (black open squares) and of the individual accuracy within sessions containing "real grammatical" trials (black filled circles) or "unreal grammatical" trials (black open circles) are shown as a function of time. BOLD signal is expressed with arbitrary units; the accuracy of performance as normalized values. The distance between single subject activation and main group activation in the conjunction analysis was about: 15 mm (subjects 1 and 2), 5 mm (subject 3), 0 mm (subjects 4, 5 and 8), 7 mm (subject 6), 4 mm (subject 7). (JPG 51 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Musso, M., Moro, A., Glauche, V. et al. Broca's area and the language instinct. Nat Neurosci 6, 774–781 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1077

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1077

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing