Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

Pregnancy leads to long-lasting changes in human brain structure

Abstract

Pregnancy involves radical hormone surges and biological adaptations. However, the effects of pregnancy on the human brain are virtually unknown. Here we show, using a prospective ('pre'-'post' pregnancy) study involving first-time mothers and fathers and nulliparous control groups, that pregnancy renders substantial changes in brain structure, primarily reductions in gray matter (GM) volume in regions subserving social cognition. The changes were selective for the mothers and highly consistent, correctly classifying all women as having undergone pregnancy or not in-between sessions. Interestingly, the volume reductions showed a substantial overlap with brain regions responding to the women's babies postpartum. Furthermore, the GM volume changes of pregnancy predicted measures of postpartum maternal attachment, suggestive of an adaptive process serving the transition into motherhood. Another follow-up session showed that the GM reductions endured for at least 2 years post-pregnancy. Our data provide the first evidence that pregnancy confers long-lasting changes in a woman's brain.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Rent or buy this article

Prices vary by article type

from$1.95

to$39.95

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: GM volume changes between pre-pregnancy and post-pregnancy session.
Figure 2: Means of conception.
Figure 3: Classification.
Figure 4: Similarity between theory-of-mind network and GM volume changes of pregnancy.
Figure 5: Surface-based measures.
Figure 6: Postpartum infant-related neural activity and attachment scores.
Figure 7: Long-term follow-up.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Brunton, P.J. & Russell, J.A. The expectant brain: adapting for motherhood. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 11–25 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Casey, M.L., MacDonald, P.C., Sargent, I.L. & Starkey, P.M. Placental endocrinology. in The Human Placenta (ed. Redman, C.W.G.) 237–272 (Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, 1993).

  3. Simerly, R.B. Wired for reproduction: organization and development of sexually dimorphic circuits in the mammalian forebrain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 507–536 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Peper, J.S., Hulshoff Pol, H.E., Crone, E.A. & van Honk, J. Sex steroids and brain structure in pubertal boys and girls: a mini-review of neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience 191, 28–37 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Sisk, C.L. & Foster, D.L. The neural basis of puberty and adolescence. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 1040–1047 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Sisk, C.L. & Zehr, J.L. Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and behavior. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 26, 163–174 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Comasco, E., Frokjaer, V.G. & Sundström-Poromaa, I. Functional and molecular neuroimaging of menopause and hormone replacement therapy. Front. Neurosci. 8, 388 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Craig, M.C. & Murphy, D.G. Estrogen: effects on normal brain function and neuropsychiatric disorders. Climacteric 10 (Suppl. 2), 97–104 (2007).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Toffoletto, S., Lanzenberger, R., Gingnell, M., Sundström-Poromaa, I. & Comasco, E. Emotional and cognitive functional imaging of estrogen and progesterone effects in the female human brain: a systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology 50, 28–52 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Holdcroft, A. et al. Phosphorus-31 brain MR spectroscopy in women during and after pregnancy compared with nonpregnant control subjects. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 26, 352–356 (2005).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Roos, A., Robertson, F., Lochner, C., Vythilingum, B. & Stein, D.J. Altered prefrontal cortical function during processing of fear-relevant stimuli in pregnancy. Behav. Brain Res. 222, 200–205 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Rutherford, J.M., Moody, A., Crawshaw, S. & Rubin, P.C. Magnetic resonance spectroscopy in pre-eclampsia: evidence of cerebral ischaemia. BJOG 110, 416–423 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Erdheim, J. & Stumme, E. Über die Schwangerschaftsveränderung der Hypophyse. Ziegler's. Beitr. Pathol. Anat. 45, 1–17 (1909).

    Google Scholar 

  14. Bergland, R.M., Ray, B.S. & Torack, R.M. Anatomical variations in the pituitary gland and adjacent structures in 225 human autopsy cases. J. Neurosurg. 28, 93–99 (1968).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Gonzalez, J.G. et al. Pituitary gland growth during normal pregnancy: an in vivo study using magnetic resonance imaging. Am. J. Med. 85, 217–220 (1988).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Oatridge, A. et al. Change in brain size during and after pregnancy: study in healthy women and women with preeclampsia. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 23, 19–26 (2002).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Hillerer, K.M., Jacobs, V.R., Fischer, T. & Aigner, L. The maternal brain: an organ with peripartal plasticity. Neural Plast. 2014, 574159 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Kinsley, C.H. & Amory-Meyer, E. Why the maternal brain? J. Neuroendocrinol. 23, 974–983 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Kinsley, C.H., Meyer, E. & Rafferty, K.A. Sex steroid hormone determination of the maternal brain: effects beyond reproduction. Mini Rev. Med. Chem. 12, 1063–1070 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Macbeth, A.H. & Luine, V.N. Changes in anxiety and cognition due to reproductive experience: a review of data from rodent and human mothers. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 452–467 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Kinsley, C.H., Franssen, R.A. & Meyer, E.A. Reproductive experience may positively adjust the trajectory of senescence. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 317–345 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F. & Perner, J. Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 42, 9–34 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Yeo, B.T. et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1125–1165 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Condon, J. & Corkindale, C. The assessment of parent-to-infant attachment: development of a self-report questionnaire instrument. J. Reprod. Infant Psychol. 16, 57–76 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Swain, J.E. et al. Approaching the biology of human parental attachment: brain imaging, oxytocin and coordinated assessments of mothers and fathers. Brain Res. 1580, 78–101 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Blakemore, S.J. The social brain in adolescence. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 267–277 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Herting, M.M., Gautam, P., Spielberg, J.M., Dahl, R.E. & Sowell, E.R. A longitudinal study: changes in cortical thickness and surface area during pubertal maturation. PLoS One 10, e0119774 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Peper, J.S. et al. Sex steroids and brain structure in pubertal boys and girls. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 332–342 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Anderson, M.V. & Rutherford, M.D. Recognition of novel faces after single exposure is enhanced during pregnancy. Evol. Psychol. 9, 47–60 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Anderson, M.V. & Rutherford, M.D. Cognitive reorganization during pregnancy and the postpartum period: an evolutionary perspective. Evol. Psychol. 10, 659–687 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Pearson, R.M., Lightman, S.L. & Evans, J. Emotional sensitivity for motherhood: late pregnancy is associated with enhanced accuracy to encode emotional faces. Horm. Behav. 56, 557–563 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Fradley, E. & Tuckey, M. Rethinking maternal sensitivity: mothers' comments on infants' mental processes predict security of attachment at 12 months. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 637–648 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Lyall, A.E. et al. Dynamic development of regional cortical thickness and surface area in early childhood. Cereb. Cortex 25, 2204–2212 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Nygaard, G.O. et al. Cortical thickness and surface area relate to specific symptoms in early relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler. 21, 402–414 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Oster, S. et al. Cerebral atrophy in AIDS: a stereological study. Acta Neuropathol. 85, 617–622 (1993).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Regeur, L., Jensen, G.B., Pakkenberg, H., Evans, S.M. & Pakkenberg, B. No global neocortical nerve cell loss in brains from patients with senile dementia of Alzheimer's type. Neurobiol. Aging 15, 347–352 (1994).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Raznahan, A. et al. How does your cortex grow? J. Neurosci. 31, 7174–7177 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Pawluski, J.L., Lambert, K.G. & Kinsley, C.H. Neuroplasticity in the maternal hippocampus: relation to cognition and effects of repeated stress. 77, 86–97 (2016).

  39. Kinsley, C.H. et al. Motherhood and the hormones of pregnancy modify concentrations of hippocampal neuronal dendritic spines. Horm. Behav. 49, 131–142 (2006).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Pawluski, J.L. et al. Pregnancy or stress decrease complexity of CA3 pyramidal neurons in the hippocampus of adult female rats. Neuroscience 227, 201–210 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Pawluski, J.L. et al. Effects of stress early in gestation on hippocampal neurogenesis and glucocorticoid receptor density in pregnant rats. Neuroscience 290, 379–388 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Pawluski, J.L. & Galea, L.A. Reproductive experience alters hippocampal neurogenesis during the postpartum period in the dam. Neuroscience 149, 53–67 (2007).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Barha, C.K., Lieblich, S.E., Chow, C. & Galea, L.A. Multiparity-induced enhancement of hippocampal neurogenesis and spatial memory depends on ovarian hormone status in middle age. Neurobiol. Aging 36, 2391–2405 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Galea, L.A. et al. Spatial working memory and hippocampal size across pregnancy in rats. Horm. Behav. 37, 86–95 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Hillerer, K.M., Neumann, I.D., Couillard-Despres, S., Aigner, L. & Slattery, D.A. Lactation-induced reduction in hippocampal neurogenesis is reversed by repeated stress exposure. Hippocampus 24, 673–683 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Henry, J.D. & Rendell, P.G. A review of the impact of pregnancy on memory function. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 29, 793–803 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Glynn, L.M. Giving birth to a new brain: hormone exposures of pregnancy influence human memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 1148–1155 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Buckwalter, J.G., Buckwalter, D.K., Bluestein, B.W. & Stanczyk, F.Z. Pregnancy and post partum: changes in cognition and mood. Prog. Brain Res. 133, 303–319 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Christensen, H., Leach, L.S. & Mackinnon, A. Cognition in pregnancy and motherhood: prospective cohort study. Br. J. Psychiatry 196, 126–132 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Kim, P. et al. The plasticity of human maternal brain: longitudinal changes in brain anatomy during the early postpartum period. Behav. Neurosci. 124, 695–700 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Friston, K. Ten ironic rules for non-statistical reviewers. Neuroimage 61, 1300–1310 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Dunson, D.B., Colombo, B. & Baird, D.D. Changes with age in the level and duration of fertility in the menstrual cycle. Hum. Reprod. 17, 1399–1403 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Sheehan, D.V. et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59 (Suppl. 20), 22–33, quiz 34–57 (1998).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Vanston, C.M. & Watson, N.V. Selective and persistent effect of foetal sex on cognition in pregnant women. Neuroreport 16, 779–782 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Cox, J.L., Holden, J.M. & Sagovsky, R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Br. J. Psychiatry 150, 782–786 (1987).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Benedet, M.J. & Alejandre, M.A. Test de Aprendizaje Verbal España-Complutense (TEA Ediciones, 1998).

  57. Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E. & Ober, B.A. California Verbal Learning Test 2nd edn. (Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, Texas, USA, 2000).

  58. Kaufman, A.S. & Lichtenberger, E. Assessing Adolescent and Adult Intelligence 3rd edn. (Wiley, Hoboken, New Jersey, USA, 2006).

  59. Davis, M.H. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology 10, 85–90 (1980).

    Google Scholar 

  60. Ashburner, J. & Ridgway, G.R. Symmetric diffeomorphic modeling of longitudinal structural MRI. Front. Neurosci. 6, 197 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Ashburner, J. & Friston, K.J. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage 26, 839–851 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Ashburner, J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95–113 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Ashburner, J. & Friston, K.J. Voxel-based morphometry—the methods. Neuroimage 11, 805–821 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Radua, J., Canales-Rodríguez, E.J., Pomarol-Clotet, E. & Salvador, R. Validity of modulation and optimal settings for advanced voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage 86, 81–90 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Salmond, C.H. et al. Distributional assumptions in voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage 17, 1027–1030 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Schrouff, J. et al. PRoNTo: pattern recognition for neuroimaging toolbox. Neuroinformatics 11, 319–337 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Golland, P. & Fischl, B. Permutation tests for classification: towards statistical significance in image-based studies. Inf. Process. Med. Imaging 18, 330–341 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Rakotomamonjy, A., Bach, F., Canu, S. & Grandvalet, Y. SimpleMKL. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 9, 2491–2521 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  69. Shawe-Taylor, J. & Cristianini, N. Kernel Methods for Pattern Analysis (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2004).

  70. MacMaster, F.P. et al. Pituitary volume in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 252–257 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Reuter, M. & Fischl, B. Avoiding asymmetry-induced bias in longitudinal image processing. Neuroimage 57, 19–21 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Reuter, M., Schmansky, N.J., Rosas, H.D. & Fischl, B. Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage 61, 1402–1418 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Dale, A.M., Fischl, B. & Sereno, M.I. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179–194 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Fischl, B., Sereno, M.I. & Dale, A.M. Cortical surface-based analysis. II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9, 195–207 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Desikan, R.S. et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968–980 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the participants for their contribution to this study. We thank A. Bulbena for supporting the project, and M. López, G. Pons, R. Martínez, L. González, E. Castaño, N. Mallorquí-Bagué, J. Fauquet and C. Pretus for helping with the data collection and scoring of the cognitive tests. In addition, we thank C. Phillips and J.D. Gispert for advice on the multivariate analyses, E. Marinetto and C. Falcón for advice on the FreeSurfer analyses, and J. van Hemmen and J. Bakker for discussions of the project and results. E.H. was supported by a Formación de Profesorado Universitario (FPU) grant by the Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia, Spanish government, and is now supported by an Innovational Research Incentives Scheme grant (Veni, 451-14-036) of the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO), E.B.-M. by a grant from the National Council of Science and Technology of Mexico, S.C. by the Consejería de Educación, Juventud y Deporte of Comunidad de Madrid and the People Programme (Marie Curie Actions) of the European Union's Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) under REA grant agreement 291820, and M.P. by an FI grant of the Agencia de Gestió d'Ajuts Universitaris de Recerca, Generalitat de Catalunya.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

E.H., E.B.-M., S.C., and O.V. designed the experiments. C.P., A.B., and F.L. recruited part of the participants and provided clinical information. E.B.-M. oversaw the overall timeline, recruitment and data collection of the project, and acquired the data together with E.H., M.P. and S.C. J.C.S., A.T., M.D., E.A.C. and O.V. provided facilities and advice on aspects of design, acquisition or interpretation. E.H. analyzed the data, except for the area and thickness analysis done by S.C. and D.G.-G. E.H. wrote the manuscript and all other authors evaluated and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elseline Hoekzema.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1 Plots of the remaining clusters

For the remaining clusters not indicated in the main figures (the left inferior orbitofrontal and the left middle frontal cluster, see Table 1), this figure depicts the (a) slice overlays,(b) plots representing the mean signal from the smoothed normalized jacobian difference images of the PRE and POST session, and (c) plots representing the mean (M±S.E.M.) signal change at each POST session relative to the pre-pregnancy baseline. FCTR=nulliparous control women, FPRG=women who were pregnant and transitioned into primiparity in-between sessions, Inf.= Inferior, Mid.= Middle, L=Left.

Supplementary Figure 2 Effect sizes

(a) Illustration of the effect sizes (Cohen's d) for the changes in GM volume in the women who were pregnant in-between sessions in comparison to the control women. All depicted effect sizes correspond to large effect sizes (Cohen's d>0.8), although it should be noted that effect sizes from mapping experiments can be optimistically biased. Effect sizes were additionally computed separately for the GM volume changes in women achieving pregnancy by means of natural conception (b), and the women achieving pregnancy by fertility treatment (c). Yellow/orange tones correspond to reductions in GM volume across sessions in primiparous women in comparison to nulliparous control women, while blue tones represent relative increases in primiparous women in comparison to the control group. Effect sizes were extracted using the VBM8 toolbox (http://www.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm/) and plotted in CARET (http://brainvis.wustl.edu).

Supplementary Figure 3 Pituitary gland volume

Bar charts (a) and scatter plots (b) depicting pituitary gland volume in each session. The pituitary gland was manually delineated as a complementary analysis to further explore the data based on the previous findings of larger pituitary volume in pregnant women. Mean pituitary gland volume was about 40 mm3 larger in the early postpartum session than in the PRE and the POST+2yrs sessions (M±SD: PRE: 622.80±91.47 mm3. POST: 663±58-93.36 mm3. POST+2yrs: 626.07±93.85 mm3), although this PRE-to-POST increase is not significant (GLM repeated measures: F=1.50, p=0.235). Accordingly, the observed increase of 7% in the POST session is subtle compared to the relative volume increases previously observed in late pregnancy, which fits with previous findings showing that the pituitary gland rapidly loses most of its volume gains shortly after birth. A significant reduction was observed between the early postpartum and the POST+2yrs session (F=21.32, p=0.001), suggesting that pituitary volumes in the POST session had not yet completely returned to pre-pregnancy levels yet. As expected, pituitary gland volume at the POST+2yrs session did not differ from the pre-pregnancy baseline (F=0.002, p=0.968).

Supplementary Figure 4 Individual GM volume changes separated by means of conception

Plots representing mean signal from the smoothed normalized jacobian difference images averaged across cluster for the women achieving pregnancy by fertility treatment (FTRT, N=16), women achieving pregnancy by natural conception (FNAT, N=9) and nulliparous control women who were not pregnant in-between sessions (FCTR, N=20). Sup. Temp. Sulcus = Superior Temporal Sulcus, Med.= Medial, Inf.= Inferior, Mid.= Middle, L=left, R=right.

Supplementary Figure 5 Surface-projected weight maps for multivariate classification

Projections of the average weight map, illustrating the relative importance of each voxel in the decision function, for the support vector machine classification results (see main manuscript) onto the brain's surface.

Supplementary Figure 6 Individual GM volume changes per cluster for all four groups

Plots representing mean signal from the smoothed normalized jacobian difference images for each group averaged across cluster. FCTR=nulliparous control women, FPRG= women who were pregnant and became first-time mothers in-between sessions. MCTR=nulliparous control men whose partners were not pregnant in-between sessions, MPRG= men whose partners were pregnant and who became first-time fathers in-between sessions, Sup. Temp. Sulcus = Superior Temporal Sulcus, Med.= Medial, Inf.= Inferior, Mid.= Middle, L=left, R=right.

Supplementary Figure 7 Weight maps for multivariate regression results

Slice overlays depicting the average weight map, illustrating the relative importance of each voxel in the multivariate regression, for the kernel ridge regression analyses (see main manuscript). (a) Absence of hostility. (b) Quality of attachment.

Supplementary Figure 8 Surface-projected weight maps for multivariate regression results

Projections of the average weight map, illustrating the relative importance of each voxel in the multivariate regression, for the kernel ridge regression analyses (see main manuscript) onto the brain's surface. (a) Absence of hostility. (b) Quality of attachment.

Supplementary Figure 9 Overlap structural changes and postpartum fMRI results

Slice overlays depicting the overlap between the changes in GM volume across pregnancy and the ‘own baby>other baby' contrast of the postpartum fMRI paradigm. A red color indicates the area was present in both maps, while light and dark blue depict areas that were only present in either the structural or the fMRI map respectively.

Supplementary Figure 10 Pre and Post plots

Illustration of GM signal in the PRE and POST sessions for the group of women who were pregnant in-between sessions and those who were not. Images were processed using a cross-sectional voxel-based morphometry approach. Due to the large inter-individual variability in GM values, the groups could not clearly be separated from one another visually when plotted in scatterplots, stressing the importance of using a pre-pregnancy baseline to map changes on an individual level. Therefore, bar charts depicting the group means (M±SEM) are provided for each of the regions. FCTR=nulliparous control women, FPRG=women who were pregnant and transitioned into primiparity in-between sessions, Sup. Temp. Sulcus = Superior Temporal Sulcus, Med.= Medial, Inf.= Inferior, Mid.= Middle, L=left, R=right.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Figures 1–10 and Supplementary Tables 1–23 (PDF 2481 kb)

Supplementary Methods Checklist (PDF 650 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hoekzema, E., Barba-Müller, E., Pozzobon, C. et al. Pregnancy leads to long-lasting changes in human brain structure. Nat Neurosci 20, 287–296 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4458

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4458

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing