Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Brief Communication
  • Published:

Bidirectional prefrontal-hippocampal interactions support context-guided memory

Abstract

We compared the dynamics of hippocampal and prefrontal interactions in rats as they used spatial contexts to guide the retrieval of object memories. Functional connectivity analysis indicated a flow of contextual information from the hippocampus to prefrontal cortex upon the rat's entry into the spatial context. Conversely, upon the onset of object sampling, the direction of information flow reversed, consistent with prefrontal control over the retrieval of context-appropriate hippocampal memory representations.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1
Figure 2: Normalized correlations between instantaneous theta amplitude across a range of shifts between LFPs recorded in HPC and mPFC.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Eichenbaum, H. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 732–744 (2014).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Kuhl, B.A. & Wagner, A.D. in Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, Vol. 9 (eds. L.R. Squire et al.) 437–444 (Academic Press, 2009).

  3. Szczepanski, S.M. & Knight, R.T. Neuron 83, 1002–1018 (2014).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Farovik, A., Dupont, L.M., Arce, M. & Eichenbaum, H. J. Neurosci. 28, 13428–13434 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Navawongse, R. & Eichenbaum, H. J. Neurosci. 33, 1002–1013 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Siapas, A.G., Lubenov, E.V. & Wilson, M.A. Neuron 46, 141–151 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Hyman, J.M., Zilli, E.A., Paley, A.M. & Hasselmo, M.E. Hippocampus 15, 739–749 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Benchenane, K. et al. Neuron 66, 921–936 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Spellman, T. et al. Nature 522, 309–314 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Anderson, K.L., Rajagovindan, R., Ghacibeh, G.A., Meador, K.J. & Ding, M. Cereb. Cortex 20, 1604–1612 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Nyhus, E. & Curran, T. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 1023–1035 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Buzsáki, G., Anastassiou, C.A. & Koch, C. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 407–420 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Adhikari, A., Sigurdsson, T., Topiwala, M.A. & Gordon, J.A. J. Neurosci. Methods 191, 191–200 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Csicsvari, J., Jamieson, B., Wise, K.D. & Buzsáki, G. Neuron 37, 311–322 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Buzsáki, G. & Wang, X.J. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 203–225 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Colgin, L.L. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 21, 467–474 (2011).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Watrous, A.J., Fell, J., Ekstrom, A.D. & Axmacher, N. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 31, 33–39 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Canolty, R.T. & Knight, R.T. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 506–515 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Tort, A.B.L., Komorowski, R.W., Manns, J.R., Kopell, N.J. & Eichenbaum, H. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 20942–20947 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Staudigl, T. & Hanslmayr, S. Curr. Biol. 23, 1101–1106 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Komorowski, R.W., Manns, J.R. & Eichenbaum, H. J. Neurosci. 29, 9918–9929 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Paxinos, G. & Watson, C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates (Elsevier, 2006).

  23. Mitra, P.P. & Bokil, H. Observed Brain Dynamics (Oxford Univ. Press, 2008).

  24. Barnett, L. & Seth, A.K. J. Neurosci. Methods 223, 50–68 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Barrens, P. J. Stat. Softw. 31, http://dx.doi.org/10.18637/jss.v031.i10 (2009).

Download references

Acknowledgements

The work was supported by NIMH grants MH 094263 and MH 051570 to H.E.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors designed the experiment. R.P., A.F. and M.B. conducted the experiment. R.P. conduced the data analyses. R.P. and H.E. wrote the paper.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Howard Eichenbaum.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1 Electrode locations and characteristics of local field potentials.

(a) Locations of electrode tips in prefrontal cortex (mPFC), dorsal hippocampus (dHPC), and ventral hippocampus (vHPC) at mm AP to bregma: mPFC 3.20; dHPC 3.14; vHPC -5.60. mPFC electrodes were localized in layers II/III (n=7); dHPC in the pyramidal layer (n=6); vHPC in the pyramidal layer (n=2) or stratum radiatum (n=2). (b) Frequency spectra show prominent power spectral density in the theta band during both the context exploration (Peak Frequency = 8.13 +/− 0.61 Hz, mPFC; 8.93 +/− 0.15 Hz, dHPC; 9.11 +/− 0.15 Hz, vHPC) and object sampling periods (Peak Frequency = mPFC: 8.3 +/− 0.43 Hz; dHPC: 8.79 +/− 0.14 Hz; vHPC: 9.03 +/− 0.13 Hz). (c) Mean coherence between dHPC and vHPC and mPFC was maximal at theta frequency both for the context exploration (Peak Frequency = dHPC-mPFC: 9.57 Hz +/−0.17 Hz; vHPC-mPFC: 9.51 +/− 0.23 Hz) and object sampling periods (Peak Frequency = dHPC-mPFC: 9.34 +/− 0.21; vHPC-mPFC: 8.91 +/− 0.14 Hz). Shading indicates S.E.M. (d) Peak power frequency during first 1-s of context exploration and object sampling did not significantly differ among areas (One-way ANOVA F1, 30 = 0, p = 0.97 +/− S.E.M. (e). Peak Coherence Frequency during first 1 s of context exploration and versus object sampling did not significantly differ (One-way ANOVA, F1, 18 = 3.49, p = 0.08). Coherence values for these frequencies were greater than those predicted by 95% jackknife-estimated confidence intervals (0.1069) for both context exploration (Coherency = dHPC-mPFC: 0.47 +/− 0.04; vHPC-mPFC: 0.38 +/− 0.02) and object sampling (Coherency = dHPC-mPFC: 0.42 +/− 0.05; vHPC-mPFC: 0.32 +/− 0.02).

Supplementary Figure 2 Normalized correlations between instantaneous theta amplitude across a range of shifts between LFPs recorded in hippocampus (HPC) and prefrontal cortex (mPFC) for individual subjects.

Correlation patterns for post-learning sessions (a,b) and for correct trials (c,d) and errors (e,f) during learning sessions.

Supplementary Figure 3 Granger causal (GC) relationships (1–25 Hz) across sessions for context exploration and object sampling.

GC was greater from HPC to mPFC during (a) context exploration (Two-way ANOVA, F = 1033.46, df = 1, p = 9.83e-215), while mPFC better predicted HPC activity during instances of (b) object sampling (Two-way ANOVA, F = 2571.27, df = 1, p < 0.0001). Furthermore, maximal GC was within the theta range for both context exploration (9.16 +/− 0.83 Hz) and object sampling periods (9.27 +/− 0.92 Hz), and the causal flow reversed within the HPC-PFC circuit at this frequency as a function of behavioral phase, such that HPC directed mPFC activity during context exploration (GC = 0.3983 +/− 0.07, Wilcoxon rank-sum, z42 = 4.60, p = 4.15e-6) and mPFC engaged HPC during object sampling (GC = 0.3518 +/− 0.04, Wilcoxon rank-sum, z42 = 4.96, p = 7.14e-8). Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) indicated the best fitting model order was at 40 (+/− 2.60) ms and 26 (+/− 2.50) ms lags for context versus object sampling periods respectively. The HPC lead over mPFC observed during contextual exploration is consistent with previously demonstrated Granger causal predictions of HPC-mPFC functional connectivity13. Shaded plots represent S.E.M. across sessions. Inset bar graph: Comparison between GC directionality for frequency of peak GC (mean +/− standard error).

Supplementary Figure 4 Movement speed patterns during context exploration and object sampling.

(a) Speed patterns over the entire 10 sec context exploration period and period surrounding object sampling for each trial in an example session. On individual trials rats rapidly enter the context then alternate irregularly between rapid movements and periods of immobility. During object sampling, rats typically sniff standing still then adjust the head to a new position for additional sampling, such that the changes in speed observed during the object sampling period reflect head turning while standing relatively still examining the object. (b) Movement speed pattern for 1 sec context exploration and object sampling periods averaged (+/− SE) for all sessions. Note that “speed” in these periods reflects qualitatively different behaviors when the animal is walking through space or turning its head while still. (c) Scatter plot of PFC-HPC lag/leads and speed over 1 sec context exploration (context) and object sampling (object) periods from all sessions. Lag/lead did not significantly correlate with speed during the context exploration period (vHPC-mPFC: r = 0.06, p = 0.85; dHPC-mPFC: r = 0.009, p = 0.97; combined HPC-mPFC: Pearson r = -0.008, p = 0.96) or the object exploration period (vHPC-mPFC: r = -0.33, p = 0.28; dHPC-mPFC: r = 0.413, p = 0.08; combined HPC-mPFC: r = 0.023, p = 0.90). Note the r-value closest to significance (p = 0.08) hints at a positive correlation suggesting greater mPFC lead over dHPC with increasing speed, which is opposite the direction observed during higher speeds associated with context exploration.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Figures 1–4 and Supplementary Tables 1–3 (PDF 882 kb)

Supplementary Methods Checklist (PDF 471 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Place, R., Farovik, A., Brockmann, M. et al. Bidirectional prefrontal-hippocampal interactions support context-guided memory. Nat Neurosci 19, 992–994 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4327

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4327

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing