Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Neuronal encoding of the switch from specific to generalized fear

Subjects

Abstract

Fear memories are crucial for survival. However, excessive generalization of such memories, characterized by a failure to discriminate dangerous from safe stimuli, is common in anxiety disorders. Neuronal encoding of the transition from cue-specific to generalized fear is poorly understood. We identified distinct neuronal populations in the lateral amygdala (LA) of rats that signaled generalized versus cue-specific associations and determined how their distributions switched during fear generalization. Notably, the same LA neurons that were cue specific before the behavioral shift to generalized fear lost their specificity afterwards, thereby tilting the balance of activity toward a greater proportion of generalizing neurons. Neuronal activity in the LA, but not the auditory cortex, was necessary for fear generalization. Furthermore, targeted activation of cAMP–PKA signaling in the LA increased neuronal excitability of LA neurons and led to generalized fear. These results provide a cellular basis in the amygdala for the alteration of emotional states from normal to pathological fear.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Discriminative conditioning with weak US causes increase in CS+-evoked, but not CS-evoked, spiking in LA neurons.
Figure 2: A strong US causes generalization of conditioned fear and changes the population distribution of LA neurons encoding cue specificity versus generalization.
Figure 3: Neuronal activity in the LA, but not ACx, is necessary for generalization of conditioned fear.
Figure 4: Distinct neuronal transitions when the same animal switched from low to high fear generalization.
Figure 5: Occlusion of further shifts in behavioral and neuronal generalization in animals exhibiting intrinsically higher generalization after weak US conditioning.
Figure 6: Activation of cAMP–PKA signaling in the LA during weak US conditioning is sufficient to cause fear generalization.
Figure 7: Increased spiking at shorter latencies in response to tone and shock after switch to higher generalization.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Quirk, G.J., Repa, J.C. & LeDoux, J.E. Fear conditioning enhances short-latency auditory responses of lateral amygdala neurons: parallel recordings in the freely behaving rat. Neuron 15, 1029–1039 (1995).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Repa, J.C. et al. Two different lateral amygdala cell populations contribute to the initiation and storage of memory. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 724–731 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. LeDoux, J.E. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 155–184 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Shepard, R.N. Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science 237, 1317–1323 (1987).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Resnik, J., Sobel, N. & Paz, R. Auditory aversive learning increases discrimination thresholds. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 791–796 (2011).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Lissek, S. et al. Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Behav. Res. Ther. 43, 1391–1424 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Jovanovic, T., Kazama, A., Bachevalier, J. & Davis, M. Impaired safety signal learning may be a biomarker of PTSD. Neuropharmacology 62, 695–704 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Bryant, R.A. et al. Enhanced amygdala and medial prefrontal activation during nonconscious processing of fear in posttraumatic stress disorder: An fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 517–523 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Protopopescu, X. et al. Differential time courses and specificity of amygdala activity in posttraumatic stress disorder subjects and normal control subjects. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 464–473 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Sheline, Y.I. et al. Increased amygdala response to masked emotional faces in depressed subjects resolves with antidepressant treatment: an fMRI study. Biol. Psychiatry 50, 651–658 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Bremner, J.D. et al. Positron emission tomographic imaging of neural correlates of a fear acquisition and extinction paradigm in women with childhood sexual-abuse-related post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol. Med. 35, 791–806 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Lissek, S. et al. Generalization of conditioned fear-potentiated startle in humans: experimental validation and clinical relevance. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 678–687 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Ghirlanda, S. & Enquist, M. A century of generalization. Anim. Behav. 66, 15–36 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Guttman, N. & Kalish, H.I. Discriminability and stimulus generalization. J. Exp. Psychol. 51, 79 (1956).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Honig, W.K. & Urcuioli, P.J. The legacy of Guttman and Kalish (1956): 25 years of research on stimulus generalization. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 36, 405–445 (1981).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Pavlov, I.P. Conditioned Reflex: an Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex (Dover Publications, 1927).

  17. Jarrell, T.W., Gentile, C.G., Romanski, L.M., McCabe, P.M. & Schneiderman, N. Involvement of cortical and thalamic auditory regions in retention of differential bradycardiac conditioning to acoustic conditioned stimuli in rabbits. Brain Res. 412, 285–294 (1987).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Thompson, R.F. Role of the cerebral cortex in stimulus generalization. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 55, 279 (1962).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Rescorla, R.A. & Wagner, A.R. A Theory of Pavlovian Conditioning: Variations in the Effectiveness of Reinforcement and Nonreinforcement (Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, 1972).

  20. Rescorla, R.A. & Holland, P.C. Behavioral studies of associative learning in animals. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 33, 265–308 (1982).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Baldi, E., Lorenzini, C.A. & Bucherelli, C. Footshock intensity and generalization in contextual and auditory-cued fear conditioning in the rat. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 81, 162–166 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Laxmi, T.R., Stork, O. & Pape, H.-C. Generalization of conditioned fear and its behavioural expression in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 145, 89–98 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Quirk, G.J., Armony, J.L. & LeDoux, J.E. Fear conditioning enhances different temporal components of tone-evoked spike trains in auditory cortex and lateral amygdala. Neuron 19, 613–624 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Weinberger, N.M. Associative representational plasticity in the auditory cortex: a synthesis of two disciplines. Learn. Mem. 14, 1–16 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. LeDoux, J.E., Farb, C. & Ruggiero, D.A. Topographic organization of neurons in the acoustic thalamus that project to the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 10, 1043–1054 (1990).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Romanski, L.M. & LeDoux, J.E. Information cascade from primary auditory cortex to the amygdala: corticocortical and corticoamygdaloid projections of temporal cortex in the rat. Cereb. Cortex 3, 515–532 (1993).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Letzkus, J.J. et al. A disinhibitory microcircuit for associative fear learning in the auditory cortex. Nature 480, 331–335 (2011).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Romanski, L.M. & LeDoux, J.E. Equipotentiality of thalamo-amygdala and thalamo-cortico-amygdala circuits in auditory fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 12, 4501–4509 (1992).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Faber, E.S.L. et al. Modulation of SK channel trafficking by beta adrenoceptors enhances excitatory synaptic transmission and plasticity in the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 28, 10803–10813 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Shaban, H. et al. Generalization of amygdala LTP and conditioned fear in the absence of presynaptic inhibition. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1028–1035 (2006).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Fourcaudot, E. et al. cAMP/PKA signaling and RIM1α mediate presynaptic LTP in the lateral amygdala. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105, 15130–15135 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Pape, H.C., Narayanan, R.T., Smid, J., Stork, O. & Seidenbecher, T. Theta activity in neurons and networks of the amygdala related to long-term fear memory. Hippocampus 15, 874–880 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Antunes, R. & Moita, M.A. Discriminative auditory fear learning requires both tuned and nontuned auditory pathways to the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 30, 9782–9787 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Han, J.-H. et al. Increasing CREB in the auditory thalamus enhances memory and generalization of auditory conditioned fear. Learn. Mem. 15, 443–453 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Hawkins, R.D. Computational Models of Learning in Simple Neural Systems (Academic Press, 1989).

  36. Milad, M.R. et al. Presence and acquired origin of reduced recall for fear extinction in PTSD: results of a twin study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 42, 515–520 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Rumpel, S., LeDoux, J., Zador, A. & Malinow, R. Postsynaptic receptor trafficking underlying a form of associative learning. Science 308, 83–88 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Lonsdorf, T.B. & Kalisch, R. A review on experimental and clinical genetic associations studies on fear conditioning, extinction and cognitive-behavioral treatment. Transl. Psychiatry 1, e41 (2011).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Zhou, Y. et al. CREB regulates excitability and the allocation of memory to subsets of neurons in the amygdala. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1438–1443 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Armony, J.L., Servan-Schreiber, D., Romanski, L.M., Cohen, J.D. & LeDoux, J.E. Stimulus generalization of fear responses: effects of auditory cortex lesions in a computational model and in rats. Cereb. Cortex 7, 157–165 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Paxinos, G. & Watson, C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates (Academic Press, 2006).

  42. Wilensky, A.E., Schafe, G.E., Kristensen, M.P. & LeDoux, J.E. Rethinking the fear circuit: the central nucleus of the amygdala is required for the acquisition, consolidation, and expression of Pavlovian fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 26, 12387–12396 (2006).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Blanchard, D.C. & Blanchard, R.J. Innate and conditioned reactions to threat in rats with amygdaloid lesions. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 81, 281–290 (1972).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Hartigan, J.A. & Hartigan, P.M. The dip test of unimodality. Ann. Stat. 13, 70–84 (1985).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Herry, C. et al. Switching on and off fear by distinct neuronal circuits. Nature 454, 600–606 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Nicolelis, M.A. et al. Chronic, multisite, multielectrode recordings in macaque monkeys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100, 11041–11046 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Wheeler, B.C. Automatic discrimination of single units. in Methods for Neural Ensemble Recordings (ed. Nicolelis, M.A.) 61–77 (CRC Press, 1999).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to A. Lüthi and M. Thattai for discussions and advice. This work was supported by the National Centre for Biological Sciences and Department of Biotechnology, India, and an International Senior Research Fellowship to S.C. (GR 0701339 MA) from The Wellcome Trust, UK.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

S.G. and S.C. designed the experiments. S.G. performed the experiments and analyzed the data. S.G. and S.C. interpreted the results and wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sumantra Chattarji.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1 Cluster sorting for isolation of single units in chronic extracellular recordings from the LA

(a-c) Sorted clusters from a single electrode on consecutive days based upon principal component scores. (d) Cluster quality based upon J3 and Davies-Bouldin index (DB) statistics. (e) Cluster stability across recording sessions was estimated by Mahalanobis-distance between the waveforms.

Supplementary Figure 2 Classification of cue-specific and generalizing neurons using a bootstrap resampling method

(a, c) Scatter plots showing re-sampled (n = 1,000) z-scores averaged over 5 trials over a duration of 200 ms after tone-onset during the habituation (gray) and testing (blue) sessions for a representative Cue-specific (a) and Generalizing (c) neuron. In each re-sampling, five trials were picked up randomly with replacement. Line plots on top and bottom right depict the distribution of the z-scores for CS+ and CS respectively. (b, d) Re-sampled normalized z-scores (bottom left) for the representative neurons shown in (a) and (c). Distributions of responses evoked by CS+ (top) and CS (bottom right) showing a CS+-specific (b) and non-specific (d) increase (>1) after conditioning.

Supplementary Figure 3 Placement of infusion cannulae into the LA

(a) Representative coronal brain section stained with cresyl violet (0.2%), indicating bilateral cannulae tacks, implanted chronically for drug infusion (muscimol or forskolin) into the LA. (b) Schematic representations of coronal sections showing cannulae placements (blue lines) in the LA for all experimental animals (N = 27).

Supplementary Figure 4 Infusion of muscimol into the auditory cortex (ACx) prior to conditioning reversibly blocks neuronal spiking

(a) Representative coronal sections showing the infusion site and spread of a fluorescently labeled muscimol (MW, 607 g) across most of the ACx. Dose: 1.0 μl/side, 10 mM. (b) Simultaneous in vivo recording of neuronal spiking and muscimol infusion in the ACx, indicating inactivation of spontaneous firing 20 min after the infusion (1.0 μl/side, 1 μg/μl), which recovered 24 h later [at a time point when fear memory was tested in conditioned animals (Fig. 3)].

Supplementary Figure 5 Placement of infusion cannulae into the ACx for muscimol experiments

Schematic representations of coronal sections showing cannulae placements (blue lines) in the ACx for all experimental animals (N = 6).

Supplementary Figure 6 Transitions in the responses of LA neurons to CS+ and CS over the course of enhanced generalization in the same animal.

Raster plots (top) and peristimulus time histograms (bottom) of a LA neuron during the behavioral switch from low (testing, day 2) to high (testing, day 3) generalized fear. A neuron that did not change its response after weak-US conditioning (i.e. Non-conditioned cell, day 2), fired more strongly to the CS+ than CS after strong-US conditioning (i.e. Cue-specific, day 3). Brown bar indicates presentation of the auditory tone (bin size, 20 ms). Insets show superimposed spike waveforms.

Supplementary Figure 7 Reconditioning with the same weak US does not cause generalization at the behavioral and neuronal levels.

(a) Experimental protocol. The same animals were conditioned twice with the same intensity of weak-US (0.5 mA). (b) Mean freezing levels (N = 6) in the same rats. During habituation, CS+ and CS elicited equally low levels of freezing (grey square). During testing, 1 day after weak-US conditioning, only the CS+, evoked significantly higher freezing compared to habituation (solid blue square; p < 0.01) and CS (p < 0.05). Reconditioning with the same weak-US failed to cause any further significant increase in freezing values to either tone (open blue square; p > 0.05). Average IBG values (inset) were low after weak-US conditioning and additional session of weak-US conditioning failed to cause any further change (p > 0.05). (c) Scatter plots illustrating population distribution of all LA neurons (n = 51, N = 6) based on their normalized responses to the CS+ and CS, after 1st weak and 2nd weak-US conditioning. (d) Pie-plots illustrating non-significant shifts in population distribution of neuronal responses in the LA during the 1st weak-US to 2nd weak-US reconditioning [Generalizing: 8% (4/51) → 6% (3/51), Cue-specific: 42% (21/51) → 46% (23/51); n = 51, χ2(2) = 0.25, p = 0.88]. (e) Population responses of all cue-specific and generalizing neurons during habituation and testing trials (n = 26/51). Weak-US conditioning induced a significant increase in the response to the CS+ (p < 0.01) and but not CS. The two responses were also significantly different, and this was unchanged even after 2nd weak-US conditioning (p < 0.01). Brown horizontal bar, tone-presentation. Bin size, 20 ms. Error bars, ±s.e.m.

Source data

Supplementary Figure 8 Spontaneous firing in LA neurons was greater in rats exhibiting high (N = 6; Fig. 5) levels of fear generalization compared to those showing low (N = 8; Fig. 4) fear generalization after weak US conditioning.

Cumulative probability distribution of spontaneous firing rates of all recorded LA neurons (solid lines) and only those showing conditioning-induced increase in firing (dotted lines) from rats exhibiting low (grey, n = 131, N = 8) and high (black, n = 114, N = 6) fear generalization. A subset of all the recorded LA neurons exhibited tone-evoked increased spiking after weak-US conditioning from low (grey, n = 37/131) and high (black, n = 43/114) generalizing rats. The frequency of spontaneous firing in all the recorded neurons (k = 0.28; p < 0.001, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test), as well as the smaller subset of conditioned neurons (k = 0.33; p = 0.02, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test), are significantly greater in the rats exhibiting high fear generalization compared to those showing low fear generalization. Spontaneous firing rate is estimated over 100 s pre-tone period (10 s, 10 trials) during habituation session. ***p < 0.001; *p< 0.05.

Source data

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Figures 1–8 and Supplementary Table 1 (PDF 3657 kb)

Supplementary Methods Checklist (PDF 139 kb)

Source data

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ghosh, S., Chattarji, S. Neuronal encoding of the switch from specific to generalized fear. Nat Neurosci 18, 112–120 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3888

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3888

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing