Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Reactivation of emergent task-related ensembles during slow-wave sleep after neuroprosthetic learning

Abstract

Brain-machine interfaces can allow neural control over assistive devices. They also provide an important platform for studying neural plasticity. Recent studies have suggested that optimal engagement of learning is essential for robust neuroprosthetic control. However, little is known about the neural processes that may consolidate a neuroprosthetic skill. On the basis of the growing body of evidence linking slow-wave activity (SWA) during sleep to consolidation, we examined whether there is 'offline' processing after neuroprosthetic learning. Using a rodent model, we found that, after successful learning, task-related units specifically experienced increased locking and coherency to SWA during sleep. Moreover, spike-spike coherence among these units was substantially enhanced. These changes were not present with poor skill acquisition or after control awake periods, demonstrating the specificity of our observations to learning. Notably, the time spent in SWA predicted the performance gains. Thus, SWA appears to be involved in offline processing after neuroprosthetic learning.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Direct control of motor cortex units.
Figure 2: Changes in phase-locking and coherent spiking during SWS after learning.
Figure 3: Changes in SFC after learning.
Figure 4: Changes in SSC after learning.
Figure 5: Lack of increase in coherent spiking with poor learning and in control sleep.
Figure 6: Changes in reactivation strength after learning.
Figure 7: Continued task performance after sleep.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Chapin, J.K., Moxon, K.A., Markowitz, R.S. & Nicolelis, M.A. Real-time control of a robot arm using simultaneously recorded neurons in the motor cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 664–670 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Koralek, A.C., Jin, X., Long, J.D. II, Costa, R.M. & Carmena, J.M. Corticostriatal plasticity is necessary for learning intentional neuroprosthetic skills. Nature 483, 331–335 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Arduin, P.J., Fregnac, Y., Shulz, D.E. & Ego–Stengel, V. “Master” neurons induced by operant conditioning in rat motor cortex during a brain-machine interface task. J. Neurosci. 33, 8308–8320 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Carmena, J.M. et al. Learning to control a brain-machine interface for reaching and grasping by primates. PLoS Biol. 1, E42 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Serruya, M.D., Hatsopoulos, N.G., Paninski, L., Fellows, M.R. & Donoghue, J.P. Instant neural control of a movement signal. Nature 416, 141–142 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Taylor, D.M., Tillery, S.I. & Schwartz, A.B. Direct cortical control of 3D neuroprosthetic devices. Science 296, 1829–1832 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Santhanam, G., Ryu, S.I., Yu, B.M., Afshar, A. & Shenoy, K.V. A high-performance brain-computer interface. Nature 442, 195–198 (2006).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Moritz, C.T., Perlmutter, S.I. & Fetz, E.E. Direct control of paralyzed muscles by cortical neurons. Nature 456, 639–642 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Jarosiewicz, B. et al. Functional network reorganization during learning in a brain-computer interface paradigm. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105, 19486–19491 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Musallam, S., Corneil, B.D., Greger, B., Scherberger, H. & Andersen, R.A. Cognitive control signals for neural prosthetics. Science 305, 258–262 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Hochberg, L.R. et al. Neuronal ensemble control of prosthetic devices by a human with tetraplegia. Nature 442, 164–171 (2006).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Hochberg, L.R. et al. Reach and grasp by people with tetraplegia using a neurally controlled robotic arm. Nature 485, 372–375 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Collinger, J.L. et al. High-performance neuroprosthetic control by an individual with tetraplegia. Lancet 381, 557–564 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Ganguly, K. & Carmena, J.M. Emergence of a stable cortical map for neuroprosthetic control. PLoS Biol. 7, e1000153 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Gilja, V. et al. A high-performance neural prosthesis enabled by control algorithm design. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 1752–1757 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Stickgold, R. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature 437, 1272–1278 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Diekelmann, S. & Born, J. The memory function of sleep. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 114–126 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Peigneux, P., Laureys, S., Delbeuck, X. & Maquet, P. Sleeping brain, learning brain. The role of sleep for memory systems. Neuroreport 12, A111–A124 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Lee, A.K. & Wilson, M.A. Memory of sequential experience in the hippocampus during slow wave sleep. Neuron 36, 1183–1194 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Walker, M.P., Brakefield, T., Morgan, A., Hobson, J.A. & Stickgold, R. Practice with sleep makes perfect: sleep-dependent motor skill learning. Neuron 35, 205–211 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Fischer, S., Nitschke, M.F., Melchert, U.H., Erdmann, C. & Born, J. Motor memory consolidation in sleep shapes more effective neuronal representations. J. Neurosci. 25, 11248–11255 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Cohen, D.A., Pascual–Leone, A., Press, D.Z. & Robertson, E.M. Off-line learning of motor skill memory: a double dissociation of goal and movement. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 18237–18241 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Fischer, S., Hallschmid, M., Elsner, A.L. & Born, J. Sleep forms memory for finger skills. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 99, 11987–11991 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Korman, M. et al. Daytime sleep condenses the time course of motor memory consolidation. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1206–1213 (2007).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Walker, M.P., Brakefield, T., Hobson, J.A. & Stickgold, R. Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature 425, 616–620 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Huber, R., Ghilardi, M.F., Massimini, M. & Tononi, G. Local sleep and learning. Nature 430, 78–81 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Tamaki, M. et al. Enhanced spontaneous oscillations in the supplementary motor area are associated with sleep-dependent offline learning of finger-tapping motor-sequence task. J. Neurosci. 33, 13894–13902 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Huber, R. et al. Arm immobilization causes cortical plastic changes and locally decreases sleep slow wave activity. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1169–1176 (2006).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Hanlon, E.C., Faraguna, U., Vyazovskiy, V.V., Tononi, G. & Cirelli, C. Effects of skilled training on sleep slow wave activity and cortical gene expression in the rat. Sleep 32, 719–729 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Moroni, F. et al. Procedural learning and sleep hippocampal low frequencies in humans. Neuroimage 42, 911–918 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Inostroza, M. & Born, J. Sleep for preserving and transforming episodic memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 36, 79–102 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Ganguly, K., Dimitrov, D.F., Wallis, J.D. & Carmena, J.M. Reversible large-scale modification of cortical networks during neuroprosthetic control. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 662–667 (2011).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Fetz, E.E. Volitional control of neural activity: implications for brain-computer interfaces. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 579, 571–579 (2007).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Koralek, A.C., Costa, R.M. & Carmena, J.M. Temporally precise cell-specific coherence develops in corticostriatal networks during learning. Neuron 79, 865–872 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Peyrache, A., Khamassi, M., Benchenane, K., Wiener, S.I. & Battaglia, F.P. Replay of rule-learning related neural patterns in the prefrontal cortex during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 919–926 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Peyrache, A., Benchenane, K., Khamassi, M., Wiener, S.I. & Battaglia, F.P. Principal component analysis of ensemble recordings reveals cell assemblies at high temporal resolution. J. Comput. Neurosci. 29, 309–325 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Lopes-dos-Santos, V., Ribeiro, S. & Tort, A.B. Detecting cell assemblies in large neuronal populations. J. Neurosci. Methods 220, 149–166 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Tononi, G. & Cirelli, C. Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 81, 12–34 (2014).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Marshall, L. & Born, J. The contribution of sleep to hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 442–450 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Pavlides, C. & Winson, J. Influences of hippocampal place cell firing in the awake state on the activity of these cells during subsequent sleep episodes. J. Neurosci. 9, 2907–2918 (1989).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Wilson, M.A. & McNaughton, B.L. Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science 265, 676–679 (1994).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Buzsaki, G. Two-stage model of memory trace formation: a role for 'noisy' brain states. Neuroscience 31, 551–570 (1989).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Rasch, B., Buchel, C., Gais, S. & Born, J. Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science 315, 1426–1429 (2007).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Jadhav, S.P., Kemere, C., German, P.W. & Frank, L.M. Awake hippocampal sharp-wave ripples support spatial memory. Science 336, 1454–1458 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Vyazovskiy, V., Borbely, A.A. & Tobler, I. Unilateral vibrissae stimulation during waking induces interhemispheric EEG asymmetry during subsequent sleep in the rat. J. Sleep Res. 9, 367–371 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Hill, D.N., Mehta, S.B. & Kleinfeld, D. Quality metrics to accompany spike sorting of extracellular signals. J. Neurosci. 31, 8699–8705 (2011).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Fee, M.S., Mitra, P.P. & Kleinfeld, D. Automatic sorting of multiple unit neuronal signals in the presence of anisotropic and non–Gaussian variability. J. Neurosci. Methods 69, 175–188 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Bjorvatn, B., Fagerland, S. & Ursin, R. EEG power densities (0.5–20 Hz) in different sleep-wake stages in rats. Physiol. Behav. 63, 413–417 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Phillips, K.G. et al. Decoupling of sleep-dependent cortical and hippocampal interactions in a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia. Neuron 76, 526–533 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Pack, A.I. et al. Novel method for high-throughput phenotyping of sleep in mice. Physiol. Genomics 28, 232–238 (2007).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Mitra, P. & Bokil, H. Observed Brain Dynamics (Oxford University Press, 2008).

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Department of Veterans Affairs (B6674 to K.G.), the Burroughs Wellcome Fund (1009855 to K.G.), the American Heart/Stroke Association (0875016N to K.G.) and a VA Psychiatric Research Advanced Fellowship (to D.S.R.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

C.C.W., T.G. and K.G. conducted the experiments. T.G. and D.S.R. analyzed the data. K.G. supervised the study. All of the authors contributed to the writing and editing of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karunesh Ganguly.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1 Single-unit waveforms and interspike interval (ISI) from microwire arrays and tetrodes and characteristics of recorded ensembles.

a, Left panel shows 100 waveforms of the task related (tri and trd) and task unrelated (tu) units shown in Fig. 1d (conventions are same). Right panel shows ISI histograms for the corresponding units in the left panel. Scales are same for all waveforms. b, Spike–width calculation for identifying putative interneurons and pyramidal cell types. W1, W2 and W3 are widths of three units between the peaks marked by shaded grey lines. c, Histogram showing counts of spike widths for each of the three category of units (bin size = 10 µs). d, Spike widths for the three categories of units (mean ± s.e.m., one–way ANOVA, F2,171 = 0.55, P > 0.05). e, Relationship between the SWSpre and SWSpost firing rates for each of the three category of units. Also plotted is the linear regression fit for trd units (slope, m= 0.89,R2 =0.89, P < 1 × 10−6). f, Firing rates of the three categories of units in SWS prior to learning the task (mean ± s.e.m., one–way ANOVA, F2,171 = 23, P > 0.05). g, Firing rates of three categories of units in SWS after learning (mean ± s.e.m., one–way ANOVA, F2,171 = 2, P > 0.05). h, Example of clusters based on amplitude feature from two channels. i, Example of one tetrode with 3 units, each of which had no RPVs (Refractory Period Violations). Shown are the waveforms, autocorrelation, cross–correlation, and linear discriminant analysis (LDA). There was no evidence of a relationship in cross–correlations and clear separation of units according to Fisher’s LDA, suggesting over sorting had not occurred. j, Respective STA averages from experiments conducted with either tetrode or microelectrode arrays. The distributions were not significantly different (mean ± s.e.m., t–test, t25 = –0.05, P > 0.05).

Supplementary Figure 2 Comparison of the modulation depth and post-learning effects for negative and positive weight units and changes in STA during task performance.

a, Comparison of the post–learning firing rates of a single positive–weight unit (trd+) and a single negative–weight unit (trd). b, Average depth of modulation for trd+ and trd units (mean ± s.e.m., t–test, t25 = 5.17, P <1 × 10−4). c, Distribution of the modulation depths for all trd+ and trd units (bin size = 5%). d, Mean STA amplitude change for the respective population of trd+ and trd units (mean ± s.e.m., t–test, t25 = 3.37, P < 0.005). e, Top panel compares the STA of a task related (trd) unit during early (blue) and late (red) trials during task performance/learning in a single session. This STA is based on ‘Beta’ band filtered LFP (i.e. 12–40 Hz). In contrast, we did not observe significant changes in other bands (e.g. SWA band, theta, alpha, higher frequencies) as shown in the unfiltered STA beneath it. f, This shows the population averages (mean ± s.e.m.) of the change in STA amplitude when comparing early and late trials from robust learning sessions. This indicates a significant drop in locking of spiking to the Beta band for task related units. Reduced locking of units to this frequency is consistent with the broader notion of ‘movement–related desynchronization’ that is commonly observed with movements (Murthy, V.N. & Fetz, E.E, 1992. Coherent 25– to 35–Hz oscillations in the sensorimotor cortex of awake behaving monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89, 5670–4) or neuroprosthetic control (McFarland, D.J., Krusienski, D.J. & Wolpaw, J.R, 2006. Brain–computer interface signal processing at the Wadsworth Center: mu and sensorimotor beta rhythms. Prog Brain Res 159, 411–9). Interestingly, tu units did not display this relationship (one–way ANOVA, F2,171 = 36, P < 1 × 10−13with significant post hoc t–tests marked by ‘*’, P < 0.05).

Supplementary Figure 3 Hypnogram.

a, Top panel depicts sleep and awake transitions. Bottom panelsshow the respective LFP activity/EMG activity. b, Comparison of SWS duration for all of the pre– and post– robust learning sessions (mean ± s.e.m., t–test, t14 = –0.02, P = 0.98).

Supplementary Figure 4 Changes in locking to spindle and ripple frequency bands.

a, Left panel shows the STA of a trd unit in the ripple band (100–300Hz) in SWSpre and SWSpost. Right panel illustrates the mean ± s.e.m. change in STA for the three classes of units (one–way ANOVA, F2,171 = 0.27, P = 0.75). b, Left panel shows the STAs in the spindle band (8–20Hz). Right panel shows the mean ± s.e.m. changes in STAs for the three unit classes (one–way ANOVA, F2,171 = 0.35, P = 0.69).

Supplementary Figure 5 Firing characteristics prior to and after learning.

a, Interspike–interval histograms of a task–related unit during SWSpre and SWSpost. These were based on spiking that occurred during the SWA negativity. The two distributions are not significantly different from each other (KS test, P> 0.05), indicating no significant change in bursting. b, Comparison of the ISI distributions for SWSpre and SWSpost. Each bar shows the percentage of units that did not experience a significant change in the ISI distribution. c, Comparison of the delta wave density in SWSpre and SWSpost for robust learning sessions (mean ± s.e.m., t–test, t14 = –1.48, P = 0.16).

Supplementary Figure 6 ON/OFF periods across LFP channels.

a, Grid shows task related and unrelated units at various sites on the microwire probe. Segments of the SWSpost LFP are shown for a variety of electrode recording sites. b, Bar plot showing temporal lags (i.e. temporal delay in milliseconds, mean ± s.e.m) between OFF periods for channels with trd and ‘distant’ tu units (i.e. at least two channels away from a trd), t–test, t13 = –0.30, P = 0.77. c, Bar plot showing density of OFF periods in the channels with trd and distant tu units (mean ± s.e.m., t–test, t13 = –0.30, P = 0.76).

Supplementary Figure 7 SFC magnitude and phase after learning.

a, Plot of the full SFC spectrum as a function of frequency before (blue) and after (red) skill acquisition for a trd and tu unit (0–20Hz band is shown in Fig 3). b, Distribution of phases of trd, tri and tu units are shown in pre and post–SWS in a polar plot. Mean phase of trd units is shown in bold blue (before) and red (after) vector. The phases were uniformly distributed and didn't change significantly for each of the comparisons for the three classes of units between SWSpre and SWSpost (watson–williams circular t-test, P > 0.05).

Supplementary Figure 8 Averaged cross-correlation histograms (CCH).

The mean of the peak–to–tail ratio for each of the three categories (trdtrd, trdtri and trdtu neuronal pairs) in their CCHs (one–way ANOVA, F2,156 = 27, P < 1 × 10−10 with significant post hoc t–tests marked by ‘*’, P < 0.05).

Supplementary Figure 9 Changes in reactivation strength during awake versus SWS.

a, Reactivation strength of the signal component during SWSpre, Awakepost (shown in gray in right panel) and SWSpost. b, Mean population difference histograms of pre– and post– reactivation strengths for the awake and SWS period. In each case the respective histogram of SWSpre was subtracted from the respective awake and SWSpost periods. Also shown is the mean population difference curve for poor learning sessions. There was a significantly greater reactivation during post–sleep (logrank test, ‘*’ indicates P < 0.05). c, Event triggered average of reactivation strength centered on the maximum delta wave negativity for SWSpost (red) and awake (purple) sessions. d, Comparison of the change in event triggered negativity for the Awakepost and SWSpost time periods. The relative reactivation compares the baseline (time less than 0) to the peak (mean ± s.e.m., t–test, t17 = –3.61, P < 0.005).

Supplementary Figure 10 Task improvements after sleep.

Average time to reward for the 30 best trials during the first BMI training session (i.e. Block1best) compared to the beginning of second session (Block2begin),overall t-test, t7 = 4.38, P < 0.005.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Figures 1–10 and Supplementary Table 1 (PDF 1253 kb)

Supplementary Methods Checklist (PDF 467 kb)

Source data

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gulati, T., Ramanathan, D., Wong, C. et al. Reactivation of emergent task-related ensembles during slow-wave sleep after neuroprosthetic learning. Nat Neurosci 17, 1107–1113 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3759

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3759

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing