Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Brief Communication
  • Published:

Rovers minimize human disturbance in research on wild animals

Abstract

Investigating wild animals while minimizing human disturbance remains an important methodological challenge. When approached by a remote-operated vehicle (rover) which can be equipped to make radio-frequency identifications, wild penguins had significantly lower and shorter stress responses (determined by heart rate and behavior) than when approached by humans. Upon immobilization, the rover—unlike humans—did not disorganize colony structure, and stress rapidly ceased. Thus, rovers can reduce human disturbance of wild animals and the resulting scientific bias.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Stress responses of wild king penguins when approached by a human or a rover.
Figure 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Ellenberg, U., Mattern, T., Seddon, P.J. & Jorquera, G.L. Biol. Conserv. 133, 95–106 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. French, S.S., DeNardo, D.F., Greives, T.J., Strand, C.R. & Demas, G.E. Horm. Behav. 58, 792–799 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Viblanc, V.A., Smith, A.D., Gineste, B. & Groscolas, R. BMC Ecol. 12, 10 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Le Maho, Y. et al. Am. J. Physiol. 263, R775–R781 (1992).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Jones, T.T. et al. Meth. Ecol. Evol. 4, 1178–1186 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Culik, B., Wilson, R. & Bannasch, R. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 98, 209–214 (1993).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Saraux, C. et al. Nature 469, 203–206 (2011).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Le Maho, Y. et al. C. R. Biol. 334, 378–384 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Gendner, J.P., Gauthier-Clerc, M., Le Bohec, C., Descamps, S. & Le Maho, Y. J. Field Ornithol. 76, 138–142 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Nicolaus, M., Bouwman, K. & Dingemanse, N. Ardea 96, 286–292 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. O'Shea, T.J., Ellison, L.E. & Stanley, T.R. J. Mamm. 92, 433–443 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Caswell, J.H., Alisauskas, R.T. & Leafloor, J.O. J. Wildl. Mgmt. 76, 1456–1461 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Chiaradia, A.F. & Kerry, K.R. Mar. Ornithol. 27, 13–20 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  14. Johnston, P., Bérubé, F. & Bergeron, N.E. J. Fish Biol. 74, 1651–1661 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Grémillet, D., Puech, W., Garçon, V., Boulinier, T. & Le Maho, Y. Open Ecol. J. 2, 49–57 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Viblanc, V.A., Valette, V., Kauffmann, M., Malosse, N. & Groscolas, R. Behav. Ecol. 23, 1178–1185 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Nimon, A.J., Schroter, R.C. & Oxenham, R.K.C. Physiol. Behav. 60, 1019–1022 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Nephew, B.C., Kahn, S.A. & Romero, L.M. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 133, 173–180 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Cyr, N.E., Dickens, M.J. & Romero, L.M. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 82, 332–344 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Groscolas, R., Viera, V., Guerin, N., Handrich, Y. & Côté, S.D. J. Exp. Biol. 213, 153–160 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Viera, V.M., Viblanc, V.A., Filippi-Codaccioni, O., Côté, S.D. & Groscolas, R. Anim. Behav. 82, 69–76 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Lengagne, T., Jouventin, P. & Aubin, T. Behaviour 136, 833–846 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Jouventin, P. La Terre et La Vie 25, 510–586 (1971).

    Google Scholar 

  24. Gutzwiller, K.J. & Marcum, H.A. J. Wildl. Mgmt. 61, 935–947 (1997).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Peltier, M.R., Wilcox, C.J. & Sharp, D.C. J. Anim. Sci. 76, 847–849 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was undertaken within Program 137 of the Institut Polaire Français Paul-Emile Victor (IPEV), with support from the French National Research Agency (A.N.R.) “PICASO” grant. We are very grateful to the mine-clearing services of the French Ministry of the Interior for the gift of a mine-clearing rover for preliminary tests. All technical developments were supported by Total Corporate Foundation. M. Brucker, N.C., J.C., F.C., B.F., R.G. and Y.L.M. were supported by the CNRS. C.L.B. and P.T. were supported by Centre Scientifique de Monaco (CSM). V.A.V. was supported by the AXA Research Fund. Y.L.M., C.L.B., P.T. and J.D.W. collaborated under the framework of the Laboratoire International Associé 647 BioSensib between the Institut Pluridisciplinaire Hubert Curien (CNRS and University of Strasbourg) and the CSM.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Y.L.M., team leader of the IPEV project, designed and performed the study in the field, worked on the analyses and cowrote the paper; C.L.B., project co-leader, and J.D.W. worked on the analyses and cowrote the paper; N.H. and L.P. worked on the analyses and provided useful comments; technical developments pertaining to rover conception, development and construction were performed by M. Brucker, N.C., J.C., F.C. and B.F.; M. Boureau, L.K., E.G. and N.V. performed the study in the field and provided useful comments; R.G. suggested the use of HR monitoring to test the impact of approaching rovers, proposed a field test, performed some analyses and helped in revising the paper; B.T. helped in interpreting data within an ethological framework and in revising the paper; F.O. added useful modifications to the manuscript; V.A.V. and C.S. ran some preanalyses and greatly contributed in the paper's revision; P.T. participated in a prestudy on the field and added useful modifications to the manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Yvon Le Maho or Céline Le Bohec.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1 Rover camouflaged with an emperor penguin chick model “huddling” with chicks in a crèche.

The rover was able to infiltrate the emperor penguin crèche without disturbance, thus demonstrating the possibility to use such a camouflaged rover to collect data from locations within a colony that are not accessible to a human investigator.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Figure 1 (PDF 156 kb)

King penguin circulating among incubating breeders

Incubating king penguins are stationary, generally maintaining neighbors and birds in transit at a minimum distance determined by the reach of their beak and flippers. (MOV 19948 kb)

Rover circulating among incubating breeders

The reactions of incubating king penguins to the rover was similar in magnitude to their reactions to conspecifics and ceased as soon as the rover stopped moving. (MOV 30018 kb)

Rover approaching southern elephant seals on a beach

Resting southern elephant seals let the rover approach to within contact distances to their heads and also to their tails, where they are usually tagged, with no visible disturbance. (MOV 26114 kb)

Source data

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Le Maho, Y., Whittington, J., Hanuise, N. et al. Rovers minimize human disturbance in research on wild animals. Nat Methods 11, 1242–1244 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.3173

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.3173

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing Anthropocene

Sign up for the Nature Briefing: Anthropocene newsletter — what matters in anthropocene research, free to your inbox weekly.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing: Anthropocene