Introduction

Many organisms have evolved the ability to sense and exploit the Earth’s magnetic field, particularly for navigation and orientation1. Three main models for magnetosensing have been promoted. Magnetic induction, which can only be applied to marine creatures, owing to the high conductivity of salt water1,2, the magnetite hypothesis that proposes a process mediated by crystals of permanently magnetic material (magnetite)1 and finally the radical pair mechanism (RPM), which relies on a chemical reaction involving specialized photoreceptors3,4.

In the RPM, the first step of the reaction requires absorption of a photon by the pigment molecule, leading to the transient formation of a radical pair of electrons in an overall singlet state (antiparallel spin orientation), in which the two unpaired electrons are at a suitable distance to undergo transition to the triplet state (parallel orientation). This transition may be sensitive to an electromagnetic field (EMF), altering the singlet-triplet balance. Return to the ground state can only occur from the singlet state, hence EMFs may alter the lifetime of the radical pair and any signal that it generates3,4. So far, the only photopigments proposed as putative candidates for the RPM are the cryptochromes (CRYs). These blue-light-sensing flavoproteins evolved from photolyases and are highly conserved across many different taxa5. CRYs are expressed in the eyes of mammals6 and migratory birds7, which are putative sites for magnetoreceptors in vertebrates8. In animals, CRYs also function as circadian photoreceptors in the Drosophila brain, mediating the light resetting of the 24 h clock9, but in vertebrates, the CRYs act as the main negative regulators for the circadian feedback loop10. The major difference between fly and vertebrate CRYs is that the former (type 1) are photosensitive, whereas the latter (type 2) are not11. Non-drosophilid insects can also encode CRY1 and CRY2’s, but CRY1s retain their light-sensing properties, whereas the CRY2s act as vertebrate-like negative regulators12.

Previous genetic analyses in Drosophila have suggested a CRY-dependent ability for magnetosensing13,14, whereas other fly studies have done so indirectly by utilizing wavelengths of light to which CRYs are sensitive15,16,17. The two experimental paradigms that utilized cry mutations in flies include a conditioning13,18 and a circadian behavioural assay14. In these studies, CRYs have been implicated as mediators of the fly’s EMF responses in a wavelength-dependent manner. Surprisingly, fly transformants carrying the hCry2 transgene can also detect EMFs in the conditioning assay, suggesting that in the fly’s cellular environment, hCRY2 can be activated by light19. In addition, mutations of the terminal Trp residue, which forms the Trp triad believed to be important for mediating radical pair formation20, does not disrupt the EMF conditioning response, indicating that an unorthodox CRY-dependent EMF-sensing mechanism may be responsible18. Finally, although the CRYs implicate the circadian clock in magnetosensitivity, a working clock is not required for EMF responses in the fly conditioning assay18.

In the conditioning assay, the EMF behavioural effects are modest but consistent13,18,19, whereas the circadian period changes induced by EMF under blue constant light are highly variable, leading to shorter or longer periods in half the flies, and no response at all in the other half14. We therefore sought a different fly behavioural assay that might respond to EMFs with more marked and robust changes. Negative geotaxis in flies (their ability to climb against gravity) has been studied by both traditional quantitative genetic and modern genomic methods21. Artificial selection for flies that show high and low levels of geotaxis has been allied to transcriptomic analyses to reveal that CRY may play a significant role in this phenotype21, and CRY’s role in fly climbing behaviour has recently been confirmed22. We therefore suspected that this phenotype could be wavelength dependent and if so, might be compromised by applying an EMF. We show here that negative geotaxis is blue-light and CRY dependent and is significantly compromised by the application of a static EMF. We further reveal that the CRY C terminus is critical for mediating the effects of the EMF, and that CRY expression in specific clock neurons, eyes and antennae contribute to the EMF phenotype. We conclude that negative geotaxis provides a reliable method for studying behavioural responses to EMFs.

Results

Climbing is wavelength- and CRY-dependent

We examined climbing ability as the percentage of flies that could climb 15 cm in 15 s at different wavelengths using a custom-made apparatus (see Methods and Fig. 1). We used either a sham exposure or a static EMF of 500 μT, which although an order of magnitude greater than the Earth’s magnetic field, is an intensity comparable with that used in previous genetic studies of fly EMF sensitivity13,14. Figure 2a reveals that under blue light (450 nm), the proportion of wild-type Canton-S sham ‘climbers’ is significantly higher than in corresponding EMF exposed flies (P=0.0004), whereas in red light (635 nm) climbing is substantially reduced under sham exposure, to levels similar to those of EMF-exposed flies under blue light. We also investigated the cry-null mutant, cry02 in blue light, which reveals responses similar to wild-type flies in red light. We conclude that negative geotaxis requires both blue-light activation and the presence of CRY, and that climbing can be disrupted by a static EMF.

Figure 1: Measuring negative geotaxis under a static EMF.
figure 1

The delivery system for EMFs consists of a double-wrapped coil system (a, top view), and a custom-made swinger apparatus (b, side view) that allows tapping three vials simultaneously with equal force so the flies fall to the bottom of the tube. IR, infrared.

Figure 2: Negative geotaxis is CRY dependent and is sensitive to EMFs.
figure 2

Mean geotactic responses±s.e.m. based on three biological replicates. Orange bars, sham exposed; purple bars, EMF exposed. Asterisks denote results of Duncan’s a posteriori test within genotype after analysis of variance (ANOVA), *P<0.05, **P<0.01, ***P<0.001. The results from Canton-S (CS) and cry02 were used as positive and negative controls for all analyses and bd represent experiments performed only at 450 nm. (a) Response of CS and cry02 exposed to different wavelengths of light. (ANOVA, genotype F2,12=16.48, P=0.00036, exposure F1,12=8.67, P=0.012, G × E interaction F2,12=9.86, P=0.002). Post hoc tests revealed significant differences only between CS in blue light under sham compared with all the other conditions (P<0.001). (b) Responses of clock gal4/80>UAScry genotypes on a cry02 background (ANOVA, genotype F6,28=3.98, P=0.005, exposure F1,28=36.1, P=2 × 10−6, G × E interaction F6,28=3.08, P=0.019. Post hoc tests reveal no significant differences between sham timgal4/UAScry or crygal4/UAScry compared with CS, nor for EMF exposure. For sham, Pdfgal4>UAScry vs cry02 P=0.1, vs CS P=0.009 timgal4crygal80>UAScry vs cry02 P=0.007, vs CS P=0.12; for EMF Pdfgal4>UAScry vs cry02 P=0.06, vs CS P=0.22, timgal4crygal80>UAScry vs cry02 P=0.039, vs CS P=0.16. (c) Responses of eye and antennal genotypes (gal4>UAScry on cry02 background) (ANOVA, genotype F8,36=5.45, P=0.00016, exposure F1,36=99.4, P~0, G × E interaction F8,36=3.25, P=0.007. Post hoc for sham, CS was not significantly different from sham pain, rh5, rh6, R7gal4>UAScry, but JOgal4>UAScry vs cry02 P=0.18, vs CS P=0.0005. For EMF, none of the genotypes were significantly different from CS or cry02). (d) Responses of cry variants driven by timgal4 (ANOVA, genotype F5,24=6.89, P=0.0004, exposure F=1,24=16.8, P=0.0005 and G × E interaction F5,24=4.13, P=0.008. Post hoc sham timgal4>cryΔ vs cry02 P=0.007, vs CS P=0.04, timgal4>cryW342F vs cry02 P=0.02, vs CS P=0.017; for EMF timgal4>cryΔ vs cry02 P=0.01, vs CS P=0.06, timgal4>cryW342F vs cry02 P=0.2, vs CS P=0.33).

Overexpressing CRY rescues EMF responses

We overexpressed fly CRY under GAL4 control, using various circadian clock drivers on a cry02 background. We observe that expressing CRY in most of the major clock neurons, using either the timgal4 driver or in a more restricted crygal4 pattern, restores high levels of climbing in sham, which was significantly reduced in EMF conditions, as in the wild type (Figs 2b and 3a). We also tested the climbing of all the gal4 driver and UAS lines that are used in this study, and all generate normal EMF responses (Fig. 3b). However, when CRY expression is restricted further using the Pdfgal4 driver, which expresses in the lateral ventral (LNv) subset of clock neurons, intermediate levels of climbing are observed that are not further disrupted by EMF (Figs 2b and 3). A similar scenario prevails when the timgal4crygal80 combination is used to drive CRY expression predominantly in the dorsal neurons plus three normally CRY-negative lateral dorsal neurons (LNds)23 with again, levels of climbing observed that are similar to those obtained with the timgal4 and crygal4 drivers, but with no significant reduction of geotaxis under EMF (Figs 2b and 3). In contrast to these restricted patterns of CRY expression, the Mai179gal4 driver that expresses in the LNvs and three CRY-positive LNd neurons23,24 generated intermediate levels of climbing, which are nevertheless susceptible to an EMF. Consequently, it appears that among the canonical clock neurons, it is the three CRY-expressing LNd cells that are required to generate a robust EMF response.

Figure 3: Control genotype responses to sham and EMFs.
figure 3

(a) Sham controls are shown to illustrate which genotypes did not respond (red bars) or did respond (black bars) to EMFs. Mean climbing scores (±s.e.m.) under blue light based on three biological replicates. Repeated measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) (F(17,36)=4.41, P=0.001) and Duncan’s post hoc analysis (*P<0.05, **P<0.01), further reveal which genotypes differ significantly in their sham responses to CS wild type. Note that JO>cry; cry02 have intermediate levels of climbing under sham, yet show an EMF response (Fig. 2c), whereas genotypes with higher levels of sham climbing (Pdf>cry;cry02, timgal4,crygal80>cry;cry02, tim>cryΔ;cry02) do not respond to EMF (Fig. 2b,d). (b) GAL4/UAS controls strains show normal EMF responses. Mean climbing scores (±s.e.m.) under blue light based on three biological replicates. Repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant exposure (F(1,64)=217.52, P=0.0004) but no effect of genotype (F(15,64)=0.818, P=0.65) nor a G × E interaction (F(15,64)=0.60, P=0.86), so all genotypes responded in the same way to the EMF. All strains are in a w1118 genetic background. Duncan’s post hoc *P<0.05, **P<0.01, ***P<0.001.

We also investigated whether major peripheral tissues in the head, namely the eyes and antennae that normally express CRY, could also contribute to EMF sensitivity. The rh5, rh6 and R7gal4 eye-specific rhodopsin drivers all restore normal levels of climbing to cry02 mutants that are significantly reduced under EMF (Figs 2c and 3). To complement these results, the eyes-absent mutant, eya2, which has a complete absence of eyes, shows a significant reduction in climbing and no further reduction under EMF (Figs 2c and 3a). The antennal drivers JOgal4 and painlessgal4 also rescue the sham/EMF response on a cry02 background, in spite of the fact that in JOgal4, the sham level of climbing is significantly reduced compared with Canton-S flies (P=0.0005, Figs 2c and 3a) and no higher than that of eya2. Furthermore, the AntpR mutant, in which antennae are transformed to mesothoracic legs, significantly reduces the climbing score under sham, but does not reduce it further under EMF (Figs 2c and 3a). These results suggest that the eyes and antennae also play significant roles in climbing and in the response to EMFs.

Finally, we expressed a number of cry variants on the cry02 background using timgal4, including human hCry1 and hCry2 transgenes, the latter having been reported to rescue the EMF effect on a conditioning paradigm19. Neither of these transgenes appears capable of rescuing the climbing phenotype beyond that of cry02, so they are not competent to respond to EMF (Fig. 2d). In contrast, a Trp to Phe mutation (cryW342F) of the terminal Trp of CRYs putative ‘Trp triad’, generates intermediate levels of climbing, which are significantly further reduced on EMF exposure (Figs 2d and 3). We also examined the sham/EMF response of the CRY C-terminal deletion mutant CRYΔ, which is constitutively active in both darkness and light25,26. Interestingly, under both sham and EMF conditions, this mutant shows intermediate levels of climbing but with no difference between the two conditions. Therefore, like cryW342F, timgal4>cryΔ retains the ability to climb but in contrast, is not responsive to an EMF, revealing a role for the CRY C terminus in magnetosensing (Figs 2d and 3a).

Discussion

We have observed that Drosophila requires a functional CRY molecule to climb against gravity, confirming the results of two earlier studies that used different measures of negative geotaxis21,22. We have extended these observations by revealing that under blue light, the climbing of wild-type flies exposed to a 500-μT static EMF is significantly reduced compared with sham exposure. The pass/fail nature of our behavioural assay clearly differentiates between the two exposure conditions. In red light, flies exposed to sham or EMF show significantly reduced climbing, very similar to the levels observed under blue light with EMF exposure. Consequently, negative geotaxis is blue-light dependent, thereby implicating the fly’s dedicated circadian photoreceptor, CRY. As red light does not activate CRY, our results imply that EMFs compromise the photoreceptor’s response to blue light. Consistent with this, the cry-null mutant fails to climb in sham conditions, but this ability can be partially or almost fully rescued by overexpressing CRY in a number of different neuronal types that include clock neurons, antennae and eyes. These results suggest that CRY mediates the effects of EMFs, as also revealed in two other behavioural paradigms, a conditioning and a circadian assay13,14,18.

There is, nevertheless, a logical problem in the inference that CRY is the sensor for EMF taken only from the cry-null mutant data, in that the phenotype of climbing is itself CRY dependent, so any mutant that does not climb cannot show a reduction in climbing due to EMF. The same is true for the circadian EMF phenotype, where a change of period under constant dim blue light, which is CRY dependent, has been reported to be further modulated by EMF14. However, in cry mutants, as there is no initial circadian period change in blue light, there is no behavioural substrate for the EMF to modify. Thus cry-null mutants, in themselves, are not informative in these two assays. In contrast, in the conditioning assay, flies of various genetic backgrounds show both positive or negative naive preferences to an EMF, and this can be modulated by association with sucrose leading to an enhanced preference for EMF after training13,18. cry-null flies do not show any preference in the first place indicating they cannot sense the EMF, so they cannot be trained, thus there is no net change in preference after training. Yet a strong indication for the role of cry in this phenotype is provided by the cry-null mutant’s initial inability to sense the EMF, which is independent of the type of cry-null allele and the flies’ genetic background13,18.

With this reservation in mind, perhaps the most convincing support for the CRY-EMF hypothesis in our climbing assay requires a mutant that climbs in sham conditions to near wild-type levels, so that the CRY molecule retains basic geotactic function, yet would climb to similar levels under EMF, reflecting a mutant, suppressed EMF response. One mutation that fulfils this requirement is CRYΔ, which although producing an unstable CRY, retains some residual molecular response to light25,26. This mutant shows intermediate levels of climbing between cry02 and wild type under sham conditions, but does not respond to the EMF by reducing its climbing. The CRY C-terminal region may therefore play a pivotal role in the intracellular signalling of the CRY response to EMF, possibly by modulating downstream protein–protein interactions.

Another mutation cryW342F that substitutes a Phe for the terminal Trp in the putative ‘Trp triad’ that is a candidate for mediating radical pair formation, shows similar levels of climbing to cryΔ, but in sharp contrast, is responsive to EMF. This result echoes the observation that cryW342F is also able to retain EMF sensitivity in the conditioning assay18. Consequently, the terminal CRY Trp342 may not be the critical residue that is a prerequisite for the RPM, and perhaps another residue within that local conformation is involved, perhaps a tyrosine27. While both the climbing and conditioning assays reveal consistent effects for the terminal Trp mutant, the same could not be stated for hCRY2. In the conditioning test, hCRY2 is EMF-sensitive19, but in the climbing assay, hCRY1 and hCRY2 behave very similarly to cry02, suggesting that they are not blue-light responsive, revealing that the ability of hCRY to rescue an EMF response is phenotype dependent.

We also obtained EMF phenotypes when we varied the expression patterns of CRY. Under the control of different clock drivers, we observed that as we reduced expression from timgal4 (expressed in nearly all clock cells) to crygal4 (only CRY-expressing cells), to Pdfgal4 (expressed in LNvs) and timgal4;crygal80 (predominantly dorsal neurons, DNs and three LNds that do not normally express high levels of CRY), we noticed that under sham conditions the proportion of climbers was generally either intermediate between the mutant and wild-type values or not statistically different from the value of the wild type. For example, timgal4;crygal80 gave 38% climbers compared with cry02 16% and wild type, 49%. Yet for the Pdfgal4 and timgal4;crygal80 drivers there were no significant differences between the sham and EMF conditions, so the EMF response had been lost. However, the Mai179gal4 driver, which expresses in the LNvs and three strongly CRY-positive LNd cells23,24, restored the intermediate levels of climbing under sham control as well as the EMF suppression. Comparing this result with that of the timgal4crygal80 combination and Pdfgal4 drivers, it would appear that CRY expressed in the three CRY-positive LNd neurons could be sufficient for restoring both climbing and the EMF responses. The LNd cluster is involved in circadian locomotor responses under light conditions28, providing a rationale for why they may play an important role in the climbing phenotype under blue light. Our results thus provide a new, non-circadian function for the CRY-positive LNds.

The clock neurons are not the only relevant cells for mediating the effects of EMFs. CRY expression in the R8 photoreceptors of pale ommatidia (via rh5gal4), or in the R8 yellow ommatidia29 and the Hofbauer-Buchner eyelet (rh6gal4)30 or in the R7 cell, is sufficient for robust climbing and EMF responses. Johnston’s organ (JO), which is located in the second antennal segment, has been previously implicated in negative geotaxis31 and our results with JOgal4, which expresses specifically in JO31,32, and paingal4, which is more widely expressed in the antennae and some central neurons33, suggest that CRY expression in JO is sufficient for mediating the effects of EMF. Consequently, there are three separate anatomical foci (LNds, eyes and antennae), where CRY expression in any one is sufficient to restore EMF sensitivity to cry mutants. While this might suggest some type of cellular redundancy, severe mutations of the eyes or the antenna, which reduce the climbing response to ~30% do not give a significant further reduction in geotaxis when exposed to EMF. While this might reflect the general behavioural effects of neurological damage in structures that might be required to be intact (even if CRY negative) to generate normal geotactic responses, a similar level of sham climbing is observed in JO>cry; cry02 flies, which are nevertheless significantly disrupted in climbing on EMF exposure (Fig. 2c). Thus an integrative scenario is suggested, where in the anatomical absence of one structure, CRY expression in the other two cannot compensate to generate an EMF response.

In other insects such as the Monarch butterfly, the antennae play a prominent role in orientation and migration34,35,36, but it remains to be seen whether Drosophila’s ability to respond to magnetic fields has any adaptive function. One well-known switch in geotactic behaviour occurs in the late larval stage, whereby larvae that have spent most of their development digging down into food (positive geotaxis) become negatively geotactic in the late 3rd larval instar before they pupate. The adult’s escape response also involves negative geotaxis, yet whether the Earth’s magnetic field (or CRY) plays any role in these adaptive phenotypes has not been studied, to our knowledge. In conclusion, our results have identified a novel and robust CRY-dependent behavioural phenotype in Drosophila that responds to EMFs, and which may be extremely useful for further neurogenetic dissection of the cellular and molecular basis of magnetosensitivity.

Methods

Fly strains

Flies were maintained in LD 12:12 at 25 °C. Canton-S flies, cry02 and all gal4 drivers and mutants (including UAS transgenes) were backcrossed into a w1118 background for 5–7 generations. timGAL4, UAScry24b and UASHAcry and CRYΔ mutants (refs 26, 37) were further crossed into a cry02 background38 using standard balancing techniques. JOgal4 and crygal80 were recombined onto the third chromosome carrying cry02. UASmychCRY1/2 and cryW342F strains were obtained from Steven Reppert (University of Massachusetts). R7gal4, rh5gal4, rh6gal4, R7gal4, AntpR, paingal4 and eya2 strains were obtained from the Bloomington stock centre (IN, USA). Mai179gal4 was obtained from Francois Rouyer (Gif, Paris).

Behavioural apparatus

An EMF delivery system was designed consisting of an aluminium box placed in a temperature-controlled room, containing two double-wrapped (50 windings each) Helmholtz coils39 that allow sham and EMF exposures to be generated. A constant static magnetic field of 500 μT was produced by the coils through a power pack (Fig. 1a). Ten, 2–3-day-old males were placed in a plastic vial and tapped to the bottom by means of a custom-made ‘swinger’ that allowed three vials to be tapped to the bottom simultaneously with exactly equal force (Fig. 1b). An infrared webcam (Logitech) was used to film the flies. Flies that were able to reach a vertical height of 15 cm in 15 s were counted as ‘climbers’, and each tube was tested 10 times, with 30 s between each of the first 5 trials, then after a 15-min rest, another 5 trials were performed. The EMF or sham was applied at random after every group of 10 trials. Each set of 10 trials on the swinger ran three different genotypes simultaneously in the three tubes. Experiments were run at 25 °C either in dim blue (450 nm, 40 nm range) or dim red light (635 nm, 20 nm range) using light-emitting diodes with an intensity at the surface of the vials of 0.25 μW cm−2. Three biological replicates were used for each genotype, and data were analysed using a multifactorial analysis of variance with repeated measures40. All statistical analyses in this study were performed using GraphPad Prism version 6.00 for Windows, (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, CA, USA) and STATISTICA (data analysis software system, version 8.0, StatSoft Inc. 2008).

Additional information

How to cite this article: Fedele, G. et al. An electromagnetic field disrupts negative geotaxis in Drosophila via a CRY-dependent pathway. Nat. Commun. 5:4391 doi: 10.1038/5391 (2014).