Abstract
Seeming public apathy over climate change is often attributed to a deficit in comprehension. The public knows too little science, it is claimed, to understand the evidence or avoid being misled1. Widespread limits on technical reasoning aggravate the problem by forcing citizens to use unreliable cognitive heuristics to assess risk2. We conducted a study to test this account and found no support for it. Members of the public with the highest degrees of science literacy and technical reasoning capacity were not the most concerned about climate change. Rather, they were the ones among whom cultural polarization was greatest. This result suggests that public divisions over climate change stem not from the public’s incomprehension of science but from a distinctive conflict of interest: between the personal interest individuals have in forming beliefs in line with those held by others with whom they share close ties and the collective one they all share in making use of the best available science to promote common welfare.
Access options
Subscribe to Journal
Get full journal access for 1 year
$213.00
only $17.75 per issue
All prices are NET prices.
VAT will be added later in the checkout.
Rent or Buy article
Get time limited or full article access on ReadCube.
from$8.99
All prices are NET prices.
References
- 1.
Pidgeon, N. & Fischhoff, B. The role of social and decision sciences in communicating uncertain climate risks. Nature Clim. Change 1, 35–41 (2011).
- 2.
Sunstein, C. R. On the divergent American reactions to terrorism and climate change. Columbia L. Rev. 107, 503–557 (2007).
- 3.
Weber, E. U. & Stern, P. C. Public understanding of climate change in the United States. Am. Psychol. 66, 315–328 (2011).
- 4.
Douglas, M. & Wildavsky, A. B. Risk and Culture: An Essay on the Selection of Technical and Environmental Dangers (Univ. California Press, 1982).
- 5.
Kahan, D. M., Braman, D., Slovic, P., Gastil, J. & Cohen, G. Cultural cognition of the risks and benefits of nanotechnology. Nature Nanotech. 4, 87–91 (2009).
- 6.
Kahan, D. M., Jenkins-Smith, H. & Braman, D. Cultural cognition of scientific consensus. J. Risk Res. 14, 147–174 (2011).
- 7.
Pearl, J. Causality: Models, Reasoning, and Inference (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2009).
- 8.
Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., Sunde, U., Schupp, J. & Wagner, G. G. Individual risk attitudes: Measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. J. Eur. Econ. Assoc. 9, 522–550 (2011).
- 9.
Ganzach, Y., Ellis, S., Pazy, A. & Ricci-Siag, T. On the perception and operationalization of risk perception. Judgment Decis. Making 3, 317–324 (2008).
- 10.
Kahneman, D. Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioral economics. Am. Econ. Rev. 93, 1449–1475 (2003).
- 11.
Peters, E., Västfjäll, D., Slovic, P., Mertz, C. K., Mazzocco, K. & Dickert, S. Numeracy and decision making. Psychol. Sci. 17, 407–413 (2006).
- 12.
Liberali, J. M., Reyna, V. F., Furlan, S., Stein, L. M. & Pardo, S. T. Individual differences in numeracy and cognitive reflection, with implications for biases and fallacies in probability judgment. J. Behav. Decis. Makinghttp://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bdm.752 (2011).
- 13.
McCright, A. M. & Dunlap, R. E. Cool dudes: The denial of climate change among conservative white males in the United States. Glob. Environ. Change 21, 1163–1172 (2011).
- 14.
Krosnick, J. A., Holbrook, A. L. & Visser, P. S. The impact of the fall 1997 debate about global warming on American public opinion. Public Underst. Sci. 9, 239–260 (2000).
- 15.
Sunstein, C. R. Misfearing: A reply. Harv. L. Rev. 119, 1110–1125 (2006).
- 16.
Kellstedt, P. M., Zahran, S. & Vedlitz, A. Personal efficacy, the information environment, and attitudes toward global warming and climate change in the United States. Risk Anal. 28, 113–126 (2008).
- 17.
Slovic, P. Trust, emotion, sex, politics, and science: Surveying the risk-assessment battlefield. Risk Anal. 19, 689–701 (1999).
- 18.
Cohen, G. L. Party over policy: The dominating impact of group influence on political beliefs. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 808–822 (2003).
- 19.
Downs, A. An Economic Theory of Democracy (Harper, 1957).
- 20.
Chen, S., Duckworth, K. & Chaiken, S Motivated heuristic and systematic processing. Psychol. Inq. 10, 44–49 (1999).
- 21.
Giner-Sorolla, R. & Chaiken, S. Selective use of heuristic and systematic processing under defense motivation. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 23, 84–97 (1997).
- 22.
Kahan, D. Fixing the communications failure. Nature 463, 296–297 (2010).
- 23.
Mercier, H. & Sperber, D. Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behav. Brain Sci. 34, 57–74 (2011).
- 24.
Hardin, G. The tragedy of the commons. Science 162, 1243–1248 (1968).
- 25.
Nisbet, M. C. in Communicating Science: New Agendas in Communication41–67 (Routledge, 2010).
- 26.
National Science Board Science and Engineering Indicators, 2010 (National Science Foundation, 2010).
- 27.
Allum, N., Sturgis, P., Tabourazi, D. & Brunton-Smith, I. Science knowledge and attitudes across cultures: A meta-analysis. Public Underst. Sci. 17, 35–54 (2008).
- 28.
Weller, J., Dieckmann, N. F., Tusler, M., Mertz, C. K., Burns, W. & Peters, E. Development and testing of an abbreviated numeracy scale: A rasch analysis approach J. Behav. Decis. Making http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1751 (2012).
- 29.
Frederick, S. Cognitive reflection and decision making. J. Econ. Perspectives 19, 25–42 (2005).
- 30.
Berry, W. D. & Feldman, S. Multiple Regression in Practice 48 (Sage University Papers Series, Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences no. 07-050, Sage Publications, 1985).
Acknowledgements
Research for this paper was financially supported by the National Science Foundation, Grant SES 0922714.
Author information
Affiliations
Yale University, Yale Law School, PO Box 208215, New Haven, Connecticut 06520, USA
- Dan M. Kahan
The Ohio State University, 235 Psychology Building, 1835 Neil Avenue, Columbus, Ohio 43210, USA
- Ellen Peters
Cultural Cognition Project Lab, Yale University, Yale Law School, PO Box 208215, New Haven, Connecticut, USA
- Maggie Wittlin
- & Lisa Larrimore Ouellette
Decision Research, 1201 Oak Street, Suite 200, Eugene, Oregon 97401, USA
- Paul Slovic
George Washington University, 2000 H Street, N.W, Washington DC 20052, USA
- Donald Braman
Temple University, 1719 North Broad St., Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19122, USA
- Gregory Mandel
Authors
Search for Dan M. Kahan in:
Search for Ellen Peters in:
Search for Maggie Wittlin in:
Search for Paul Slovic in:
Search for Lisa Larrimore Ouellette in:
Search for Donald Braman in:
Search for Gregory Mandel in:
Contributions
D.M.K., E.P., M.W. and L.L.O. contributed to all aspects of the paper, including study design, statistical analysis and writing and revisions. P.S., D.B. and G.M. contributed to the design of the study, to substantive analysis of the results and to revisions of the paper.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Corresponding author
Correspondence to Dan M. Kahan.
Supplementary information
PDF files
Rights and permissions
To obtain permission to re-use content from this article visit RightsLink.
About this article
Further reading
-
Extreme opponents of genetically modified foods know the least but think they know the most
Nature Human Behaviour (2019)
-
Bringing climate scientist’s tools into classrooms to improve conceptual understandings
Journal of Environmental Studies and Sciences (2019)
-
Science as a Vaccine
Science & Education (2019)
-
Understanding island residents’ anxiety about impacts caused by climate change using Best–Worst Scaling: a case study of Amami islands, Japan
Sustainability Science (2019)
-
A change in the wind? US public views on renewable energy and climate compared
Energy, Sustainability and Society (2018)