Letter | Published:

Promoting pro-environmental action in climate change deniers

Nature Climate Change volume 2, pages 600603 (2012) | Download Citation

  • An Addendum to this article was published on 27 July 2012

This article has been updated

Abstract

A sizeable (and growing) proportion of the public in Western democracies deny the existence of anthropogenic climate change1,2. It is commonly assumed that convincing deniers that climate change is real is necessary for them to act pro-environmentally3,4. However, the likelihood of ‘conversion’ using scientific evidence is limited because these attitudes increasingly reflect ideological positions5,6. An alternative approach is to identify outcomes of mitigation efforts that deniers find important. People have strong interests in the welfare of their society, so deniers may act in ways supporting mitigation efforts where they believe these efforts will have positive societal effects. In Study 1, climate change deniers (N=155) intended to act more pro-environmentally where they thought climate change action would create a society where people are more considerate and caring, and where there is greater economic/technological development. Study 2 (N=347) replicated this experimentally, showing that framing climate change action as increasing consideration for others, or improving economic/technological development, led to greater pro-environmental action intentions than a frame emphasizing avoiding the risks of climate change. To motivate deniers’ pro-environmental actions, communication should focus on how mitigation efforts can promote a better society, rather than focusing on the reality of climate change and averting its risks.

Access optionsAccess options

Rent or Buy article

Get time limited or full article access on ReadCube.

from$8.99

All prices are NET prices.

Change history

  • 04 July 2012

    This Letter has an addendum associated with it, please see the pdf for full details.

References

  1. 1.

    , , & Global Warming’s Six Americas (Yale Project on Climate Change Communication, 2011).

  2. 2.

    , , , & Australians’ Views of Climate Change (Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation, 2011).

  3. 3.

    & Climate Change Denial: Heads in the Sand (Earthscan, 2011).

  4. 4.

    & The role of social and decision sciencesin communicating uncertain climate risks. Nature Clim. Change 1, 35–41 (2011).

  5. 5.

    The growing climate divide. Nature Clim. Change 1, 195–196 (2011).

  6. 6.

    & The politicization of climate change and polarization in the American public’s views of global warming, 2001–2010. Soc. Quart. 52, 155–194 (2011).

  7. 7.

    Political divisions over climate change and environmental issues in Australia. Environ. Politics 20, 78–96 (2011).

  8. 8.

    & Defeating Kyoto: The Conservative movement’s impact on U.S. climate change policy. Soc. Prob. 50, 348–373 (2003).

  9. 9.

    & Merchants of Doubt: How a Handful of Scientists Obscured the Truth on Issues from Tobacco Smoke to Global Warming (Bloomsbury Press, 2010).

  10. 10.

    , & The organisation of denial: Conservative think tanks and environmental scepticism. Environ. Politics 17, 349–385 (2008).

  11. 11.

    & in Creating a Climate for Change: Communicating Climate Change and Facilitating Social Change (eds Moser, S. C. & Dilling, L.) Ch. 32, 491–516 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2007).

  12. 12.

    Talking past each other? Cultural framing of skeptical and convinced logics in the climate change debate. Org. Environ. 24, 3–33 (2011).

  13. 13.

    , & Cultural cognition of scientific consensus. J. Risk Res. 14, 147–174 (2011).

  14. 14.

    The case for motivated reasoning. Psychol. Bull. 108, 480–498 (1990).

  15. 15.

    Whole-system science, Nature Clim. Change 1, 1 (2011).

  16. 16.

    Meet the humanities. Nature Clim. Change 1, 177–179 (2011).

  17. 17.

    Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. J. Soc. Issues 56, 407–424 (2000).

  18. 18.

    Contributions of psychology to limiting climate change. Am. Psychol. 66, 303–314 (2011).

  19. 19.

    , & Barriers perceived to engaging with climate change among the UK public and their policy implications. Glob. Environ. Change 17, 445–459 (2007).

  20. 20.

    , & Reorienting climate change communication for effective mitigation: Forcing people to be green or fostering grass-roots engagement? Sci. Commun. 30, 305–327 (2009).

  21. 21.

    et al. in IPCC Climate Change 2007: Mitigation (eds Metz, B. et al.) 692–743 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2007).

  22. 22.

    , , & A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 82, 878–902 (2002).

  23. 23.

    , , & Fundamental dimensions of social judgment: Understanding the relations between judgments of competence and warmth. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 89, 899–913 (2005).

  24. 24.

    , & Group virtue: The importance of morality (vs. competence and sociability) in the positive evaluation of in-groups. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 93, 234–249 (2007).

  25. 25.

    et al. in IPCC Climate Change 2007: Impacts, Adaptation and Vulnerability (eds Parry, M. L. et al.) Ch. 11, 507–540 (Cambridge Univ.Press, 2007).

  26. 26.

    & Framing theory. Ann. Rev. Politi. Sci. 10, 103–126 (2007).

  27. 27.

    & Public understanding of climate change in the United States. Am. Psychol. 66, 315–328 (2011).

  28. 28.

    & Anti-reflexivity: The American conservative movement’s success in undermining climate science and policy. Theor. Culture Soc. 27, 100–133 (2010).

  29. 29.

    , , & Perceptions of climate change and willingness to save energy related to flood experience. Nature Clim. Change 1, 46–49 (2011).

  30. 30.

    et al. Folk theory of social change. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 12, 227–246 (2009).

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by an Australian Research Council Discovery Project Grant (DP0984678) to the first author. The authors thank A. Mackintosh, M. Manning, O. Hoegh-Guldberg, J. Lawrence and A. Ryan for their comments on manuscript drafts.

Author information

Affiliations

  1. School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St Lucia Queensland 4072, Australia

    • Paul G. Bain
    • , Matthew J. Hornsey
    • , Renata Bongiorno
    •  & Carla Jeffries

Authors

  1. Search for Paul G. Bain in:

  2. Search for Matthew J. Hornsey in:

  3. Search for Renata Bongiorno in:

  4. Search for Carla Jeffries in:

Contributions

P.G.B. designed the studies, coordinated data collection, analysed the data and wrote the paper. M.J.H. contributed to the design and analysis of both studies and writing the paper. R.B. contributed to the design and analysis of Study 2 and writing the paper. C.J. contributed to the analysis of Study 1 and writing the paper.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul G. Bain.

Supplementary information

About this article

Publication history

Received

Accepted

Published

DOI

https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate1532

Further reading