Abstract
The social intelligence hypothesis states that the demands of social life drive cognitive evolution1,2,3. This idea receives support from comparative studies that link variation in group size or mating systems with cognitive and neuroanatomical differences across species3,4,5,6,7, but findings are contradictory and contentious8,9,10. To understand the cognitive consequences of sociality, it is also important to investigate social variation within species. Here we show that in wild, cooperatively breeding Australian magpies, individuals that live in large groups show increased cognitive performance, which is linked to increased reproductive success. Individual performance was highly correlated across four cognitive tasks, indicating a ‘general intelligence factor’ that underlies cognitive performance. Repeated cognitive testing of juveniles at different ages showed that the correlation between group size and cognition emerged in early life, suggesting that living in larger groups promotes cognitive development. Furthermore, we found a positive association between the task performance of females and three indicators of reproductive success, thus identifying a selective benefit of greater cognitive performance. Together, these results provide intraspecific evidence that sociality can shape cognitive development and evolution.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Relevant articles
Open Access articles citing this article.
-
Wild Australian magpies learn to pull intact, not broken, strings to obtain food
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Open Access 28 April 2023
-
What drives horse success at following human-given cues? An investigation of handler familiarity and living conditions
Animal Cognition Open Access 19 April 2023
-
Sleep loss impairs cognitive performance and alters song output in Australian magpies
Scientific Reports Open Access 22 April 2022
Access options
Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals
Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription
$29.99 / 30 days
cancel any time
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 51 print issues and online access
$199.00 per year
only $3.90 per issue
Rent or buy this article
Get just this article for as long as you need it
$39.95
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout




References
Humphrey, N. in Growing Points in Ethology (eds Bateson, P. P. G. & Hinde, R. A. ) 303–317 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1976)
Byrne, R. W. & Whiten, A. Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans (Clarendon, 1988)
Shultz, S. & Dunbar, R. I. The evolution of the social brain: anthropoid primates contrast with other vertebrates. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 2429–2436 (2007)
Bond, A. B., Kamil, A. C. & Balda, R. P. Social complexity and transitive inference in corvids. Anim. Behav. 65, 479–487 (2003)
Street, S. E., Navarrete, A. F., Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N. Coevolution of cultural intelligence, extended life history, sociality, and brain size in primates. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 7908–7914 (2017)
Forss, S. I. F., Willems, E., Call, J. & van Schaik, C. P. Cognitive differences between orang-utan species: a test of the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Sci. Rep. 6, 30516 (2016)
Fox, K. C. R., Muthukrishna, M. & Shultz, S. The social and cultural roots of whale and dolphin brains. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1, 1699–1705 (2017)
Holekamp, K. E. Questioning the social intelligence hypothesis. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 65–69 (2007)
DeCasien, A. R., Williams, S. A. & Higham, J. P. Primate brain size is predicted by diet but not sociality. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1, 0112 (2017)
Sayol, F. et al. Environmental variation and the evolution of large brains in birds. Nat. Commun. 7, 13971 (2016)
Morand-Ferron, J., Cole, E. F. & Quinn, J. L. Studying the evolutionary ecology of cognition in the wild: a review of practical and conceptual challenges. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 91, 367–389 (2016)
Thornton, A. & Lukas, D. Individual variation in cognitive performance: developmental and evolutionary perspectives. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367, 2773–2783 (2012)
Sallet, J. et al. Social network size affects neural circuits in macaques. Science 334, 697–700 (2011)
Kanai, R ., Bahrami, B ., Roylance, R . & Rees, G. Online social network size is reflected in human brain structure. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 1327–1334 (2012)
Fischer, S., Bessert-Nettelbeck, M., Kotrschal, A. & Taborsky, B. Rearing-group size determines social competence and brain structure in a cooperatively breeding cichlid. Am. Nat. 186, 123–140 (2015)
MacLean, E. L. et al. The evolution of self-control. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, E2140–E2148 (2014)
Plomin, R. The genetics of G in human and mouse. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 136–141 (2001)
Chandra, S. B., Hosler, J. S. & Smith, B. H. Heritable variation for latent inhibition and its correlation with reversal learning in honeybees (Apis mellifera). J. Comp. Psychol. 114, 86–97 (2000)
Isden, J., Panayi, C., Dingle, C. & Madden, J. Performance in cognitive and problem-solving tasks in male spotted bowerbirds does not correlate with mating success. Anim. Behav. 86, 829–838 (2013)
Shaw, R. C., Boogert, N. J., Clayton, N. S. & Burns, K. C. Wild psychometrics: evidence for ‘general’ cognitive performance in wild New Zealand robins, Petroica longipes. Anim. Behav. 109, 101–111 (2015)
Hopkins, W. D., Russell, J. L. & Schaeffer, J. Chimpanzee intelligence is heritable. Curr. Biol. 24, 1649–1652 (2014)
Galsworthy, M. J. et al. Assessing reliability, heritability and general cognitive ability in a battery of cognitive tasks for laboratory mice. Behav. Genet. 35, 675–692 (2005)
Buchanan, K. L., Grindstaff, J. L. & Pravosudov, V. V. Condition dependence, developmental plasticity, and cognition: implications for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 28, 290–296 (2013)
Plomin, R. & Deary, I. J. Genetics and intelligence differences: five special findings. Mol. Psychiatry 20, 98–108 (2015)
Boogert, N. J., Anderson, R. C., Peters, S., Searcy, W. A. & Nowicki, S. Song repertoire size in male song sparrows correlates with detour reaching, but not with other cognitive measures. Anim. Behav. 81, 1209–1216 (2011)
Hughes, J. M. et al. High levels of extra-group paternity in a population of Australian magpies Gymnorhina tibicen: evidence from microsatellite analysis. Mol. Ecol. 12, 3441–3450 (2003)
Kotrschal, A. et al. Artificial selection on relative brain size in the guppy reveals costs and benefits of evolving a larger brain. Curr. Biol. 23, 168–171 (2013)
Arnold, K. E ., Ramsay, S. L ., Donaldson, C. & Adam, A. Parental prey selection affects risk-taking behaviour and spatial learning in avian offspring. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 2563–2569 (2007)
Silk, J. B., Alberts, S. C. & Altmann, J. Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science 302, 1231–1234 (2003)
Croston, R., Branch, C. L., Kozlovsky, D. Y., Dukas, R. & Pravosudov, V. V. Heritability and the evolution of cognitive traits. Behav. Ecol. 26, 1447–1459 (2015)
Pike, K. N. How much do Helpers help? Variation in Helper Behaviour in the Cooperatively Breeding Western Australian Magpie. MSc Thesis, Univ. Western Australia (2016)
Kaplan, G. Australian Magpie: Biology and Behaviour of an Unusual Songbird (CSIRO Publishing, 2004)
Edwards, E. K., Mitchell, N. J. & Ridley, A. R. The impact of high temperatures on foraging behaviour and body condition in the Western Australian Magpie Cracticus tibicen dorsalis. Ostrich 86, 137–144 (2015)
Morand-Ferron, J. Why learn? The adaptive value of associative learning in wild populations. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 16, 73–79 (2017)
Sherry, D. F. in Cognitive Ecology: the Evolutionary Ecology of Information Processing and Decision Making (ed. Dukas, R. ) 261–296 (Univ. Chicago Press, 1998)
Lotem, A. & Halpern, J. Y. Coevolution of learning and data-acquisition mechanisms: a model for cognitive evolution. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367, 2686–2694 (2012)
Leadbeater, E. What evolves in the evolution of social learning? J. Zool. 295, 4–11 (2015)
Amici, F., Aureli, F. & Call, J. Fission–fusion dynamics, behavioral flexibility, and inhibitory control in primates. Curr. Biol. 18, 1415–1419 (2008)
Boogert, N. J., Giraldeau, L.-A. & Lefebvre, L. Song complexity correlates with learning ability in zebra finch males. Anim. Behav. 76, 1735–1741 (2008)
Rowe, C. & Healy, S. D. Measuring variation in cognition. Behav. Ecol. 25, 1287–1292 (2014)
Griffin, A. S. & Guez, D. Innovative problem solving in nonhuman animals: the effects of group size revisited. Behav. Ecol. 26, 722–734 (2015)
Nowicki, S., Searcy, W. & Peters, S. Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a review and experimental test of the “nutritional stress hypothesis”. J. Comp. Physiol. A 188, 1003–1014 (2002)
Burnham, K. P . & Anderson, D. R. Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach (Springer, 2002)
Symonds, M. R. E. & Moussalli, A. A brief guide to model selection, multimodel inference and model averaging in behavioural ecology using Akaike’s information criterion. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65, 13–21 (2011)
Kembel, S. W. et al. Picante: R tools for integrating phylogenies and ecology. Bioinformatics 26, 1463–1464 (2010)
Nakagawa, S. & Schielzeth, H. Repeatability for Gaussian and non-Gaussian data: a practical guide for biologists. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 85, 935–956 (2010)
Acknowledgements
We thank E. Russell and the late I. Rowley for access to their life-history records, and for allowing us to continue work on their Guildford magpie population; A. Wolton for help with fieldwork; R. Lymbery for help with statistical analyses; and N. Boogert and A. Young for helpful comments and discussion. This work was funded by an ARC Discovery grant awarded to A.R.R., A.T. and M. B. V. Bell, and a University of Western Australia International Postgraduate Research Scholarship and Endeavour Research Fellowship awarded to B.J.A. A.T. received additional support from a BBSRC David Phillips Fellowship (BB/H021817/1).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
B.J.A., A.R.R. and A.T. conceived and designed the study. B.J.A. wrote the manuscript. B.J.A. and E.K.E. carried out data collection. All authors discussed results and commented on the manuscript.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Additional information
Reviewer Information Nature thanks T. Bugnyar and the other anonymous reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Extended data figures and tables
Extended Data Figure 1 Adult cognitive test set.
a–c, The cognitive test series used to quantify individual variation in inhibitory control (a), associative and reversal learning (b) and spatial memory (c).
Extended Data Figure 2 Developmental trajectory of cognitive performance.
a, b, The developmental trajectory of Australian magpies at 100, 200 and 300 days after fledging for two cognitive traits: behavioural inhibition (a; n = 48 trials) and spatial memory (b; n = 46 trials). c, Developmental trajectory for behavioural inhibition and spatial memory combined (n = 94 trials). Green dots, individuals from small groups (containing 1–7 individuals); blue dots, individuals from large groups (≥8 individuals). Scores are measured as either the number of trials taken to succeed at the task or the number of locations searched, so lower scores indicate better performance.
Extended Data Figure 3 Frequency distribution of general cognitive performance in relation to group size.
a, b, Frequency distribution of general cognitive performance among individuals in small groups (a; containing <8 individuals, n = 29 individuals) and large groups (b, >8 individuals, n = 17 individuals).
Extended Data Figure 4 Juvenile cognitive test batteries.
a–i, Cognitive test batteries presented to individuals at 100 (a–c), 200 (d–f) and 300 (g–i) days after fledging, containing four tasks designed to quantify inhibitory control (a, d, g), associative and reversal learning (b, e, h) and spatial memory (c, f, i). b is shown in black and white, because individuals were unable to complete the associative and reversal learning tasks at 100 days after fledging. Red circles indicate that individuals had to search a different location at each age tested in order to obtain the food reward in the spatial memory task.
Extended Data Figure 5 Example of the lids used in the cognitive tasks.
The lids used in the associative learning, reversal learning and spatial memory tasks. The lids were held firmly in place by elastic bands, and swivelled when pecked, allowing individuals to search wells for their contents.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information
This file contains Supplementary Results and Discussion, Supplementary References and Supplementary Tables 1-26. (PDF 1175 kb)
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Ashton, B., Ridley, A., Edwards, E. et al. Cognitive performance is linked to group size and affects fitness in Australian magpies. Nature 554, 364–367 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1038/nature25503
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nature25503
This article is cited by
-
Spatial memory predicts home range size and predation risk in pheasants
Nature Ecology & Evolution (2023)
-
Wild Australian magpies learn to pull intact, not broken, strings to obtain food
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (2023)
-
Repeated testing does not confound cognitive performance in the Western Australian magpie (Cracticus tibicen dorsalis)
Animal Cognition (2023)
-
What drives horse success at following human-given cues? An investigation of handler familiarity and living conditions
Animal Cognition (2023)
-
Sleep loss impairs cognitive performance and alters song output in Australian magpies
Scientific Reports (2022)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.