Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Anthropogenic electromagnetic noise disrupts magnetic compass orientation in a migratory bird

Abstract

Electromagnetic noise is emitted everywhere humans use electronic devices. For decades, it has been hotly debated whether man-made electric and magnetic fields affect biological processes, including human health1,2,3,4,5. So far, no putative effect of anthropogenic electromagnetic noise at intensities below the guidelines adopted by the World Health Organization1,2 has withstood the test of independent replication under truly blinded experimental conditions. No effect has therefore been widely accepted as scientifically proven1,2,3,4,5,6. Here we show that migratory birds are unable to use their magnetic compass in the presence of urban electromagnetic noise. When European robins, Erithacus rubecula, were exposed to the background electromagnetic noise present in unscreened wooden huts at the University of Oldenburg campus, they could not orient using their magnetic compass. Their magnetic orientation capabilities reappeared in electrically grounded, aluminium-screened huts, which attenuated electromagnetic noise in the frequency range from 50 kHz to 5 MHz by approximately two orders of magnitude. When the grounding was removed or when broadband electromagnetic noise was deliberately generated inside the screened and grounded huts, the birds again lost their magnetic orientation capabilities. The disruptive effect of radiofrequency electromagnetic fields is not confined to a narrow frequency band and birds tested far from sources of electromagnetic noise required no screening to orient with their magnetic compass. These fully double-blinded tests document a reproducible effect of anthropogenic electromagnetic noise on the behaviour of an intact vertebrate.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Magnetic compass orientation of migratory European robins tested at the University of Oldenburg requires aluminium screening.
Figure 2: Connecting and disconnecting the grounding of the screens turns on and off the birds’ magnetic compass orientation capabilities.
Figure 3: Artificially produced broadband electromagnetic noise disrupts the magnetic compass orientation of birds tested inside the grounded aluminium-screened huts.
Figure 4: The disruptive effect of broadband electromagnetic noise on magnetic compass orientation is not limited to a single narrow frequency range.
Figure 5: In a rural location, European robins show magnetic compass orientation without screening.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. International Commission for Non-Ionizing Radiation Protection. ICNIRP guidelines for limiting exposure to time-varying electric, magnetic, and electromagnetic fields (up to 300 GHz). Health Phys. 74, 494–522 (1998)

  2. International Commission for Non-Ionizing Radiation Protection. ICNIRP statement on the “Guidelines for limiting exposure to time-varying electric, magnetic, and electromagnetic fields (up to 300 GHz)”. Health Phys. 97, 257–258 (2009)

  3. World Health Organization. Extremely Low Frequency Fields. Environmental Health Criteria Monograph no. 238,. http://www.who.int/peh-emf/publications/elf_ehc/en/ (2007)

  4. Health Protection Agency. Health Effects from Radiofrequency Electromagnetic Fields. http://www.hpa.org.uk/webc/HPAwebFile/HPAweb_C/1317133827077 (2012)

  5. The INTERPHONE Study Group Brain tumour risk in relation to mobile telephone use: results of the INTERPHONE international case–control study. Int. J. Epidemiol. 39, 675–694 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Johansen, C. et al. Cellular telephones and cancer—a nationwide cohort study in Denmark. J. Natl. Cancer Inst. 93, 203–207 (2001)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Merkel, F. W. & Wiltschko, W. Magnetismus und Richtungsfinden zugunruhiger Rotkehlchen. Vogelwarte 23, 71–77 (1965)

    Google Scholar 

  8. Wiltschko, W. & Wiltschko, R. Magnetic compass of European robins. Science 176, 62–64 (1972)

    Article  CAS  ADS  Google Scholar 

  9. Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. Magnetic Orientation in Animals (Springer, 1995)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  10. Ritz, T., Thalau, P., Phillips, J. B., Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. Resonance effects indicate a radical-pair mechanism for avian magnetic compass. Nature 429, 177–180 (2004)

    Article  CAS  ADS  Google Scholar 

  11. Ritz, T. et al. Magnetic compass of birds is based on a molecule with optimal directional sensitivity. Biophys. J. 96, 3451–3457 (2009)

    Article  CAS  ADS  Google Scholar 

  12. Zapka, M. et al. Visual but not trigeminal mediation of magnetic compass information in a migratory bird. Nature 461, 1274–1277 (2009)

    Article  CAS  ADS  Google Scholar 

  13. Hein, C. M. et al. Night-migratory garden warblers can orient with their magnetic compass using the left, the right or both eyes. J. R. Soc. Interface 7, 227–233 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Hein, C. M., Engels, S., Kishkinev, D. & Mouritsen, H. Robins have a magnetic compass in both eyes. Nature 471, E11 (2011)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Engels, S., Hein, C. M., Lefeldt, N., Prior, H. & Mouritsen, H. Night-migratory songbirds possess a magnetic compass in both eyes. PLoS ONE 7, e43271 (2012)

    Article  CAS  ADS  Google Scholar 

  16. Swanson, J. & Kheifets, L. Biophysical mechanisms: a component in the weight of evidence for health effects of power-frequency electric and magnetic fields. Radiat. Res. 165, 470–478 (2006)

    Article  CAS  ADS  Google Scholar 

  17. Cochran, W. W., Mouritsen, H. & Wikelski, M. Migrating songbirds recalibrate their magnetic compass daily from twilight cues. Science 304, 405–408 (2004)

    Article  CAS  ADS  Google Scholar 

  18. Chernetsov, N., Kishkinev, D., Kosarev, V. & Bolshakov, C. V. Not all songbirds calibrate their magnetic compass from twilight cues: a telemetry study. J. Exp. Biol. 214, 2540–2543 (2011)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Bruderer, B. The study of bird migration by radar part 2: major achievements. Naturwiss 84, 45–54 (1997)

    Article  CAS  ADS  Google Scholar 

  20. Sanderson, F. J., Donald, P. F., Pain, D. J., Burfield, I. J. & Van Bommel, F. P. Long-term population declines in afro-palearctic migrant birds. Biol. Conserv. 131, 93–105 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Schulten, K., Swenberg, C. E. & Weller, A. A biomagnetic sensory mechanism based on magnetic field modulated coherent electron spin motion. Z. Phys. Chem. 111, 1–5 (1978)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Ritz, T., Adem, S. & Schulten, K. A model for photoreceptor-based magnetoreception in birds. Biophys. J. 78, 707–718 (2000)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Maeda, K. et al. Chemical compass model of avian magnetoreception. Nature 453, 387–390 (2008)

    Article  CAS  ADS  Google Scholar 

  24. Rodgers, C. T. & Hore, P. J. Chemical magnetoreception in birds: the radical pair mechanism. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 353–360 (2009)

    Article  CAS  ADS  Google Scholar 

  25. Liedvogel, M. & Mouritsen, H. Cryptochromes—a potential magnetoreceptor: what do we know and what do we want to know? J. R. Soc. Interface 7, 147–162 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Mouritsen, H. & Hore, P. J. The magnetic retina: light-dependent and trigeminal magnetoreception in migratory birds. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 22, 343–352 (2012)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Solov'yov, I. A., Domratcheva, T. & Schulten, K. Separation of photo-induced radical pair in cryptochrome to a functionally critical distance. Sci. Rep. 4, 3845 (2014)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  28. Kirschvink, J. L., Winklhofer, M. & Walker, M. M. Biophysics of magnetic orientation: strengthening the interface between theory and experimental design. J. R. Soc. Interface 7 Suppl. 2,. S179–S191 (2010)

  29. Wu, L. Q. & Dickman, J. D. Neural correlates of a magnetic sense. Science 336, 1054–1057 (2012)

    Article  CAS  ADS  Google Scholar 

  30. Gauger, E. M., Rieper, E., Morton, J. J. L., Benjamin, S. C. & Vedral, V. Sustained quantum coherence and entanglement in the avian compass. Phys. Rev. Lett. 106, 040503 (2011)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  31. Kirschvink, J. L. Uniform magnetic fields and double-wrapped coil systems: improved techniques for the design of bioelectromagnetic experiments. Bioelectromagnetics 13, 401–411 (1992)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Emlen, S. T. . & Emlen, J. T. A technique for recording migratory orientation of captive birds. Auk 83, 361–367 (1966)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Mouritsen, H., Feenders, G., Hegemann, A. & Liedvogel, M. Thermal paper can replace typewriter correction paper in Emlen funnels. J. Ornithol. 150, 713–715 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Muheim, R., Phillips, J. B. & Åkesson, S. Polarized light cues underlie compass calibration in migratory songbirds. Science 313, 837–839 (2006)

    Article  CAS  ADS  Google Scholar 

  35. Hein, C. M., Zapka, M. & Mouritsen, H. Weather significantly influences the migratory behaviour of night-migratory songbirds tested indoors in orientation cages. J. Ornithol. 152, 27–35 (2011)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Batschelet, E. Circular Statistics in Biology (Academic, 1981)

    MATH  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank M. Wuschek, Rohde & Schwarz, Bundesnetzagentur, and ETS Lindgren for help with measuring the electromagnetic fields, the workshops at the University of Oldenburg, especially T. Geiger, for building equipment, etc, and a large number of Bachelors, Masters and PhD students for help in conducting the experiments. We are grateful to the following for financial support: Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency (QuBE: N66001-10-1-4061 to P.J.H. and H.M.), VW-Stiftung (Lichtenberg professorship to H.M.), DFG (FOR 701 and MO 1408/2-2 to H.M.), Heinz Neumüller Stiftung (to C.M.H. and S.E.), BMBF (to H.M.), the European Research Council (to P.J.H.) and the EMF Biological Research Trust (to P.J.H.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

S.E. and N.-L.S. contributed equally to this work and are listed alphabetically. H.M. and N.-L.S. designed the study. S.E., N.L., C.M.H., M.Z., A.M. and D.E. performed the experiments. S.E., N.L., C.M.H., M.Z. and H.M. analysed the data. A.K., P.J.H. and N.-L.S. provided physical insight needed to properly produce and measure the electromagnetic fields. N.-L.S. and S.E. were in charge of generating the electromagnetic noise. N.-L.S. measured the electromagnetic fields. H.M., P.J.H., N.-L.S. and S.E. wrote most of the paper. All authors read and commented on the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Henrik Mouritsen.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Extended data figures and tables

Extended Data Figure 1 Wooden huts and experimental locations.

a, Photograph of one of the four identical wooden huts used for our experiments. b, Photograph from the inside of an experimental hut showing the aluminium screening, parts of the Merritt coil system, and the table on which the funnels were placed. The insert shows the self-cutting screws used to connect the aluminium plates. c, Simple map of the city of Oldenburg. Built-up areas are shown in grey and nature-protected areas in green. Black lines denote highways, blue denotes water. Red stars: ‘1’ indicates the location of the University campus and ‘2’ the rural location used for some of the tests. d, Map of the University of Oldenburg Wechloy Campus. 1, main University building housing the biology, chemistry, physics and mathematics institutes; 2, botanical greenhouse; 3, iron-free wooden building; 4, the locations of the four wooden huts used for our experiments; 5, ‘Next Energy’ building.

Extended Data Figure 2 Electromagnetic noise measurements in the range from 40 Hz to 32 kHz.

a, Magnetic field intensity (B). b, Electric field intensity (E). The colour coding of the traces corresponds to Fig. 4. Notice that the frequency-axis (f) is logarithmic.

Extended Data Table 1 The accumulated time-dependent magnetic field intensity summed over all the frequencies in the spectra recorded for each behavioural test condition

PowerPoint slides

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Engels, S., Schneider, NL., Lefeldt, N. et al. Anthropogenic electromagnetic noise disrupts magnetic compass orientation in a migratory bird. Nature 509, 353–356 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13290

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13290

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing