Main

In oscillatory interference models, path integration is performed by velocity-dependent variation in the frequencies of theta-band oscillations, which combine to generate the grid-cell pattern2,3,4,6,7. In addition, learned associations to environmental sensory inputs (possibly mediated by place cells) ensure that grids are spatially stable over time and are sufficient to maintain firing in familiar environments2,3,8. In rats, the majority of grid cells show theta-modulated firing9,10, and the model predicts specific relationships between modulation frequency, running velocity and grid scale4, which have been verified in grid cells11 and in putative velocity-controlled oscillatory inputs identified as interneurons within the septohippocampal circuit7.

Yartsev et al.1 recorded the firing of grid cells from bats trained to crawl within the recording environment, a behaviour that they perform very slowly (a mean speed of 3.7 cm s−1 versus 17.6 cm s−1 in our rat data), often stopping entirely (supplementary figure 11 in ref. 1). The authors found grid cells with very low firing rates (a mean peak rate of 0.56 Hz versus 5.14 Hz in our data) and little significant theta modulation. However, matching movement speed is important for comparisons involving theta. At low speeds movement-related theta rhythmicity is strongly attenuated12 and the need for path integration is reduced. Equally importantly, low firing rates impede detection of theta rhythmicity (5–10 Hz), which requires periods containing plenty of spikes fired within tens to hundreds of milliseconds of each other (something that is absent in bat interspike interval histograms; supplementary figure 2b in ref. 1).

We examined whether differences in movement speeds and firing rates between the rat data and the bat data could explain the apparent lack of theta rhythmicity in bat grid cells. We took random samples of 25 cells from a representative data set of 85 grid cells recorded in rat MEC (Fig. 1a, bottom row), extracted periods of slow running to match bat movement speeds, and duplicated this data until it exceeded the duration of the longest bat trial (60 min). We then randomly discarded spikes to match the mean firing rates of each of the 25 published bat grid cells. From the 25 down-sampled rat cells matching each bat grid cell, we selected the one with the median theta index as representative. This process was repeated 10 times. Subjecting the 10 sets of 25 down-sampled cells to the analyses of Yartsev et al. produced a relative absence of theta rhythmicity (Fig. 1b, fourth row). So, if rats moved as slowly as bats and their grid cells fired as infrequently, rat grid cells would show bat levels of gridness (below the higher levels seen in rats), and theta modulation would be very hard to detect.

Figure 1: Down-sampled rat grid cells and oscillatory interference reproduce bat grid-cell firing.
figure 1

a, b, The firing of grid cells in rats (a) resembles grid-cell firing in bats1 if the rat data are down-sampled to match the low firing rates and slow movements of the bat data (b). c, d, The oscillatory interference model simulates theta-modulated grid cell firing in rats (c), and also apparently un-modulated grid-cell firing in bats when firing rates are reduced (d). ad, Top row, example firing-rate maps (peak rate and gridness, above). Second row, example spike-train autocorrelograms. Third row, distributions of gridness scores. Fourth row, distributions of theta modulation (theta index). Grid cells have gridness > 0.33 (red line). ‘Theta-modulated cells’ have a theta index of ≥ 5 (red line). The theta index exceeded the 95th percentile for that cell’s temporally shuffled spike times for 58% of rat cells (a) but only for 2% of cells down-sampled to match the bat data (b; averaged over 10 samples of 25 cells). This rises to 14% if speed is not down-sampled, 20% if only the 25 most strongly theta-modulated rat cells are used and 72% for the 25 most strongly theta-modulated cells, if speed is unmatched. However, we do not consider this last cell population to be comparable to the bat grid cells because of the pre-selection of only the most strongly theta-modulated cells and the difference in movement speed between running rats and crawling bats. Theta index, gridness and shuffling follow ref. 1 (in which theta index is theta power divided by mean power 0–50 Hz), except for a, bottom row, which shows theta index calculated following ref. 13 (that is, theta power divided by mean power 0–125 Hz), giving higher values that match the proportions of theta-modulated cells in ref. 13 (which range from 62% in layer V, where most bat cells were recorded, to 90% in layer III). e, Schematic showing how theta-modulated inhibitory spike trains (top, black ticks) drive the grid cell’s membrane potential (middle, black trace), producing spikes when exceeding a threshold (middle, red dashed line). Spatial firing fields (bottom) are defined by constructive interference (top, grey lines show theta modulation; middle, grey line shows the resulting interference pattern), but the underlying oscillations are undetectable at low firing rates (see Methods for details).

PowerPoint slide

Most importantly, to disprove the model requires knowing how much theta rhythmicity it predicts in low-firing-rate cells. Simulations (using code adapted from ref. 7) with strong theta modulation and typical firing rates for rats (Fig. 1c) also lack significant theta modulation when firing rates are reduced to bat levels (Fig. 1d, fourth row). Although spatially modulated firing is driven by interference between theta-modulated inputs, the theta rhythmicity is undetectable in low-rate spike trains (Fig. 1e).

Local field potentials and multi-unit activity were also reported in bats1, but these reflect the physical arrangement and coherence of populations of cells, which may vary between species and are not addressed by the model (although spatially offset grids require phase-offset oscillators7, suggesting no overall phase preference in the model). Finally, consistent with the model, grids might be set up through oscillatory interference during the initial training of the bats to not fly out of the box (by physically blocking from above), and maintained (at lower firing rates) by learned sensory associations during subsequent slow crawling in the now highly familiar box.

Methods

The activity of 85 grid cells was recorded from superficial and deep layers of rat MEC during 20 min foraging in 1-m2 arenas using standard procedures8. Random samples of 25 cells were speed matched by removing periods of fast running, retaining periods of ≥0.5 s, until the median speed was 3.7 cm s−1. Speed-matched data were duplicated and concatenated to exceed the duration of the longest bat trial (60 min). Cell firing rates were down-sampled by randomly removing spikes, in turn, to match the mean firing rate of each of the 25 bat grid cells (mean rate taken as 25% of the peak rates found by Yartsev et al. (range of mean rates, 0.03–0.40 Hz)). Spike-train autocorrelograms combined the individual autocorrelograms from each period of slow running11 and were mean-normalized to avoid low-frequency power reducing the theta index (compare with figure 4g in ref. 1). Grid cells were simulated as leaky integrate-and-fire neurons (time constant 20 ms) receiving three oscillatory inhibitory spike trains7 (Poisson processes with rate 50 + 30cos(2πft), in which frequency (f) varies around 8 Hz according to running velocity, with a peak inhibitory synaptic conductance14 of 14 pS) and a noisy persistent excitatory current sampled from N(m,2m), in which m = 336 nA for low firing rates and m = 436 nA for high rates (mean peak rates are 0.48 Hz and 5.11 Hz, respectively).