Abstract
Cues that reliably predict rewards trigger the thoughts and emotions normally evoked by those rewards. Humans and other animals will work, often quite hard, for these cues. This is termed conditioned reinforcement. The ability to use conditioned reinforcers to guide our behaviour is normally beneficial; however, it can go awry. For example, corporate icons, such as McDonald’s Golden Arches, influence consumer behaviour in powerful and sometimes surprising ways1, and drug-associated cues trigger relapse to drug seeking in addicts and animals exposed to addictive drugs, even after abstinence or extinction2,3. Yet, despite their prevalence, it is not known how conditioned reinforcers control human or other animal behaviour. One possibility is that they act through the use of the specific rewards they predict; alternatively, they could control behaviour directly by activating emotions that are independent of any specific reward. In other words, the Golden Arches may drive business because they evoke thoughts of hamburgers and fries, or instead, may be effective because they also evoke feelings of hunger or happiness. Moreover, different brain circuits could support conditioned reinforcement mediated by thoughts of specific outcomes versus more general affective information. Here we have attempted to address these questions in rats. Rats were trained to learn that different cues predicted different rewards using specialized conditioning procedures that controlled whether the cues evoked thoughts of specific outcomes or general affective representations common to different outcomes. Subsequently, these rats were given the opportunity to press levers to obtain short and otherwise unrewarded presentations of these cues. We found that rats were willing to work for cues that evoked either outcome-specific or general affective representations. Furthermore the orbitofrontal cortex, a prefrontal region important for adaptive decision-making4, was critical for the former but not for the latter form of conditioned reinforcement.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 51 print issues and online access
$199.00 per year
only $3.90 per issue
Rent or buy this article
Prices vary by article type
from$1.95
to$39.95
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout



Similar content being viewed by others
References
Robinson, T. N., Borzekowski, D. L., Matheson, D. M. & Kraemer, H. C. Effects of fast food branding on young children’s taste preferences. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 161, 792–797 (2007)
Shaham, Y., Shalev, U., Lu, L., de Wit, H. & Stewart, J. The reinstatement model of drug relapse: history, methodology and major findings. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 168, 3–20 (2003)
O’Brien, C. P. Anticraving medications for relapse prevention: a possible new class of psychoactive medications. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, 1423–1431 (2005)
Murray, E. A., O’Doherty, J. P. & Schoenbaum, G. What we know and do not know about the functions of the orbitofrontal cortex after 20 years of cross-species studies. J. Neurosci. 27, 8166–8169 (2007)
Rescorla, R. A. Learning about qualitatively different outcomes during a blocking procedure. Anim. Learn. Behav. 27, 140–151 (1999)
Ganesan, R. & Pearce, J. M. Effect of changing the unconditioned stimulus on appetitive blocking. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 14, 280–291 (1988)
Weissenborn, R., Robbins, T. W. & Everitt, B. J. Effects of medial prefrontal or anterior cingulate cortex lesions on responding for cocaine under fixed-ratio and second-order schedules of reinforcement in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 134, 242–257 (1997)
Pears, A., Parkinson, J. A., Hopewell, L., Everitt, B. J. & Roberts, A. C. Lesions of the orbitofrontal but not medial prefrontal cortex disrupt conditioned reinforcement in primates. J. Neurosci. 23, 11189–11201 (2003)
Parkinson, J. A., Roberts, A. C., Everitt, B. J. & Di Ciano, P. Acquisition of instrumental conditioned reinforcement is resistant to the devaluation of the unconditioned stimulus. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 58, 19–30 (2005)
Padoa-Schioppa, C. & Assad, J. A. Neurons in orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value. Nature 441, 223–226 (2006)
Roesch, M. R., Taylor, A. R. & Schoenbaum, G. Encoding of time-discounted rewards in orbitofrontal cortex is independent of value representation. Neuron 51, 509–520 (2006)
Feierstein, C. E., Quirk, M. C., Uchida, N., Sosulski, D. L. & Mainen, Z. F. Representation of spatial goals in rat orbitofrontal cortex. Neuron 51, 495–507 (2006)
O’Doherty, J. P., Deichmann, R., Critchley, H. D. & Dolan, R. J. Neural responses during anticipation of a primary taste reward. Neuron 33, 815–826 (2002)
Gottfried, J. A., O’Doherty, J. & Dolan, R. J. Encoding predictive reward value in human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Science 301, 1104–1107 (2003)
Tremblay, L. & Schultz, W. Relative reward preference in primate orbitofrontal cortex. Nature 398, 704–708 (1999)
Roesch, M. R. & Olson, C. R. Neuronal activity related to reward value and motivation in primate frontal cortex. Science 304, 307–310 (2004)
Gallagher, M., McMahan, R. W. & Schoenbaum, G. Orbitofrontal cortex and representation of incentive value in associative learning. J. Neurosci. 19, 6610–6614 (1999)
Izquierdo, A., Suda, R. K. & Murray, E. A. Bilateral orbital prefrontal cortex lesions in rhesus monkeys disrupt choices guided by both reward value and reward contingency. J. Neurosci. 24, 7540–7548 (2004)
Pickens, C. L. et al. Different roles for orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in a reinforcer devaluation task. J. Neurosci. 23, 11078–11084 (2003)
Setlow, B., Holland, P. C. & Gallagher, M. Disconnection of the basolateral amygdala complex and nucleus accumbens impairs appetitive Pavlovian second-order conditioned responses. Behav. Neurosci. 116, 267–275 (2002)
Taylor, J. R. & Robbins, T. W. Enhanced behavioral control by conditioned reinforcers following microinjections of d-amphetamine into the nucleus accumbens. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 84, 405–412 (1984)
Cousens, G. A. & Otto, T. Neural substrates of olfactory discrimination learning with auditory secondary reinforcement. I. Contributions of the basolateral amygdaloid complex and orbitofrontal cortex. Integr. Physiol. Behav. Sci. 38, 272–294 (2003)
Parkinson, J. A. et al. The role of the primate amygdala in conditioned reinforcement. J. Neurosci. 21, 7770–7780 (2001)
Setlow, B., Gallagher, M. & Holland, P. C. The basolateral complex of the amygdala is necessary for acquisition but not expression of CS motivational value in appetitive Pavlovian second-order conditioning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 15, 1841–1853 (2002)
Robledo, P., Robbins, T. W. & Everitt, B. J. Effects of excitotoxic lesions of the central amygdaloid nucleus on the potentiation of reward-related stimuli by intra-accumbens amphetamine. Behav. Neurosci. 110, 981–990 (1996)
Parkinson, J. A., Olmstead, M. C., Burns, L. H., Robbins, T. W. & Everitt, B. J. Dissociation of effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on appetitive Pavlovian approach behavior and the potentiation of conditioned reinforcement and locomotor activity by d-amphetamine. J. Neurosci. 19, 2401–2411 (1999)
Cador, M., Robbins, T. W. & Everitt, B. J. Involvement of the amygdala in stimulus–reward associations: interactions with the ventral striatum. Neuroscience 30, 77–86 (1989)
Hall, J., Parkinson, J. A., Connor, T. M., Dickinson, A. & Everitt, B. J. Involvement of the central nucleus of the amygdala and nucleus accumbens core in mediating Pavlovian influences on instrumental behavior. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 1984–1992 (2001)
Balleine, B. W. & Corbit, L. H. Double dissociation of nucleus accumbens core and shell on the general and outcome-specific forms of pavlovian-instrumental transfer. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 71 16. (2005)
Corbit, L. H. & Balleine, B. W. Double dissociation of basolateral and central amygdala lesions on the general and outcome-specific forms of pavlovian-instrumental transfer. J. Neurosci 25, 962–970 (2005)
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to A. Delamater and P. Holland for their advice on this work. This work was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA).
Author Contributions K.A.B. and G.S. conceived the experiments; K.A.B., D.N.M. and T.M.F. carried out the experiments; K.A.B. and G.S. analysed the data and co-wrote the manuscript with assistance from each of the other authors.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information
The file contains Supplementary Results and Supplementary Figures S1-S5 with Legends. (PDF 687 kb)
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Burke, K., Franz, T., Miller, D. et al. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in the pursuit of happiness and more specific rewards. Nature 454, 340–344 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06993
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06993
This article is cited by
-
Medial orbitofrontal cortical regulation of different aspects of Pavlovian and instrumental reward seeking
Psychopharmacology (2023)
-
Taking stock of value in the orbitofrontal cortex
Nature Reviews Neuroscience (2022)
-
The self in context: brain systems linking mental and physical health
Nature Reviews Neuroscience (2021)
-
Dopamine transients do not act as model-free prediction errors during associative learning
Nature Communications (2020)
-
Orbitofrontal control of visual cortex gain promotes visual associative learning
Nature Communications (2020)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.