Co-mimics have a mutualistic relationship despite unequal defences

Abstract

In the first clear mathematical treatment of natural selection, Müller1 proposed that a shared warning signal (mimicry) would benefit defended prey species by sharing out the per capita mortality incurred during predator education. Although mimicry is a mainstay of adaptationist thinking, there has been repeated debate on whether there is a mutualistic or a parasitic relationship between unequally defended co-mimic species2,3,4,5. Here we show that the relationship between unequally defended species is mutualistic. We examined this in a ‘novel world’6 of artificial prey with wild predators (great tit, Parus major). We kept the abundance of a highly defended prey (‘model’) constant and increased the density of a moderately defended prey (‘defended mimic’) of either perfect or imperfect mimetic resemblance to the model. Both model and defended mimic showed a net benefit from a density-dependent decrease in their per capita mortality. Even when the effect of dilution through density was controlled for, defended mimics did not induce additional attacks on the model, but we found selection for accurate signal mimicry. In comparison, the addition of fully edible (batesian) mimics did increase additional attacks on the model, but as a result of dilution this resulted in no overall increase in per capita mortality. By ignoring the effects of density, current theories may have overestimated the parasitic costs imposed by less defended mimics on highly defended models.

Access options

Rent or Buy article

Get time limited or full article access on ReadCube.

from$8.99

All prices are NET prices.

Figure 1: Per capita mortality of models and defended mimics, and relative predation risk of models.
Figure 2: Per capita mortality of models and edible mimics, total numbers of models and edible mimics killed, and relative predation risk for models.

References

  1. 1

    Müller, F. Ueber die Vortheile der Mimicry bei Schmetterlingen. Zool. Anz. 1, 54–55 (1878)

  2. 2

    Dixey, F. A. Presidential Address, Section D. Rep. Br. Assoc. Advmt Sci. 199–207 (1919)

  3. 3

    Owen, R. E. & Owen, A. R. G. Mathematical paradigms for mimicry—recurrent sampling. J. Theor. Biol. 109, 217–247 (1984)

  4. 4

    Speed, M. P. Muellerian mimicry and the psychology of predation. Anim. Behav. 45, 571–580 (1993)

  5. 5

    Mallet, J. & Joron, M. Evolution of diversity in warning color and mimicry: Polymorphisms, shifting balance, and speciation. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 30, 201–233 (1999)

  6. 6

    Alatalo, R. V. & Mappes, J. Tracking the evolution of warning signals. Nature 382, 708–710 (1996)

  7. 7

    Dumbacher, J. P. & Fleischer, R. C. Phylogenetic evidence for colour pattern convergence in toxic pitohuis: Müllerian mimicry in birds? Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268, 1971–1976 (2001)

  8. 8

    Mallet, J. Causes and consequences of a lack of coevolution in Müllerian mimicry. Evol. Ecol. 13, 777–806 (1999)

  9. 9

    Kapan, D. D. Three-butterfly system provides a field test of müllerian mimicry. Nature 409, 338–340 (2001)

  10. 10

    Symula, R., Schulte, R. & Summers, K. Molecular phylogenetic evidence for a mimetic radiation in Peruvian poison frogs supports a Müllerian mimicry hypothesis. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268, 2415–2421 (2001)

  11. 11

    Benson, W. W. Natural selection for Müllerian mimicry in Heliconius erato in Costa Rica. Science 176, 936–939 (1972)

  12. 12

    Mallet, J. & Barton, N. H. Strong natural selection in a warning-colour hybrid zone. Evolution Int. J. Org. Evolution 43, 421–431 (1989)

  13. 13

    Rowe, C., Lindström, L. & Lyytinen, A. The importance of pattern similarity between Müllerian mimics in predator avoidance learning. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271, 407–413 (2004)

  14. 14

    Speed, M. P., Alderson, N. J., Hardman, C. & Ruxton, G. D. Testing Müllerian mimicry: an experiment with wild birds. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 267, 725–731 (2000)

  15. 15

    Sherratt, T. N., Speed, M. P. & Ruxton, G. D. Natural selection on unpalatable species imposed by state-dependent foraging behaviour. J. Theor. Biol. 228, 217–226 (2004)

  16. 16

    Kokko, H., Mappes, J. & Lindström, L. Alternative prey can change model–mimic dynamics between parasitism and mutualism. Ecol. Lett. 6, 1068–1076 (2003)

  17. 17

    Speed, M. P. Batesian, quasi-Batesian or Müllerian mimicry? Theory and data in mimicry research. Evol. Ecol. 13, 755–776 (1999)

  18. 18

    Bates, H. W. Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon valley. Lepidoptera: Heliconidae. Trans. Linn. Soc. Lond. 23, 495–566 (1862)

  19. 19

    Sheppard, P. M. Natural Selection and Heredity (Hutchinson, London, 1975)

  20. 20

    Fisher, R. A. The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection 2nd edn (Dover Publications, New York, 1958)

  21. 21

    Marshall, G. A. K. On diaposematism, with reference to some limitations of the Müllerian hypothesis of mimicry. Trans. Entomol. Soc. 1908, 93–142 (1908)

  22. 22

    Huheey, J. E. Studies in warning coloration and mimicry. VIII. Further evidence for a frequency-dependent model of predation. J. Herpetol. 14, 223–230 (1980)

  23. 23

    Sherratt, T. N. State-dependent risk-taking by predators in systems with defended prey. Oikos 103, 93–100 (2003)

  24. 24

    Ihalainen, E., Lindström, L. & Mappes, J. Investigating Müllerian mimicry: predator learning and variation in prey defences. J. Evol. Biol. 20, 780–791 (2007)

  25. 25

    Lindström, L., Alatalo, R. V., Lyytinen, A. & Mappes, J. Strong antiapostatic selection against novel rare aposematic prey. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 9181–9184 (2001)

  26. 26

    Huheey, J. E. Mathematical models of mimicry. Am. Nat. 31, s22–s41 (1988)

  27. 27

    Sheppard, P. M. The evolution of mimicry: a problem in ecology and genetics. Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 24, 131–140 (1959)

  28. 28

    Lindström, L., Lyytinen, A., Mappes, J. & Ojala, K. Relative importance of taste and visual appearance for predator education in Müllerian mimicry. Anim. Behav. 72, 323–333 (2006)

  29. 29

    Nonacs, P. Foraging in a dynamic mimicry complex. Am. Nat. 126, 165–180 (1985)

  30. 30

    Lindström, L., Alatalo, R. V., Lyytinen, A. & Mappes, J. The effect of alternative prey on the dynamics of imperfect Batesian and Müllerian mimicries. Evolution Int. J. Org. Evolution 58, 1294–1302 (2004)

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank H. Nisu for taking care of the birds, and G. Ruxton and I. Harvey for commenting on the manuscript. The Academy of Finland (CoE programme) and the University of Jyväskylä provided funding, and the Konnevesi Research Station provided facilities. H.M.R. is funded by NERC, and M.P.S. is funded by Leverhulme trust.

Author Contributions All authors contributed equally to the planning of the work and writing the paper. H.M.R. and E.I. were the primary data collectors. First authorship was decided between H.M.R. and E.I.; authors after the first are listed in alphabetical order.

Author information

Correspondence to Hannah M. Rowland.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints. The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

This file contains Supplementary Notes which include Supplementary Figure S1 illustrating the artificial prey items presented on the black-and-white aviary background; Supplementary Time census analyses investigating the mortality consequences to the prey when predators forage for a fixed time clarified by Supplementary Figure S2; Supplementary analysis of risk of predation during learning studies the relative risk of the Model in more detail during the learning process of the birds pictured in Supplementary Figure S3; and Supplementary analysis presented in Supplementary Tables S1-4 of the match between our experimental results and Müller’s original equation based on two estimates of the quantity of prey needed to reach learning. (PDF 256 kb)

Supplementary Figure S1 was inadvertently omitted from Supplementary Information document in original publication. This was corrected on 5th October 2007.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rowland, H., Ihalainen, E., Lindström, L. et al. Co-mimics have a mutualistic relationship despite unequal defences. Nature 448, 64–67 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1038/nature05899

Download citation

Further reading

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.