Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Neural correlates of implied motion

Abstract

Current views of the visual system assume that the primate brain analyses form and motion along largely independent pathways1; they provide no insight into why form is sometimes interpreted as motion. In a series of psychophysical and electrophysiological experiments in humans and macaques, here we show that some form information is processed in the prototypical motion areas of the superior temporal sulcus (STS). First, we show that STS cells respond to dynamic Glass patterns2, which contain no coherent motion but suggest a path of motion3. Second, we show that when motion signals conflict with form signals suggesting a different path of motion, both humans and monkeys perceive motion in a compromised direction. This compromise also has a correlate in the responses of STS cells, which alter their direction preferences in the presence of conflicting implied motion information. We conclude that cells in the prototypical motion areas in the dorsal visual cortex process form that implies motion. Estimating motion by combining motion cues with form cues may be a strategy to deal with the complexities of motion perception in our natural environment.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Optimal Glass patterns activate the STS population.
Figure 2: Responses to implied motion in a single cell.
Figure 3: Interactions between real and implied motion.
Figure 4: Interactions between real and implied motion in a single cell.
Figure 5: Monkey psychophysics.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L. & Macko, K. Object vision and spatial vision: two central pathways. Trends Neurosci. 6, 414–417 (1983)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Glass, L. Moiré effect from random dots. Nature 223, 578–580 (1969)

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Ross, J., Badcock, D. R. & Hayes, A. Coherent global motion in the absence of coherent velocity signals. Curr. Biol. 10, 679–682 (2000)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Geisler, W. S. Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction. Nature 400, 65–69 (1999)

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Geisler, W. S., Albrecht, D. G., Crane, A. M. & Stern, L. Motion direction signals in the primary visual cortex of cat and monkey. Vis. Neurosci. 18, 501–516 (2001)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Jancke, D. Orientation formed by a spot's trajectory: A two-dimensional population approach in primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 20, RC86 (1–6) (2000)

  7. Kourtzi, Z. & Kanwisher, N. Activation in human MT/MST by static images with implied motion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 48–55 (2000)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Wilson, H. R., Wilkinson, F. & Asaad, W. Concentric orientation summation in human form vision. Vision Res. 37, 2325–2330 (1997)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Burr, D. C. & Ross, J. Direct evidence that ‘Speedlines’ influence motion mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 22, 8661–8664 (2002)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Smith, M. A., Bair, W. & Movshon, J. A. Signals in macaque striate cortical neurons that support the perception of Glass patterns. J. Neurosci. 22, 8334–8345 (2002)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Schoppmann, A. & Hoffmann, K.-P. Continuous mapping of direction selectivity in the cat's visual cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 2, 177–181 (1976)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Bremmer, F., Ilg, U. J., Thiele, A., Distler, C. & Hoffmann, K. P. Eye position effects in monkey cortex. I. Visual and pursuit-related activity in extrastriate areas MT and MST. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 944–961 (1997)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Snowden, R. J., Treue, S., Erickson, R. G. & Andersen, R. A. The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion. J. Neurosci. 11, 2768–2785 (1991)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Qian, N. & Andersen, R. A. Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. II. Physiology. J. Neurosci. 14, 7367–7380 (1994)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Maunsell, J. H. & Van Essen, D. C. Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. J. Neurophysiol. 49, 1127–1147 (1983)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Albright, T. D. Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. J. Neurophysiol. 52, 1106–1130 (1984)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Pack, C. & Born, R. T. Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain. Nature 409, 1040–1042 (2001)

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Newsome, W. T. & Pare, E. B. A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). J. Neurosci. 8, 2201–2211 (1988)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Rudolph, K. & Pasternak, T. Transient and permanent deficits in motion perception after lesions of cortical areas MT and MST in the macaque monkey. Cereb. Cortex 9, 90–100 (1999)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Batschelet, E. Circular Statistics in Biology (Academic, London, 1981)

    MATH  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank J. Constanza, D. Diep, L. Abavare and M. Bronzel for technical assistance, C. Distler for the surgeries, and G. Stoner and T. Albright for comments and discussions. The Human Frontiers Science Program and the Australian Research Council supported this work financially.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bart Krekelberg.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Krekelberg, B., Dannenberg, S., Hoffmann, KP. et al. Neural correlates of implied motion. Nature 424, 674–677 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1038/nature01852

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nature01852

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing