Abstract
Cognitive impairment is common among individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). It has been suggested that some aspects of intelligence are preserved or even superior in people with ASD compared with controls, but consistent evidence is lacking. Few studies have examined the genetic overlap between cognitive ability and ASD/ADHD. The aim of this study was to examine the polygenic overlap between ASD/ADHD and cognitive ability in individuals from the general population. Polygenic risk for ADHD and ASD was calculated from genome-wide association studies of ASD and ADHD conducted by the Psychiatric Genetics Consortium. Risk scores were created in three independent cohorts: Generation Scotland Scottish Family Health Study (GS:SFHS) (n=9863), the Lothian Birth Cohorts 1936 and 1921 (n=1522), and the Brisbane Adolescent Twin Sample (BATS) (n=921). We report that polygenic risk for ASD is positively correlated with general cognitive ability (beta=0.07, P=6 × 10−7, r2=0.003), logical memory and verbal intelligence in GS:SFHS. This was replicated in BATS as a positive association with full-scale intelligent quotient (IQ) (beta=0.07, P=0.03, r2=0.005). We did not find consistent evidence that polygenic risk for ADHD was associated with cognitive function; however, a negative correlation with IQ at age 11 years (beta=−0.08, Z=−3.3, P=0.001) was observed in the Lothian Birth Cohorts. These findings are in individuals from the general population, suggesting that the relationship between genetic risk for ASD and intelligence is partly independent of clinical state. These data suggest that common genetic variation relevant for ASD influences general cognitive ability.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are pervasive neurodevelopmental disorders that manifest during childhood. These disorders are highly heritable,1, 2, 3, 4 and recent genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have found that a portion of this heritability is attributable to common genetic variants.5 Cognitive difficulties are common in individuals with ADHD or ASD. Children with ADHD have been found to have a 7–12 point lower average full-scale intelligent quotient (FIQ) compared with controls.6, 7 Furthermore, individuals with ADHD demonstrate reduced working memory capacity and poorer processing speed and reading comprehension.8, 9, 10, 11 Executive function deficits in children with ADHD have been found to persist into adulthood12 despite remission of ADHD symptoms13, 14 although other studies find that impairment persists only in individuals whose ADHD remains.15 The relationship between autism and intelligence is, however, more complex. The majority of autistic individuals are intellectually impaired16 although a few studies have shown areas of superior functioning compared with controls,17 particularly on non-verbal measures.18, 19
General cognitive ability (g) is a latent trait that can be extracted from performance across diverse tests of cognitive aptitude. General cognitive ability has heritability estimates of approximately 30% in young childhood20 increasing to 80% in adolescence.21 Like ADHD and ASD, differences in general cognitive ability are highly polygenic, with about half of the heritability captured by common genetic variants.22, 23, 24 Despite a strong phenotypic relationship between autism, ADHD and cognitive decrements, few studies have examined the genetic overlap between these traits. A study examining the genetic correlation between five psychiatric disorders did not find significant genetic overlap between ADHD and ASD attributable to common single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs).5 Twin studies suggest that there are shared genetic effects across autism,25, 26 ADHD and cognitive ability7, 27 although these studies are confounded by the clinical state of the affected individuals. Poor cognitive ability may arise via the social and communicative impairments present in autistic or ADHD individuals or the pleiotropic effect of genetic risk variants on cognition. A recent study found that rare copy number variants that increase risk for autism are associated with cognitive differences in healthy controls. Control carriers of the 16p11.2 deletion were significantly impaired on measures of verbal intelligence, working memory and executive function.28 However, no study to our knowledge has assessed the relationship between common genetic risk for ASD or ADHD and cognitive function in the general population.
The genetic overlap between different heritable traits can be investigated using polygenic risk profiling. This method takes the SNP effect sizes from a reference study and calculates the genome-wide weighted sum of the alleles that an individual carries, which then serves as an index of the genetic load for a particular disorder. This method has the advantage of analysing genetic overlap between cognitive ability, ADHD and ASD, without the confounding effect of disease state.
The aim of this study was to test whether ADHD and ASD polygenic profile scores29 are associated with cognitive ability in a large population-based cohort. Generation Scotland: Scottish Family Health Study (GS:SFHS)30 provides genome-wide genotyping data and four measures of cognitive function for 9863 individuals, some of whom are in family groups. Two other samples: the combined Lothian birth cohorts 1921 and 1936 (LBC1921 and LBC1936) (1522 individuals combined)31 and the Brisbane Adolescent Twin Study (BATS)32 were used for replication. LBC1921 and LBC1936 provided measures of cognitive function in childhood and old age for the same individuals. BATS consisted of 921 individuals with cognitive ability measured in adolescence. We hypothesized that individuals with a greater burden of ADHD or ASD risk alleles would perform worse on tests of cognitive function, consistent with findings in affected individuals. We also sought to test whether individuals with a high genetic load for ASD would perform better on non-verbal cognitive measures, similar to autistic individuals.
Methods
Cohort description and cognitive testing
Generation Scotland: Scottish Family Health Study (GS:SFHS)
GS:SFHS is a family-based epidemiological cohort; the protocol for recruitment is described in detail in the Supplementary Materials and in previous publications.30 Briefly, genome-wide SNP data were ascertained for 9863 individuals (mean age=52.2 years, s.d.=13.64), 5788 females and 4075 males (6815 unrelated participants). Four tests of cognitive function were available: Mill Hill vocabulary scale junior and senior synonyms,33 verbal declarative memory (logical memory),34 the Wechsler digit symbol substitution task (digit symbol coding (DSC)),34 and verbal fluency. A measure of general cognitive ability was derived by entering all four cognitive tests into a principal components analysis (PCA) and extracting the first unrotated principal component,35 which explained 44% of the variance across these tests. Each test loaded moderately onto the component (0.48–0.54). Further details on cognitive testing are included in the Supplementary Materials and summarized in Supplementary Table S1.
Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 (LBC1936)
The 1936 Lothian Birth Cohort (LBC1936) participants used in the present study consisted of 1005 (496 males and 509 females) community-dwelling individuals mostly living in and around the City of Edinburgh, Scotland. Almost all of them completed the Moray House Test (MHT) during the Scottish Mental Health Survey of 1947 at age 11 years, which was re-administered at a follow-up assessment at age 70 years (Scottish Research Council, 1933).36, 37 At age 70 years in LBC1936, the National Adult Reading Test (NART)38 and tests of logical memory, digit span backwards, spatial span and verbal paired associates were administered from the Wechsler memory scale (WMS-III UK).39 Tests from the Wechsler adult intelligence scale (WAIS-III) were also administered at age 70 years: DSC, block design, letter number sequencing, and matrix reasoning.34, 40 Verbal fluency41 and lifetime change in IQ were also assessed. General cognitive ability was constructed in LBC1936 by performing PCA of block design, matrix reasoning, letter number sequencing, digit span backwards, symbol search and DSC and extracting the first principal component. This component explained 53% of the variance across these six tests, (loading range 0.37–0.43). The LBC1936 cognitive tests are described in greater detail in the Supplementary Methods and summarized in Supplementary Table S1.
Lothian Birth Cohort 1921 (LBC1921)
The 1921 Lothian birth cohort (LBC1921) contributed 517 participants to the present study, 302 females and 215 males who completed the MHT at age 11 years as part of the Scottish Mental Health Survey in 1932.42, 43 The MHT was completed again at age 79 years. Participants completed the MHT at mean age 11 years and then again at mean age 79 years.31 At age 79 years, tests of logical memory,39 verbal fluency41 and Raven’s progressive matrices33 were administered. These three tests were entered into PCA to derive general cognitive ability, and the first principal component explained 52% of the variance across these tests (loadings 0.58–0.64), summarized in Supplementary Table S1. Change in IQ from age 11 to age 79 years was ascertained as described for LBC1936.
Brisbane Adolescent Twin Sample (BATS)
BATS consists of 4500 individuals (~1800 families) and targets twin adolescents and their siblings, with the majority recruited through primary and secondary schools in South East Queensland.32 The present study used 921 genotyped individuals randomly selected, 1 per family (53.2% females; mean age=16.7 years, s.d.=1.6). Measures of cognitive function included FIQ44 (N=902), verbal IQ (VIQ) (Subtests: Information, Vocabulary, Arithmetic; N=903) and performance IQ (Spatial, Object Assembly; N=902). Processing speed was obtained from the Digit Symbol Substitution task, a subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-R)40 (N=874). Reading ability was assessed using a contextualized version of the NART45 (N=813). The tests used to create FIQ are summarized in Supplementary Table S1.
Genotyping
GS:SFHS
Details of DNA extraction are described elsewhere.30 Briefly, blood samples were collected using standard operating procedures and stored using a laboratory information management system at the Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Facility Genetics Core, Edinburgh, UK (www.wtcrf.ed.ac.uk). DNA samples were genotyped by the WT-CRF using the IlluminaHumanOmniExpressExome -8v1.0 BeadChip (San Diego, CA, USA) and Infinum chemistry.46 Genotypes were processed using the GenomeStudio Analysis software v2011.1 (Illumina).
Lothian birth cohorts
Samples were subject to genome-wide genotyping via the extraction of venous blood from participants and genotyping at the Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Facility, Edinburgh, UK (www.wtcrf.ed.ac.uk). Genotyping was performed using the Illumina 610-Quadv1 whole-genome SNP array (Illumina, San Diego, CA, USA). The sample collection, quality control and genotyping process is described in greater detail elsewhere.47
BATS
Genome-wide genotyping data was collected (from DNA extracted from venous blood) using the Illumina 610-Quadv1 whole-genome SNP array. Quality control and imputation was carried out using the ENIGMA (Enhancing Neuro Imaging Genetics through Meta-Analysis) protocols.48 Imputation was carried out to the 1000G phase1 integrated reference panel (April 2012, NCBI build 37) using MiniMac, a computationally efficient implementation of the MaCH algorithm.49
Polygenic profiling
The method to create polygenic risk scores has been previously described50 and is implemented in PLINK.51 Further detail is included in the Supplementary Methods. Summary statistics from the Psychiatric Genetics Consortium (PGC) cross-disorder GWAS, consisting of 4788 ASD trio cases, 4788 trio ASD pseudo controls, 161 ASD cases, 526 ASD controls and 1947 ADHD trio cases, 1947 trio ADHD pseudo controls, 840 ADHD cases and 688 ADHD controls,5 were used as the discovery set to create polygenic risk scores for ASD and ADHD. The PGC GWAS of ADHD and ASD explained 28% and 17% of the variance in liability to those disorders, respectively.5, 29 Any SNPs genotyped or imputed in our cohorts were used to create the polygenic risk scores, not just those showing statistically significant associations in the original GWAS. Five SNP set scores were generated, using P-value threshold cutoffs of 0.01, 0.05, 0.1, 0.5 and 1 from the original GWAS; however, as the SNP-set derived from a P-value threshold <0.5 explained the greatest amount of variance in cognitive function (Figure 1), only these results are presented throughout. Profile scores were generated using raw genotype data for GS:SFHS and the LBC. Polygenic profiling for the BATS cohort was performed on data imputed to the 1000 genomes data set using the ENIGMA2 protocol described elsewhere.48 For comparability of results, we calculated profile scores for GS:SFHS and the LBC using imputed genotypes and found the results to be largely consistent (see Supplementary Materials for details).
Statistical analyses
Generation of Scotland cohort
Mixed linear model analyses were implemented in ASReml-R (www.vsni.co.uk/software/asreml), with cognitive test scores as the dependent variable and age, sex, the first four MDS components and polygenic risk score as fixed effects. Family structure was fitted as a random effect by creating the inverse of a relationship matrix using pedigree kinship information. P-values for fixed effects were calculated using Wald’s conditional F-test. In order to calculate the variance in cognitive test scores explained by polygene score, the change in the sum of residual variance and the additive genetic variance after removing the polygenic risk score from the model and then dividing this by the sum of residual variance and the additive genetic variance. There is a substantial male preponderance in ASD and ADHD, and therefore the interaction between sex and each polygenic risk score was also analysed by fitting an interaction term in each model (Supplementary Table S6).
LBC cohorts
Linear regression analyses were implemented in the R statistical software package (http:www.r-project.org/), using models that adjusted for age, sex and the first four MDS components for population stratification. The cognitive phenotypes were assigned as the dependent variable with polygenic score for autism or ADHD as the independent variable. Results from the LBC1936 and LBC1921 analyses were combined together and a fixed-effect meta-analysis performed to increase power. Fixed-effect meta-analyses were carried out using the ‘meta’ package implemented in R.
BATS
Linear regression analyses were carried out as in the LBC cohorts, implemented in STATA (version 11, Statacorp, College Station, TX, USA).
Results
Relationship of ASD polygenic score to cognition
Polygenic risk for ASD was positively associated with general cognitive ability in GS:SFHS (beta=0.07, P=6 × 10−7). Polygenic load for ASD in GS:SFHS was also associated with three of the individual tests, delayed and immediate logical memory combined (beta=0.04, P=1 × 10−4), performance on the Mill Hill vocabulary test (beta=0.05, P=3 × 10−6) and verbal fluency (beta=0.04, P=2 × 10−4) (Table 1). These P-values all remain significant after Bonferonni correction. Greater polygenic risk for ASD was associated with better cognitive function across all four of these measures in this large adult population. The effect sizes are small. ASD polygenic scores explain <0.5% of the variance in cognitive test scores in all cases (Figure 1). A positive relationship between ASD polygenic risk scores and cognitive ability was also found in BATS with FIQ (beta=0.07, P=0.029) and VIQ (beta=0.07, P=0.029) (Table 2). A fixed-effect meta-analysis of general cognitive ability in GS:SFHS and LBC and FIQ in BATS found a significant positive association of ASD risk with cognitive ability (beta=0.06, Z=6.75, P<0.0001; Figure 2). In the LBC cohorts, no significant associations with ASD polygenic risk were found in the combined meta-analyses (Table 3). Additional cognitive tests derived from the WMS-III and WAIS-III were also tested for association with autism polygenic risk in LBC1936; however, no significant associations were found (Supplementary Table S2).
Relationship of ADHD polygenic score to cognition
No significant associations between polygenic risk for ADHD and cognitive ability were found in GS:SFHS (Table 1). No significant associations between polygenic risk for ADHD and cognitive ability were found in BATS (Table 2). Polygenic risk for ADHD was associated with age 11 IQ in the LBC1936 cohort; however, in contrast to ASD, greater polygenic load was negatively correlated with IQ (beta=−0.09, P=0.004). This result was not replicated in LBC1921. A meta-analysis of the LBC1921 and LBC1936 revealed a significant association with age 11 IQ (beta=−0.08, Z=−3.3, P=0.001), and this P-value remains significant after Bonferonni correction for multiple testing. As previous studies have found significant genetic covariance between ADHD and depression,5 statistical analyses were also carried out on the GS:SFHS control individuals (n=7667) after removing those with a lifetime diagnosis of depression. A negative association between ADHD polygenic risk scores and DSC scores in GS:SFHS was significant when depressed individuals were removed from the analysis (beta=−0.02, P=0.03) (Supplementary Table S4).
Discussion
This study found a positive association between common genetic risk variants for ASD and general cognitive ability in individuals drawn from the general population. ASD polygenic risk scores were also positively correlated with measures of verbal fluency, logical memory and vocabulary. In contrast, genetic risk for ADHD was negatively correlated with age 11 IQ in members of the combined Lothian Birth Cohorts, although this finding was not confirmed in any of the other cohorts. The degree of genetic overlap between ASD and ADHD attributable to common genetic variation has previously been shown to be non-significant,5 and therefore a differential effect of ASD and ADHD polygenic risk scores is plausible. These results provide evidence that common genetic variation associated with ASD confers better general cognitive ability in a non-clinical population.
The rate of intellectual disability in autistic individuals is estimated to be 70%16 although this is lower when broader ASDs are included.52 A recent study examined the impact of copy number variants associated with neuropsychiatric disease on cognition in healthy controls.28 A rare chromosome 16p11.2 deletion, which is a highly penetrant genetic risk factor in autism,53 was found to significantly impair cognitive function in healthy control carriers. In contrast, our results suggest that common genetic risk for autism correlates positively with cognitive function. The relationship between autism and intelligence may therefore be complicated by the existence of distinct classes of genetic risk. Autism has the highest twin heritability of all psychiatric disorders,54, 55 yet only 17% of this variance is attributable to common variants.5 This discrepancy is most likely owing to rare and de novo mutations, which are increasingly recognized as an important source of genetic risk for autism56 and are negatively associated with cognition in healthy individuals.28 Individuals with high functioning autism often display an atypical Wechsler intelligence profile, with strengths on matrix reasoning and block design and weaknesses on DSC and symbol search subtests.57, 58, 59, 60 We did not find any evidence that genetic risk for autism in non-clinical individuals was consistently associated with strengths or weaknesses in any of these domains. It is notable, however, that in the GS:SFHS, DSC was the only test of cognition where individuals with high polygenic profiles scores for autism did not perform better.
Individuals with ADHD typically have poorer cognitive abilities than their age-matched counterparts. Executive function deficits are found to persist into adulthood8 despite remission of ADHD symptoms13, 14 although other studies find impairments only in individuals whose ADHD remains.15 Symptoms of ADHD, such as hyperactivity and inattention, associate with scholastic impairment in children drawn from a population-based sample.61 Furthermore, polygenic risk for ADHD associates positively with hyperactivity and impulsivity in the general population.62 We provide suggestive evidence that polygenic risk for ADHD associates with ADHD-like traits in the general population by showing a negative relationship between age 11 IQ and ADHD genetic risk in the LBC. We also demonstrate a weak negative association with DSC in GS:SFHS when individuals with a lifetime diagnosis of depression were removed from the sample. Taken together, these data suggest that ADHD may lie on a continuous distribution of traits present in the general population; however, the association of ADHD polygenic risk and cognitive impairment should be interpreted cautiously and warrants further replication, as consistent evidence for this was not found across all cohorts in this study. Sex-specific analyses revealed a significant interaction between sex and ASD polygenic risk score in the context of DSC in GS:SFHS, indicating that genetic risk for ASD in females is positively correlated with performance on DSC, in contrast to males. There is a notable sex bias in ADHD and ASD prevalence, with roughly 2.5 males affected for every female for ADHD63 and 4 males for every female affected in ASD.64 Interestingly, these data show that the effects of ASD genetic risk on DSC performance are more pronounced in females from the general population.
There are a number of limitations to this study that should be noted. Consistent replication was not achieved across studies. Polygenic risk for autism, although robustly associated with cognition in GS:SFHS, only replicated in BATS. The failure to consistently replicate the finding in GS:SFHS may be due to the smaller sample size of the LBC cohorts. Furthermore, the LBCs comprise older participants and measuring general cognitive ability in this age group may have affected the results. Similarly, ADHD was found to associate with age 11 IQ in the LBC cohort, but this did not replicate in BATS or GS:SFHS, although the relationship between cognitive function and ADHD risk was negative in 4/5 tests in GS:SFHS. Furthermore, many of the significant P-values reported in this study are modest and do not survive correction for multiple testing. The lack of replication may have arisen from the unavoidable but significant heterogeneity among cognitive tests across the three cohorts. A lack of power to detect association in the smaller cohorts may also have contributed to the lack of replication. This is not unexpected when considering the amount of variance in cognitive function we were able to explain in GS:SFHS: <0.5% in all cases. This is consistent with other studies that have employed polygenic risk scoring to assess genetic overlap between disorders or across cohorts. According to recent estimates, 32% of the genetic liability for schizophrenia (SCZ) can be explained by common genetic variation; however, using polygenic risk scores to predict SCZ status in an independent cohort, only ~7% of the variance in liability could be explained.65 The genetic variance explained by common SNPs increases as a function of sample size. As the PGC GWAS of ASD and ADHD were considerably smaller than the SCZ GWAS (N for autism ~6700, ADHD ~16 000, SCZ ~150 000), the degree of polygenic overlap between autism, ADHD and cognition will be an underestimate.
The current study suggests that genetic risk for autism is positively correlated with cognitive function in non-clinical cohorts and is likely to be independent of ASD pathology. The genetic basis of ADHD and autism are poorly understood; however, understanding the significant and opposing directions of genetic covariance between these traits and cognitive function will provide opportunities to investigate their biological origins.
References
Hallmayer J, Cleveland S, Torres A, Phillips J, Cohen B, Torigoe T et al. Genetic heritability and shared environmental factors among twin pairs with autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68: 1095–1102.
Larsson H, Dilshad R, Lichtenstein P, Barker ED . Developmental trajectories of DSM-IV symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: genetic effects, family risk and associated psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2011; 52: 954–963.
Lichtenstein P, Carlstrom E, Rastam M, Gillberg C, Anckarsater H . The genetics of autism spectrum disorders and related neuropsychiatric disorders in childhood. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167: 1357–1363.
Sherman DK, McGue MK, Iacono WG . Twin concordance for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a comparison of teachers' and mothers' reports. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154: 532–535.
Lee SH, Ripke S, Neale BM, Faraone SV, Purcell SM, Perlis RH et al. Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Nat Genet 2013; 45: 984–994.
Frazier TW, Demaree HA, Youngstrom EA . Meta-analysis of intellectual and neuropsychological test performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology 2004; 18: 543–555.
Kuntsi J, Eley TC, Taylor A, Hughes C, Asherson P, Caspi A et al. Co-occurrence of ADHD and low IQ has genetic origins. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2004; 124B: 41–47.
Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Doyle AE, Seidman LJ, Wilens TE, Ferrero F et al. Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. J Consult Clin Psychol 2004; 72: 757–766.
Mayes SD, Calhoun SL . WISC-IV and WISC-III profiles in children with ADHD. J Atten Disord 2006; 9: 486–493.
Miller AC, Keenan JM, Betjemann RS, Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Olson RK . Reading comprehension in children with ADHD: cognitive underpinnings of the centrality deficit. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2013; 41: 473–483.
Tillman C, Eninger L, Forssman L, Bohlin G . The relation between working memory components and ADHD symptoms from a developmental perspective. Dev Neuropsychol 2013; 36: 181–198.
Biederman J, Fried R, Petty CR, Wozniak J, Doyle AE, Henin A et al. Cognitive development in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a controlled study in medication-naive adults across the adult life cycle. J Clin Psychiatry 2011; 72: 11–16.
Biederman J, Petty CR, Ball SW, Fried R, Doyle AE, Cohen D et al. Are cognitive deficits in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder related to the course of the disorder? A prospective controlled follow-up study of grown up boys with persistent and remitting course. Psychiatry Res 2009; 170: 177–182.
Miller M, Ho J, Hinshaw SP . Executive functions in girls with ADHD followed prospectively into young adulthood. Neuropsychology 2012; 26: 278–287.
Halperin JM, Trampush JW, Miller CJ, Marks DJ, Newcorn JH . Neuropsychological outcome in adolescents/young adults with childhood ADHD: profiles of persisters, remitters and controls. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008; 49: 958–966.
Fombonne E . Past and future perspectives on autism epidemiology. Understanding Autism, from Basic Neuroscience to Treatment. Taylor & Francis, New York, USA, 2006, pp 25–48.
Iuculano T, Rosenberg-Lee M, Supekar K, Lynch CJ, Khouzam A, Phillips J et al. Brain organization underlying superior mathematical abilities in children with autism. Biol Psychiatry 2014; 75: 223–230.
Bolte S, Dziobek I, Poustka F . Brief report: the level and nature of autistic intelligence revisited. J Autism Dev Disord 2009; 39: 678–682.
Dawson M, Soulieres I, Gernsbacher MA, Mottron L . The level and nature of autistic intelligence. Psychol Sci 2007; 18: 657–662.
Spinath FM, Ronald A, Harlaar N, Price TS, Plomin R . Phenotypic g early in life: on the etiology of general cognitive ability in a large population of twin children aged 2-4 years. Intelligence 2003; 31: 195–210.
Edmonds CJ, Isaacs EB, Visscher PM, Rogers M, Lanigan J, Singhal A et al. Inspection time and cognitive abilities in twins aged 7 to 17 years: age-related changes, heritability, and genetic covariance. Intelligence 2008; 36: 210–225.
Benyamin B, Pourcain B, Davis OS, Davies G, Hansell NK, Brion MJ et al. Childhood intelligence is heritable, highly polygenic and associated with FNBP1L. Mol Psychiatry 2014; 19: 253–258.
Deary IJ, Johnson W, Houlihan LM . Genetic foundations of human intelligence. Hum Genet 2009; 126: 215–232.
Plomin R, Haworth CM, Meaburn EL, Price TS, Davis OS . Common DNA markers can account for more than half of the genetic influence on cognitive abilities. Psychol Sci 2013; 24: 562–568.
Hoekstra RA, Happe F, Baron-Cohen S, Ronald A . Limited genetic covariance between autistic traits and intelligence: findings from a longitudinal twin study. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2010; 153B: 994–1007.
Nishiyama T, Taniai H, Miyachi T, Ozaki K, Tomita M, Sumi S . Genetic correlation between autistic traits and IQ in a population-based sample of twins with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). J Hum Genet 2009; 54: 56–61.
Greven CU, Rijsdijk FV, Asherson P, Plomin R . A longitudinal twin study on the association between ADHD symptoms and reading. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2011; 53: 234–242.
Stefansson H, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Steinberg S, Magnusdottir B, Morgen K, Arnarsdottir S et al. CNVs conferring risk of autism or schizophrenia affect cognition in controls. Nature 2013; 505: 361–366.
Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genetics Consortium. Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet 2013; 381: 1371–1379.
Smith BH, Campbell H, Blackwood D, Connell J, Connor M, Deary IJ et al. Generation Scotland: the Scottish Family Health Study; a new resource for researching genes and heritability. BMC Med Genet 2006; 7: 74.
Deary IJ, Gow AJ, Pattie A, Starr JM . Cohort profile: the Lothian Birth Cohorts of 1921 and 1936. Int J Epidemiol 2011; 41: 1576–1584.
Wright MJ, Martin NG . Brisbane Adolescent Twin Study: outline of study methods and research projects. Aust J Psychology 2004; 56: 14.
Raven JC, Court JH, Raven J . Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. H.K.Lewis: London, UK, 1977.
Wechsler D . WAIS-III UK Administration and Scoring Manual. Psychological Corporation: London, UK, 1998.
Deary IJ, Penke L, Johnson W . The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nat Rev Neurosci 2010; 11: 201–211.
Deary IJ, Gow AJ, Taylor MD, Corley J, Brett C, Wilson V et al. The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936: a study to examine influences on cognitive ageing from age 11 to age 70 and beyond. BMC Geriatr 2007; 7: 28.
Education SCfRi The Trend of Scottish Intelligence: A Comparison of the 1947 and 1932 Surveys of the Intelligence of Eleven-Year-Old Pupils. University of London Press: London, UK, 1949.
Nelson HE . The National Adult Reading Test (NART): test manual. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson 1982.
Wechsler D . Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1987.
Wechsler D . Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. Psychological Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1981.
Lezak MD . Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995.
Scottish Council for Research in Education:The intelligence of Scottish children: A National Survey of an age-group. University of London Press: London, UK, 1933.
Deary IJ, Whiteman MC, Starr JM, Whalley LJ, Fox HC . The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: following up the Scottish mental surveys of 1932 and 1947. J Pers Soc Psychol 2004; 86: 130–147.
Jackson DN . Multidimensional Aptitude Battery II. Sigma Assessment Systems: Port Huron, MI, USA, 1998.
Beardsall L, Huppert FA . Improvement in NART word reading in demented and normal older persons using the Cambridge Contextual Reading Test. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1994; 16: 232–242.
Gunderson KL . Whole-genome genotyping on bead arrays. Methods Mol Biol 2009; 529: 197–213.
Davies G, Tenesa A, Payton A, Yang J, Harris SE, Liewald D et al. Genome-wide association studies establish that human intelligence is highly heritable and polygenic. Mol Psychiatry 2011; 16: 996–1005.
Team Egs. ENIGMA2 1KGP Cookbook (v3) (Online). Enhancing Neuroimaging Genetics through MetaAnalysis (ENIGMA) Consortium, 2013.
Howie B, Fuchsberger C, Stephens M, Marchini J, Abecasis GR . Fast and accurate genotype imputation in genome-wide association studies through pre-phasing. Nat Genet 44: 955–959.
Purcell SM, Wray NR, Stone JL, Visscher PM, O'Donovan MC, Sullivan PF et al. Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature 2009; 460: 748–752.
Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MA, Bender D et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet 2007; 81: 559–575.
Chakrabarti S, Fombonne E . Pervasive developmental disorders in preschool children: confirmation of high prevalence. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162: 1133–1141.
Weiss LA, Shen Y, Korn JM, Arking DE, Miller DT, Fossdal R et al. Association between microdeletion and microduplication at 16p11.2 and autism. N Engl J Med 2008; 358: 667–675.
Bailey A, Le Couteur A, Gottesman I, Bolton P, Simonoff E, Yuzda E et al. Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study. Psychol Med 1995; 25: 63–77.
Rosenberg RE, Law JK, Yenokyan G, McGready J, Kaufmann WE, Law PA . Characteristics and concordance of autism spectrum disorders among 277 twin pairs. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2009; 163: 907–914.
Ronemus M, Iossifov I, Levy D, Wigler M . The role of de novo mutations in the genetics of autism spectrum disorders. Nat Rev Genet 2014; 15: 133–141.
Dennis M, Lockyer L, Lazenby AL, Donnelly RE, Wilkinson M, Schoonheyt W . Intelligence patterns among children with high-functioning autism, phenylketonuria, and childhood head injury. J Autism Dev Disord 1999; 29: 5–17.
Koyama T, Tachimori H, Osada H, Takeda T, Kurita H . Cognitive and symptom profiles in Asperger's syndrome and high-functioning autism. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2007; 61: 99–104.
Mayes SD, Calhoun SL . WISC-IV and WIAT-II profiles in children with high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2008; 38: 428–439.
Oliveras-Rentas RE, Kenworthy L, Roberson RB 3rd, Martin A, Wallace GL . WISC-IV profile in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: impaired processing speed is associated with increased autism communication symptoms and decreased adaptive communication abilities. J Autism Dev Disord 2012; 42: 655–664.
Rodriguez A, Jarvelin MR, Obel C, Taanila A, Miettunen J, Moilanen I et al. Do inattention and hyperactivity symptoms equal scholastic impairment? Evidence from three European cohorts. BMC Public Health 2007; 7: 327.
Martin JM, Hamshere ML, Stergiakouli E, O'Donovan MC, Thapar A . Genetic risk for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder contributes to neurodevelopmental traits in the general population. Biol Psychiatry 2014; 76: 664–671.
Visser SN, Bitsko RH, Danielson ML, Perou R . Increasing prevalence of parent-reported attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among children — United States, 2003 and 2007. MMWR 2010; 59: 1439–1443.
Chakrabarti S, Fombonne E . Pervasive developmental disorders in preschool children. JAMA 2001; 285: 3093–3099.
Psychiatric Genetics Consortium - Schizophrenia Working Group. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature 2014; 511: 421–427.
Acknowledgements
Generation Scotland has received core funding from the Chief Scientist Office of the Scottish Government Health Directorates CZD/16/6 and the Scottish Funding Council HR03006. We are grateful to all the families who took part, the general practitioners and the Scottish School of Primary Care for their help in recruiting them and the whole Generation Scotland team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, health-care assistants and nurses. We acknowledge with gratitude the financial support received for this work from the Dr Mortimer and Theresa Sackler Foundation. For the Lothian Birth Cohorts (LBC1921 and LBC1936), we thank Paul Redmond for database management assistance; Alan Gow, Martha Whiteman, Alison Pattie, Michelle Taylor, Janie Corley, Caroline Brett and Caroline Cameron for data collection and data entry; nurses and staff at the Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Facility, where blood extraction and genotyping was performed; staff at the Lothian Health Board; and the staff at the SCRE Centre, University of Glasgow. The research was supported by a program grant from Age UK (Disconnected Mind) and by grants from the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC). The work was undertaken by The University of Edinburgh Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, part of the cross council Lifelong Health and Wellbeing Initiative (MR/K026992/1). Funding from the Medical Research Council (MRC) and BBSRC is gratefully acknowledged. DJM is an NRS Career Research Fellow funded by the CSO. BATS were funded by the Australian Research Council (A79600334, A79906588, A79801419, DP0212016, DP0664638, and DP1093900) and the National Health and Medical Research Council (389875) Australia. MKL is supported by a Perpetual Foundation Wilson Fellowship. SEM is supported by a Future Fellowship (FT110100548) from the Australian Research Council. GWM is supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC), Australia, Fellowship (619667). We thank the twins and siblings for their participation, Marlene Grace, Ann Eldridge and Natalie Garden for cognitive assessments, Kerrie McAloney, Daniel Park, David Smyth and Harry Beeby for research support, Anjali Henders and staff in the Molecular Epidemiology Laboratory for DNA sample processing and preparation and Scott Gordon for quality control and management of the genotypes. This work is supported by a Stragetic Award from the Wellcome Trust, reference 104036/Z/14/Z.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
AMM has received financial support from Pfizer (formerly Wyeth), Janssen and Lilly. AMM has done consultancy work for Roche Pharmaceuticals. The other authors declare no conflict of interest.
Additional information
Supplementary Information accompanies the paper on the Molecular Psychiatry website
Supplementary information
PowerPoint slides
Rights and permissions
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if thematerial is not included under the Creative Commons license, users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to reproduce the material. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
About this article
Cite this article
Clarke, TK., Lupton, M., Fernandez-Pujals, A. et al. Common polygenic risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is associated with cognitive ability in the general population. Mol Psychiatry 21, 419–425 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2015.12
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2015.12
This article is cited by
-
A predictive ensemble classifier for the gene expression diagnosis of ASD at ages 1 to 4 years
Molecular Psychiatry (2023)
-
Oxytocin Exposure in Labor and its Relationship with Cognitive Impairment and the Genetic Architecture of Autism
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2023)
-
Clinical autism subscales have common genetic liabilities that are heritable, pleiotropic, and generalizable to the general population
Translational Psychiatry (2022)
-
Identification of shared and differentiating genetic architecture for autism spectrum disorder, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and case subgroups
Nature Genetics (2022)
-
Discordant associations of educational attainment with ASD and ADHD implicate a polygenic form of pleiotropy
Nature Communications (2021)