Main

Extracellular matrix (ECM) molecules have important roles in the structural changes of brain synapses involved in neural plasticity, learning, and memory.1 ECM interacts with cells through cell surface receptors, such as integrin, cadherin, and neural adhesion molecules,2 and these interactions affect cell proliferation, growth, migration, synaptic stabilization, and apoptosis. Thus, the ECM develops a wide range of signals within the brain tissue.3

Matrix metalloproteinases (MMPs) comprise a family of protein-digesting enzymes that have an important role in structural maintenance and conversion of the ECM.4 MMPs target many substrates, including proteases, growth factors, cytokines, cell surface receptors, and cell adhesion molecules.5 Excess activation of MMPs occurs under several pathophysiological conditions, such as rheumatoid arthritis and rupture of atherosclerotic plaques.6, 7, 8 Thus, MMP activity is tightly controlled at the level of transcription, activation of the precursor zymogens, and inhibition by the tissue inhibitors of metalloproteinase (Timp).9

At present, four members of the Timp family (Timp-1 to Timp-4), possessing 35–50% amino-acid sequence homology, have been identified. All Timp isotypes contain 12 cysteines that form 6 disulfide bonds. To inhibit MMP activity, Timp proteins form a 1:1 complex with a zinc-binding site in the catalytic region of MMP.10

Each Timp protein has unique characteristics. Timp-1, Timp-2, and Timp-4 are present in soluble form.11, 12, 13 Timp-3, which is tightly bound to the ECM, is involved in cell proliferation, apoptosis, and angiogenesis.11, 12, 13 Timp-1 binds to proMMP-9, Timp-2 binds to proMMP-2, and Timp-3 binds to both proMMP-2 and proMMP-9.13, 14

In the central nervous system, the MMP/Timp system is responsive to changes in neural activity.15 Deregulation of MMP activity is involved in various neurological diseases, including multiple sclerosis, infection with human immunodeficiency virus, and spinal cord injury.16, 17, 18 Moreover, MMPs are implicated in the invasion of malignant glioma cells into the brain parenchyma.19

In Alzheimer's disease, MMP inhibits angiogenesis and accumulation of amyloid-β.16, 20, 21 Recently, MMPs have been reported to control synaptic activity in the hippocampus and to affect learning and memory.22 Other studies have clarified the contributions of Timp-1 and Timp-2 in learning and memory. Mice deficient for Timp-1 or Timp-2 exhibit defective memory function.23, 24, 25 Conversely, mice overexpressing Timp-1 showed a slight, but significant, improvement in learning and memory.24 However, there is no published evidence for a role of Timp-3 in the regulation of cognitive function.

This study investigated the effects of Timp-3 on learning and memory. We conducted various behavioral tests with wild-type (WT) and Timp-3 knockout (KO) mice, and further examined the expression of Timp-3 and compared gelatinolytic activity in WT and KO brain tissues.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Timp-3 KO Mice

Timp-3 KO mice were produced using the gene-targeting technique described by Kawamoto et al.26 Briefly, mice carrying the mutant allele were backcrossed with C57BL/6 mice to generate KO mice in a C57BL/6 background. Genotyping of mice was performed by PCR using tail DNA.26

Experimental Conditions for Behavioral Tests

All behavioral tests were conducted in the laboratory at 22 °C and 55% (50–60%) humidity. Illumination for the experimental device was set at 250 lx. The ventilation fan provided a masking noise of 40 dB, which was deemed appropriate for behavioral tests.

Mice were maintained in individual acrylic cages and naturalized to the environment during the 3 days before testing. They were given access to dry, solid feed (Labo MR Stock from Nihon Nosan, Yokohama, Japan) ad libitum. The room was maintained on a 12-h light and dark cycle, with the light cycle starting at 0800 hours and ending at 2000 hours. All tests were started after 1000 hours and conducted during the light period.

The passive avoidance test and water maze test with an invisible platform were conducted with 12 male WT mice and 12 KO mice. The open-field habituation test, active avoidance test, and water maze test with a visible platform were conducted using a different set of 12 male WT mice and 12 KO mice.

The experimental protocols were approved by the Osaka University Medical School Animal Care and Use Committee, and performed according to the Osaka University Medical School Guidelines for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

Passive Avoidance Test

An avoidance-learning box was constructed, with a lighted chamber (15 × 15 × 20 cm3) and dark chamber (15 × 15 × 20 cm3) connected to each other. A guillotine door separated the two compartments. A mouse was placed in the lighted chamber, and the guillotine door was opened. When the mouse spontaneously moved into the dark chamber, the guillotine door was closed. Within 10 s, a 3-s, 160-V AC electrical shock was delivered through the grid floor. The latency period for the mouse to enter the dark chamber was recorded. After this single learning trial, the mouse was immediately removed from the device.

After 24 h, the same mouse was put in the lighted chamber for the single retention test. The latency period (≤300 s) for the mouse to enter the dark chamber was recorded. No electric shock was given during the retention test.

Active Avoidance Test

An avoidance-learning box with two connecting compartments (each 15 × 15 × 20 cm3) was constructed. The mouse was able to move freely between the two compartments. To detect movement, two infrared ray beams were attached on both walls on the sides of each compartment, 2 cm above the floor and 5 cm from the gate. The avoidance-learning box, which was placed in a ventilated, sound-attenuating chamber to maintain a background noise level of 64 dB throughout the session, was indirectly illuminated by white bulbs fixed to the ceiling of the chamber. A 1500-Hz pure tone with 85 dB of sound pressure was used as a conditioned stimulus (CS), and a 140-V AC electrical shock delivered from the grid floor was used as an unconditioned stimulus (US).

A mouse was placed in one compartment and allowed to move between the two compartments throughout the training session. The US, which was delivered from the grid floor 5 s after the CS was delivered, overlapped for a maximum of 15 s. When the mouse moved to the connecting compartment within 5 s from the time of the CS, the US was not delivered, and the movement of the mouse was counted as avoidance behavior. When the mouse failed to move to the connecting compartment, the US was delivered. Both the CS and US were terminated immediately when the mouse moved to the connecting compartment after the onset of the US. The number of migration reactions in inter-trial intervals (25 s on average) was measured as an indicator of spontaneous activity. This active avoidance test was performed for 3 consecutive days, with one 50-trial session per day.

Water Maze Test

The water maze test was conducted using a round pool (inside diameter=95 cm; depth=35 cm) filled with water, made opaque by the addition of titanium oxide to a depth of 22 cm. The temperature of the water was maintained at 22±1 °C using a thermostatic heater. The pool was set on a pedestal (30 cm in height), and was enclosed (area=130 × 130 cm2) by four white walls (120 cm in height). As extra-maze cues, letter-sized posters, a CCD camera, and a black doll (20 cm in height) were attached to the walls.

In the invisible platform test, a clear, round platform (diameter=10 cm) was submerged 0.5 cm below the surface of the water. Training trials were conducted for 5 consecutive days, with 5 trial sessions per day. In the three quadrants away from the platform, a mouse placed near the wall was released into the pool. The releasing quadrants varied with pseudo-random sequences for each mouse. The escape latency period (measured for a maximum of 60 s) was defined as the time it took for a mouse to reach the platform. The training trial terminated when the mouse reached the platform and remained on it for 10 s. In cases in which the mouse did not find the platform within 60 s, the mouse was guided to the platform by the experimenter and was kept on the platform for 10 s. On the day after 5 consecutive days of access training, the platform was removed, and a 1-min probe test was performed. The 1-min probe test measured latency time in the quadrant in which the platform was previously placed.

The visible platform test was conducted independently using a different group of 12 WT and 12 KO mice (ie, different groups of mice from those used in the invisible platform test). Methodology for the visible platform test was similar, except (1) the platform was not submerged, but rather was placed 0.5 cm above the surface of the water, and (2) as a cue, a stick with a black cube on top was placed on the platform.

Open-Field Habituation Test

The apparatus for the open-field habituation test was manufactured by Taiyo Electric. (Tokyo, Japan). WT and KO mice were placed in an acrylic box (30 × 30 × 30 cm3) stored within a ventilated, soundproof chamber. An incandescent bulb, which was fixed to the ceiling of the chamber, provided lighting of 110 lx in the chamber. A fan attached to the wall of the chamber produced a masking noise of 45 dB. Habituation of the mice to the environment was measured as a function of locomotion and rearing behavior. The number of episodes of locomotion and rearing behavior, which were recorded with infrared ray beams placed on the lateral side of the box, was scored. The open-field habituation test was conducted for 3 days, with one 10-min session per day.

Reverse Transcription-PCR

RNA was isolated from mouse hippocampus using the ISOGEN (Nippon Gene, Tokyo, Japan) kit, according to the manufacturer's instructions. Timp-3 gene expression was detected by reverse transcribing the isolated RNA and amplifying the product with PCR (RT-PCR). The PCR amplification was performed with the following primers: Timp-3: Timp-3F, 5′-CACGGAAGCCTCTGAAAGTC-3′, and Timp-3R, 5′-CCCAAAATTGGAGAGCATGT-3′. GAPDH: GAPDH-F, 5′-AAATGGTGAAGGTCGGTGTG-3′, and GAPDH-R, 5′-GCAGAAGGGGCGGAGATGAT-3′.

Immunostaining

WT and Timp-3 KO mice were killed, and their brains were collected, fixed with formaldehyde, embedded in paraffin, and cut into 10-μm-thick sections with a microtome. To remove endogenous peroxidase, the sections were treated with 0.3% hydrogen peroxide in methanol at room temperature. The sections were washed with 0.05 M phosphate buffer (pH 7.6) thrice for 3 min. The sections were then treated with phosphate buffer containing 0.5% bovine serum albumin and 0.1% sodium azide for 10 min at room temperature. This was performed to absorb nonspecific proteins. Subsequently, the sections were allowed to react with the anti-Timp-3 rabbit polyclonal antibody (ProteinTech, Chicago, IL, USA) (500 × dilution) overnight at 4 °C. Next, the sections were washed thrice for 3 min with 0.05 M phosphate buffer (pH 7.6), and then allowed to react for 30 min with the secondary antibody conjugated with peroxidase. Thereafter, the sections were again washed with 0.05 M phosphate buffer. After a 5-min chromogenic reaction with 3,3′-Diaminobenzidine HCl, the sections were counterstained with Mayer's hematoxylin for 5 min. The sections were then dehydrated and encapsulated for observation under an optical microscope.

In Situ Zymography

Gelatinolytic activity in mouse brain sections was determined by in situ zymography with DQ-gelatin-FITC (Molecular Probes, Eugene, OR, USA) as described previously.27, 28 Briefly, unfixed whole mouse brain was embedded in OCT compound. Sections (of 10-μm thickness) were cut and air dried for 1 h, re-hydrated in PBS, and incubated at 37 °C in DQ-gelatin-FITC solution (100 μg/ml in PBS) for 1 h. Sections were then washed thrice in PBS, fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde, and examined under a fluorescent microscope to detect green fluorescence due to gelatinolytic activity.

Statistical Analysis

Results are expressed as mean±s.e. Comparison among groups was performed by one-way ANOVA; Student's t-test was used when appropriate. A value of P<0.05 was considered significant.

RESULTS

Passive Avoidance Test

WT and KO mice first underwent preconditioning. When a mouse moved from the lighted chamber into the dark chamber, it received an electric shock. Twenty-four hours after preconditioning, one retention trial was performed. In the retention trial, the latency time in the lighted chamber was measured and compared between the two mice groups. As a result of receiving an electric shock in the dark chamber, the mean latency time in the retention trial was prolonged in both WT and KO mice, indicating that both types of mice acquired avoidance memory. Moreover, no significant difference was observed in latency times for WT and KO mice, either before (54.4±19.5 s for WT, 27.7±7.5 s for KO; P=0.20) or after (300±0.0 s for WT, 276±18.7 s for KO; P=0.23) conditioning. These results show that there were no memory deficits in KO mice compared with WT mice.

Active Avoidance Test

WT and KO mice were placed in the avoidance-learning box with two compartments connected. A beep was presented, and 5 s later, an electronic shock was given through the grid floor. When this process was repeated, the mice learned to escape the shock by migrating into the adjoining compartments at the sound of the beep. A migration reaction within 5 s from the beep was defined as the ‘avoidance’ reaction. The number of avoidance reactions was counted over 3 days, with 50 trials per day. Moreover, the number of migrations between trial intervals in the absence of stimuli was counted to compare general activity levels.

We found no differences in activity levels between WT and KO mice over the 3 days of testing (F(1,22)=1.88, P=0.183). Successful avoidance increased in both WT and KO mice as a function of training days, indicating that both types of mice acquired avoidance memory. Furthermore, there was no difference between the number of avoidance reactions in WT and KO mice during the 3 days of testing (F(1,22)=0.46, P=0.505). These results indicate that no memory deficits were detected in KO mice.

Water Maze Test with Invisible Platform

The water maze test with the invisible platform was conducted to evaluate spatial memory in WT and KO mice. Each mouse underwent 5 access tests daily for 5 consecutive days. Escape latency period was defined as the time (measured for a maximum of 60 s) it took for each mouse to reach the platform submerged in the pool of opaque water.

On the first day, no significant difference was observed between WT and KO mice. However, on the second day, memory acquisition in KO mice was significantly reduced compared with WT mice. Although a significant difference in memory acquisition was also observed on the third and fourth days, the difference gradually decreased (Figure 1a), and the difference in memory acquisition was no longer significant on day 5.

Figure 1
figure 1

Water maze tests. (a) Invisible platform test of wild-type (WT; n=12) and Timp-3 knockout (KO; n=12) mice. A clear, round platform was submerged 0.5 cm beneath the surface of milk-colored water in a pool. Escape latency period is the time (measured for a maximum of 60 s) it took for each mouse entering the pool to reach the submerged, invisible platform. Training trials were performed for 5 consecutive days, with 5 trial sessions per day. Data are expressed as mean±s.e. *P<0.05. (b) Visible platform test of WT (n=12) and Timp-3 KO (n=12) mice. The platform was placed 0.5 cm above the surface of water in a pool. As a visual cue, a stick with a black cube on top was placed on the platform. The location of the platform was fixed from day 1 through day 5, and varied from day 7 through day 11. Escape latency period (measured for a maximum of 60 s) is the time it took for each mouse, after entering the pool, to reach the visible platform. Five trial sessions were performed per day. Data are expressed as mean±s.e.

To measure the time spent in the quadrant in which the platform had been previously placed, a 1-min probe test was conducted the day after (ie, on day 6) completion of the access tests. The time spent in the target quadrant was longer than that achieved merely by chance, indicating that both types of mice acquired memory of the previous location of the platform. Moreover, no significant difference was observed in the time that WT or KO mice spent in the target quadrant (19.6±1.71 s for WT; 20.9±1.27 s for KO; P=0.561).

Taken together, these results indicate that although KO mice eventually learned the location of the platform, their speed in acquiring memory was significantly slower than that of WT mice.

Water Maze Test with Visible Platform

In humans, a Timp-3 mutation causes Sorsby's fundus dystrophy (SFD), a disease characterized by the loss of central vision during the fourth or fifth decade of life. It is likely that the KO mice used in this study were able to recognize visual cues, given that they eventually learned the location of the platform. This conclusion is supported by a previous study showing that learning in water maze tests relies on the use of visual cues.29 To further investigate whether KO mice were able to use visual cues during a water maze test, a visible platform test was conducted.

The mean escape latency period gradually decreased from day 1 to day 5 in both WT and KO mice, indicating that both types of mice acquired memory of the location of the platform (Figure 1b). Mean escape latency times for WT and KO mice were similar during the 5 days of testing. This finding indicates that both types of mice approached the platform aided by the visible cue, rather than by spatial memory. Moreover, the results suggest that the swimming ability of KO mice was not compromised. Moreover, latency times of WT and KO mice were not different, even when the location of the visible platform was varied (on days 7 through 11). The fact that latency time at day 7, the first day in the varied visible platform test, was not longer than that at day 5, the last day in the fixed visible platform test, also indicates that both types of mice approached the platform aided by the visual cue rather than by spatial memory.

Probe tests conducted on day 6 (13.0±1.40 s for WT; 15.4±0.97 s for KO; P=0.162) and day 12 (12.5±1.18 s for WT; 12.9±0.98 s for KO; P=0.821) also showed no difference in latency times between WT and KO mice. Furthermore, latency times observed for both types of mice were not longer than the latency times expected if achieved merely by chance. These results also indicate that movement of mice in the visible platform test was dependent on the use of visual cues, rather than on memory of the location of the platform.

Taken together, our findings suggest that KO mice were able to recognize visual cues in the water maze test. An alternative interpretation is that visual failure in KO mice was minimal and did not substantially compromise the recognition of visual cues.

Open-Field Habituation Test

To evaluate the motion and mobility of the mice, the numbers of locomotion and rearing were scored for 10 min per day for 3 days with sensors placed on the lateral side of the chamber. There was no significant difference between WT and KO mice in the numbers of locomotion (107.8±5.76 in WT, 116.9±10.76 in KO; P=0.490) and rearing (99.2±6.95 in WT, 91.6±9.19 in KO; P=0.535) on day 1; however, significant differences were observed on day 3 (Figure 2a and b). Both locomotion and rearing decreased over time in WT mice, but did not substantially change over time in KO mice. This indicated that WT, but not KO, mice became accustomed to the environment.

Figure 2
figure 2

Open-field habituation test. (a) The graph shows the change in locomotion score for WT (n=12) and Timp-3 KO (n=12) mice over time (days). The locomotion score, which represents the number of migrations of each mouse in a period of 10 min, is expressed as a percentage. Data are expressed as mean±s.e. *P<0.05. (b) The graph shows changes in rearing score in WT (n=12) and Timp-3 KO (n=12) mice over time (days). The rearing score, which represents the number of times that each mouse rose (ie, exhibited rearing behavior) in a period of 10 min, is expressed as a percentage. Data are expressed as mean±s.e. *P<0.05.

Timp-3 Expression in the Brain

RT-PCR showed the expression of Timp-3 in the hippocampus of WT mice (Figure 3a). In contrast, no Timp-3 was detected in the hippocampus of KO mice. Timp-3 expression was confirmed by immunostaining brain slices. Timp-3 was expressed in the choroid plexus (not shown) and in the hippocampus (Figure 3b). This was consistent with the results from the water maze test, because the hippocampus is considered to be directly involved in memory process.

Figure 3
figure 3

Expression of Timp-3 in the hippocampus. (a) RT-PCR of the Timp-3 gene. RT-PCR was performed with RNA isolated from the hippocampus of wild-type (WT) and three different Timp-3 knockout (KO) mice. Amplification of the glyceraldehyde-3-phosphate dehydrogenase gene (GAPDH) served as a control. (b) Immunostaining of Timp-3. A Timp-3 antibody was hybridized to mouse brain sections and visualized with 3-3′-diaminobenzidine and a hematoxylin counterstain. Timp-3 expression is indicated in the hippocampus with arrows. The boxed region shown below is magnified × 4 to illustrate Timp-3 staining. DG, dentate gyrus.

In Situ Zymography

In situ zymography showed that gelatinolytic activity was enhanced in the hippocampus of KO mice compared with that of WT mice. This result was reproducible in four independent experiments. Data from a representative experiment are shown in Figure 4. This result indicated that gelatinolytic enzyme activity was more active in the KO than in WT mice brains.

Figure 4
figure 4

In Situ zymography. In situ zymography shows the gelatinolytic activity (green fluorescence) of enzymes in the brains of wild-type (WT) and Timp-3 knockout (KO) mice. Representative photographs are shown. DG, dentate gyrus.

DISCUSSION

The ECM in a normal, healthy tissue is maintained by a balance of synthesis and degradation, and has a significant role in maintaining tissue homeostasis. In the central nervous system, the balance between Timp and MMP is believed to be involved in synaptic plasticity, particularly in the mechanisms underlying memory. Previous reports have shown that ECM molecules activated signal transduction pathways through diverse cell surface receptors.1 For example, integrins, the primary laminin receptors, are expressed in the adult hippocampus30 and are involved in the stabilization of LTP.31 In this paper, we have shown that Timp-3 KO mice showed increased MMP activity in the hippocampus and impaired cognitive function compared with WT mice. Similarly, Timp-1-deficient mice also showed learning and memory disturbances.23, 24 This result could also be explained by the fact that Timp-1 is expressed in the hippocampus, and synaptic plasticity is influenced by an increase in MMP activity. Timp-2 is also suggested to be involved in synaptic plasticity underlying learning and memory.25

We conducted behavioral tests and brain tissue analyses in Timp-3 KO mice to clarify the involvement of Timp-3 in cognitive function. Three behavioral tests were used to evaluate memory function, and an open-field test was used to evaluate habituation. We detected a decline in memory function in KO mice in the water maze test with the invisible platform, but not in passive or active avoidance tests.

Immunostaining showed the presence of Timp-3 in the hippocampus, which is considered to be the main brain region involved in memory. Moreover, in situ zymography showed that the hippocampi of Timp-3 KO mice had more gelatinolytic activity than did WT mice, indicating that MMP activity was deregulated in Timp-3 KO mice. Whether hippocampal deregulation of enzymatic activity in Timp-3 KO mice is directly or indirectly involved in delayed acquisition of memory in the water maze test remains to be elucidated. Moreover, it is not known whether other regions of the brain contribute to the abnormality observed in Timp-3 KO mice. Consistent with a previous report, Timp-3 expression was also detected in the choroid plexus.32

During central nervous system development, Timp-3 is expressed in the embryonic ventricular zone and postnatal subventricular zone, where neurogenesis occurs.33 In addition, Timp-3 is expressed in the rostral migratory stream; a sub-population of cells in the subventricular zone migrates along the rostral migratory stream to the olfactory bulb, where cells differentiate into neurons. It is possible that lack of Timp-3 expression during brain development might have long-term effects on cognition, given that impaired cognitive function was observed in adult Timp-3 KO mice in this study.

In the water maze test using a visible platform, Timp-3 KO mice were able to use visual cues to reach the platform. However, in humans mutations in the Timp-3 gene result in SFD, a disease characterized by the loss of central vision during the fourth or fifth decade of life. A possible explanation for the discrepancy is that the Timp-3 KO mice we used were relatively young (3 months old). Alternatively, the difference in the nature of genetic alterations in SFD patients versus Timp-3 KO mice is another possibility. Most mutations observed in the Timp-3 gene in SFD patients involve either the introduction of a new cysteine residue in the C-terminal domain or the presence of an odd number of cysteine residues because of the introduction of a stop codon.34 Some of these SFD mutations may result in production of higher molecular-weight protein complexes, possibly dimers.35 Dimerized Timp-3 protein has an active role in the SFD disease process by accumulating in the eyes.35 In a study of eye tissues obtained from SFD patients, the thickened Bruch's membrane was strongly Timp-3 positive, except for sites where the retinal pigment epithelial cells, which normally produce Timp-3, had degenerated.36 In contrast, Timp-3 KO mice do not express Timp-3 transcripts, do not synthesize Timp-3 protein, and do not accumulate Timp-3 protein in the eye tissue. On the basis of these observations, we do not find it too surprising that Timp-3 KO mice were able to recognize visual cues.

The results of the open-field test indicated that habituation was functional in WT mice, but seemed to be lacking in KO mice. This may be because KO mice were unable to remember the environment over time, and thus could not habituate during the test period. It is also possible that the KO mice had a neurological disorder that will impair both memory and habituation.

Timp-3 has been shown to have various functions in previous reports that described other phenotypes of Timp-3 KO mice. For example, at the age of 21 months, Timp-3 KO mice exhibited left ventricular enlargement similar to that observed in dilated cardiomyopathy, cardiac muscle cell hypertrophy, and contractile dysfunction.37 Others reported that Timp-3 KO mice exhibited a spontaneous air space enlargement in the lung, and enhanced collagen degradation in the peribronchiolar space.38 In addition, Timp-3 was reportedly involved in apoptosis, cell proliferation, inhibition of cell proliferation, and angiogenesis. However, this study showed for the first time that Timp-3 deficiency impaired cognitive dysfunction, potentially through the deregulation of ECM homeostasis within the brain.