Introduction

When celebrities reveal health narratives, their stories have the potential to stimulate public interest and awareness of illness or medical procedures, inspire others to face similar medical issues, and promote public health policy.1,2 Media coverage of celebrity cancer experiences has been shown to impact health service utilization and adherence to preventive health guidelines.1,3,4,5 The influence of celebrity health narratives differs depending on audience characteristics. One study has shown a stronger impact of celebrity health narratives among the less educated and those who share demographic characteristics with the celebrity6; another study has suggested that an emotional involvement with the celebrity may be influential.7 Interestingly, coverage of celebrity health events is not universally associated with improved public health outcomes8,9; sometimes wrong, misleading, or alarming information is communicated.10 Moreover, subgroups can interpret and utilize appropriate health messages in unexpected ways.9

On 14 May 2013, actress, director, and humanitarian Angelina Jolie described in an opinion piece in The New York Times that she had recently undergone a prophylactic double mastectomy. Through genetic testing, Ms Jolie learned that she carried a rare BRCA1 gene mutation and publically disclosed that her doctors estimated her risk of developing breast cancer to be 87%. She went on to say that her breast cancer risk was now reduced to less than 5% by undergoing the breast surgery. In her commentary, Ms Jolie noted that only a fraction of breast cancers result from the inherited gene mutation but concluded that access to gene testing and lifesaving preventive treatment should be a priority for all women. The story was featured in news and entertainment media of all kinds; Ms Jolie’s picture appeared on the cover of People magazine on two consecutive weeks following her revelation (15 May 2013 and 22 May 2013) and TIME magazine (27 May 2013) as well as a host of European and Asian periodicals. In Britain, Jolie’s picture appeared on the front page of every national newspaper immediately following her disclosure.11

Given the intense media attention, this study was designed to examine immediate recall and public reaction to the story. We asked whether the typical American adult recalled the Angelina Jolie story, what elements of the story they retained, and how they understood and perceived what was described by and about this celebrity. We were especially interested in the public’s ability to distinguish the genetic context of Angelina Jolie’s risk of breast cancer from the lower risk that characterizes the vast majority of women who do not carry a BRCA mutation. We also wondered about the extent to which exposure to the story affected respondents’ self-rated breast/ovarian cancer risk in light of their family history. Finally, we were interested in the immediate impact of the story on information seeking and health discussions with family, genetic counselors and other health practitioners, cancer-related anxiety, and initiation of cancer-related preventive behaviors. In order to assess the immediate impact of the story on these questions, we designed and launched a rapid response survey within 3 weeks of Angelina Jolie’s New York Times commentary.

Materials and Methods

The study survey was conducted online within the United States between 3 June 2013 and 5 June 2013 among 2,572 adults (aged 18 years and older) by Harris Interactive on behalf of Johns Hopkins School of Public Health via its Quick Query product. The questions regarding Angelina Jolie and her announcement were included as part of Harris’ routine omnibus survey that includes questions from multiple research initiatives. Omnibus survey respondents are recruited to participate regularly in research. As a collective, the respondents are intended to reflect the national population (considering age, sex, income, race, education, and region). To ensure representativeness, Harris employs a two-stage approach to respondent inclusion in the Omnibus poll. In stage 1, the outgoing sample is balanced demographically (based on the demographic factors including age, sex, income, race, education, and region), and incoming data are monitored against known targets for these variables according to the US Census Bureau’s 2012 Current Population Survey. This ensures that data can be weighted to these targets without extreme weighting. In stage 2, Harris weights the data on demographic variables (age, sex, income, race, education, and region), in addition to a propensity score variable. This process is intended to adjust for attitudinal and behavioral factors that may differ between the sample group participants who participate in online surveys and those who do not. The Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health Institutional Review Board determined that the research protocol for this study was exempt.

To facilitate the survey’s rapid implementation and budget restrictions, we were limited to 12 questions. Although most of our items were based on previous measures, some modification was made for the sake of efficiency and format. For the most part, participants were asked to endorse responses from a list of multiple options, and we used only a few open-ended responses. Readers can see the complete survey in the Supplementary Appendix online.

Besides sociodemographic variables, participants were asked if they had read, heard, or seen any Angelina Jolie stories in the previous 3 weeks. The survey used a skip pattern, so only participants who accurately checked that Ms Jolie “had surgery (double mastectomy) to reduce her risk of breast cancer” were prompted to describe their information sources, attitudes toward the story, and understanding of Jolie’s self-reported lifetime risk of developing breast cancer.

All participants, regardless of whether they were aware of Ms Jolie’s surgery, answered general questions about their family history of breast and ovarian cancer, awareness of the proportion of cancers related to BRCA mutations, and the average woman’s risk of developing breast cancer if she does not have the BRCA mutation. We also asked if respondents had engaged in any health-seeking activities related to breast or ovarian cancer in the past 3 weeks.

Using an approach derived from that used in the Health Information National Trends Survey,12 family history of breast and ovarian cancer was based on the participant’s identification of the number of first (self, mother, father, son, daughter, sister, brother, and father), second (aunt, uncle, nephew, and niece), and third (cousin) degree relatives reported to have breast or ovarian cancer. This way of assessing family history is consistent with models of family risk assessment that focused on personal cancer history and the number of affected paternal and maternal first, second, and third degree of relatives.13,14,15

Using the format suggested by Lipkus,16 female respondents were asked to estimate their own lifetime breast or ovarian cancer risk, and males were asked to estimate a female relative’s lifetime risk of developing these types of cancers, compared with others of their same race and age, indicating if they thought their risk (or a family member’s risk) was higher, the same, or lower. We used a modification of Schwartz et al.’s17 three-item measure of numeracy previously used in a study of screening for mammography.

Approach and Analysis

The statistical analysis for this study used SPSS version 12.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL). The data set provided to us by Harris included propensity score weighting for age, sex, race/ethnicity, education, region, and household in order to bring data in line with respondents’ propensity to be online (based on the US Census Bureau’s 2012 Current Population Survey). In stage 2, Harris uses back-end weighting with the collected data. Harris’ two-stage approach of sampling and weighting was used to ensure the data represented the US national population. Additionally, we used the SPSS option for weighted data in our analyses.

Descriptive statistics for categorical variables were provided as proportions. The χ2 test was used to compare the background characteristics between respondents who were aware and not aware of the story, correctly recalled Jolie’s estimated risk of breast cancer, and answered questions about the BRCA mutation and breast cancer risks. In determining any differential impact of story awareness on health-related behaviors during the 3 weeks following the story, group differences were examined by statistically controlling for numeracy and family history.

Results

Almost three quarters of sampled adults (74%; n = 1,907) correctly indicated that Angelina Jolie had undergone a double mastectomy to reduce her risk of developing breast cancer (question 1 in the Supplementary Appendix online). Significant differences in awareness existed across sociodemographic groups ( Table 1 ) with higher recall of the story among women and whites and lower recall among younger, unmarried, less affluent, less educated, and numerate respondents. There were no significant differences in story awareness evident in regard to geographical region and family history of cancer.

Table 1 Percentages of sociodemographic variables, numeracy, and personal and family risk of cancer predicting awareness, recall, and knowledge (N = 2,572)a

Respondents were asked what Jolie’s risk of developing breast cancer was (item 3 in the Supplementary Appendix online). Among respondents aware of the story, almost half (47%) reported her risk within a reasonable range (80–90%) of Jolie’s disclosed estimated risk of 87%. Correct recall was higher among women, nonminorities, wealthier, and older respondents, including those who had been divorced, separated, or widowed and those who had higher numeracy scores (see Table 1 ).

Among respondents who accurately reported Jolie’s estimated risk, fewer than 10% had the information necessary to interpret Ms Jolie’s risk of developing cancer relative to a woman unaffected by the BRCA gene mutation. To investigate this question, we asked respondents to indicate the proportion of breast cancer cases for which a BRCA gene mutation is responsible (correct response less than 10%) and the average woman’s risk of getting breast cancer over her lifetime if she does not have a BRCA mutation (correct response between 5 and 15%) (items 5 and 6 in the Supplementary Appendix online). Slightly more than one in five respondents (22.6%; n = 206) correctly reported the contribution of BRCA mutations to all breast cancer cases, and 31.2% (n = 284) knew an average woman’s risk; 8.9% (n = 81) correctly answered both questions.

We also explored the extent to which individuals who did not recall Jolie’s risk responded to the two knowledge questions; 19.9% (n = 198) correctly answered the BRCA mutation question; 31.6% (n = 315) correctly answered the average risk question; and 5.8% (n = 58) correctly responded to both questions, and this number was significantly lower than the group with correct recall of Jolie’s risk (P < 0.02).

For the overall survey sample, 7.9% of respondents correctly answered both questions ( Table 1 , column 4). Individuals were less likely to correctly respond to the BRCA mutation question than the question about average risk, 23 (n = 583) compared with 31% (n = 794), respectively. Interestingly, men were significantly more accurate in their knowledge of this information than women, as were better educated and more numerate respondents, as well as those from the Western and Midwest geographic regions. Lower knowledge rates were reported by the individuals who had been divorced, separated, or widowed. Notably, family history of breast and ovarian cancer was not related to more accurate knowledge of cancer risks ( Table 1 ).

We further explored the relationship between family history and an individual’s rating of one’s own or family member’s risk of dying from breast or ovarian cancer compared with the average population risk for others of the same race and age, stratified by awareness of the Jolie story. We found that individuals aware of the story and who reported at least one affected first-degree relative, were less likely to rate their personal risk as higher than the population average relative to those who had similar family health histories and did not know the story (39 vs. 59%; P < 0.05). Perhaps even more striking and worrisome is that about half of all individuals aware of the story and without a family history of cancer rated their cancer risk as lower than the population average relative to those without a family history and unaware of the story (47 vs. 52%, but the difference was not statistically significant). There were no sex differences in these associations.

Although the story was widely reported, only 3.4% of the respondents who were aware of the story indicated that they had read Ms Jolie’s commentary originally in The New York Times. Most respondents became aware of the story though a national or local television news story (61.2%) or entertainment piece (21.5%). A quarter (25.7%) of respondents reported exposure to the story from two or more information sources.

We assessed attitudes toward the story by asking respondents what they thought and felt after hearing about Jolie’s story by indicating with which of eight close-ended statements about the story they agreed (item 4 in the Supplementary Appendix online). Almost three-fourths (72%) of both men and women felt Ms Jolie did the right thing by publically announcing her medical situation. However, more women than men felt she did the right thing in having the surgery (73.5 vs. 65.7%, respectively; P < 0.002). More than half of the women (57.4%) said that they would have undergone the surgery themselves if they carried the “faulty” gene. Half of the women (49.9%) said they would recommend the surgery to a family member under those circumstances; in contrast, only 40.0% of men would make that recommendation to a family member carrying the gene (P < 0.0001). Both males and females reported considering or encouraging a family member to get genetic testing to test for the faulty breast cancer gene at about the same rate (8.9% for women and 9.1% for males).

Some respondents (7.8% for women and 8.9% for males) indicated that the story had resulted in heightened anxiety about their own or a family member’s risk of developing cancer, whereas ~5% (4.6% for women and 4.7% for males) said that the story made them less anxious about cancer risks; there are no sex differences in these items. More women (9%) than men (4.9%) (P < 0.004) indicated that the story made them feel more motivated to do something about their health such as seeing a doctor, having a mammogram, or seeing a genetic counselor.

All respondents were asked if they took any health-related action over the past 3 weeks (the time span which included Ms Jolie’s public disclosure). Although the majority of respondents (80%) did not endorse any of the options listed (see item 9 in the Supplementary Appendix online), a subset appeared to have initiated story-related behaviors of some kind. For instance, 6.6% had a mammogram and/or encouraged a family member to have a mammogram, 6.4% indicated that they had learned more about breast or ovarian cancer from talk shows on television or radio, etc., 6.4% reported seeking information about insurance coverage for genetic testing, and 5.8% indicated that they talked with relatives about their family history of cancer. Very few participants indicated that they looked for information about genetic tests for breast or ovarian cancer through the Internet (2.7%), spoke with a doctor (2.0%) or a genetic counselor (1.3%), and sought information about insurance coverage and costs for preventive surgery (1.5%) or about their cancer risk.

There was a positive association between respondent endorsement of positive attitudes toward the Jolie’s story and an indication of positive health actions in the past 3 weeks (Pearson correlation of 0.25; P < 0.0001); nevertheless, endorsement of the behaviors did not differ between respondents who were aware or not aware of the story, after controlling for demographic variables, numeracy, and personal and family history.

Discussion

Based on our population sample, we estimate that three of every four American adults were aware of Angelina Jolie’s prophylactic mastectomy, and almost half could recall Jolie’s estimated risk of breast cancer before surgery. Nevertheless, fewer than 10% of all respondents had sufficient information necessary to interpret the information about Ms Jolie’s risk of developing breast cancer relative to women unaffected by the BRCA gene mutation.

Awareness of the Jolie story was not associated with greater knowledge of breast cancer risk, with or without consideration of the BRCA gene mutation. In fact, among those who followed the story, women were less accurate than men, almost always overestimating the contribution of the BRCA mutation to breast cancer and the average women’s lifetime risk of breast cancer.

We investigated whether awareness of Jolie’s story was associated with respondents ability to more accurately relate their own family history to their own (or family member’s) cancer risk and found that there were substantial errors evident; about half thought that lack of family history was associated with lower than average risk of cancer while it also appeared that individuals aware of the story, and with at least one cancer-affected first-degree relative, were less likely to rate their own (or family member’s) cancer risk higher than average. These findings suggest that respondents largely interpreted a negative family history as protective against cancer and that exposure to the story appeared to confuse, rather than clarify, an understanding of the relationship between a positive family history and increased cancer risk.

It is noteworthy that more than 8% of the population reported some increased anxiety in the weeks following the story. Both men and women overwhelmingly approved of Angelina going public with her story; however, more women than men approved of her decision to have prophylactic surgery or said that they would recommend the surgery to a family member if they carried the mutation. Indeed, 57% of women said that they would have undergone the surgery themselves if they carried the “faulty” gene.

As a point of perspective, a 2008 survey of US women with a BRCA mutation found that 36% had opted for the prophylactic bilateral mastectomy, the highest uptake rate for this surgery in the world.18

Men have a role in this story. They have and help frame opinions and provide support to women about medical decisions, as the popular media suggested about Jolie’s partner Brad Pitt. Clinicians’ attitudes and advice toward prophylactic mastectomy may also vary by gender; a recent European study found that male general practitioners were less positive than female practitioners toward recommending prophylactic mastectomy for women who carry a BRCA mutation.19

While 9% of women reported feeling motivated to do something they were putting off as a result of the story, like having a mammogram or seeing a doctor or a genetic counselor, there was little evidence that they acted on these intentions, at least not in the 3 weeks immediately following the story. This is consistent with outcomes of a previous mass media campaign by Myriad Genetics to promote BRCA1/2 testing, which resulted in a short-term increase in inquiries, but little evidence of population-level changes in discussions with health-care providers or testing increases.20,21

Awareness of the Jolie story was notably much higher than reported awareness of the previously mentioned Myriad campaign (74 vs. 36%).20 Celebrities can successfully raise awareness about a health issue, but it is a greater challenge for health journalists to ensure accurate understanding.22 This is difficult when the issue involves an understanding of risk and probabilities as is true of the genetic information that Angelina Jolie communicated. In this regard, approximately one-third of our respondents (31.7%) incorrectly predicted how many times a coin would come up heads in 1,000 coin flips. This is indicative of quantitative skill deficits found for almost half of the Americans who are characterized as functioning at below basic (22%) or basic (33%) quantitative skill levels.23 The 2002 mass media campaign about BRCA1/2 testing resulted in increased awareness but not knowledge and was accompanied by a greater proportion of testing inquiries by women who were not at high risk for a BRCA1/2 mutation.21,24 It is not surprising that while Angelina Jolie’s health story heightened awareness of breast cancer, it did not help the public (even the subset with greater risk due to family history) to better understand the risks of the disease for the vast majority of women who do not have a BRCA mutation.

A strength of this study is the immediacy and reach of the data collection. The online survey was fielded over 2 days within 3 weeks of the media story about Ms Jolie; it included 2,572 adults (aged 18 years and older) and was conducted by Harris Interactive. In their approach, Harris ensures a representative sample that also includes a propensity score for using the Internet. But, propensity scores do not account for all the variation from this sample to the overall United States. For example, 17.2% of our respondents reported either a personal history of breast or ovarian cancer or a similar history in a first-degree relative; we would expect that figure to be closer to 10% in a national sample, indicating that respondents who chose to do this survey have a greater personal or close family history of relevant cancers compared to the overall population.25,26

To facilitate rapid implementation of the survey, we were restricted in the number and format of the questions asked, and the survey was largely restricted to multiple choice options. As a result, nuanced aspects of this story are lost.

We also made a choice regarding the range of cancer risks considered to be “accurately” reported. Hereditary cancer risks are generally presented as ranges, to represent the existing uncertainty in the scientific literature. Risks for breast cancer for women with a mutation in the BRCA1 gene have been reported to range from 44 to 87% by the age of 70 years.27,28 While we do not know the exact risk figures reported to Ms Jolie by her genetic counselor, we do know that she chose to report an 87% risk in her commentary. As this work was on awareness and recall of celebrity revelations, we focused on a range (80–90%) close to the specific risk provided in Ms Jolie’s New York Times piece.

One problem with the survey is that it failed to allow non–whole-number responses. The correct answer to one of the three numeracy questions used in our measure was a decimal (0.1%).17 As some respondents may have known that 0 was incorrect, they may have entered 1 as the smallest number that was acceptable. On the other hand, respondents may have thought the answer was 1%. As we cannot tell the right answers from the wrong on this item, we decided to drop that question and use the other two questions as our numeracy indicator. We think the measure was nonetheless useful as it provided a somewhat more sensitive measure of population function relative to numeric concepts than educational level.

As our understanding of the genetic contribution to disease risk continues to unfold, it will be vital for health journalists to seek out clinical and scientific experts who can communicate population risks in ways that inform, rather than alarm. It is recommended that clinicians seek ways to reach the most vulnerable and high-risk populations with clear and useful information.4,10,22 Preparing for inevitable future events similar to the Angelina Jolie’s announcement involves transforming heightened public awareness into heightened public understanding and appropriate use of medical resources.

Disclosure

The authors declare no conflict of interest.