Introduction

Reciprocal altruism is a form of mutual co-operation1,2, in which one individual helps another and receives assistance itself in return some time later3,4,5. Various studies have evaluated the actions of multiple individuals simultaneously cooperating in behaviours known as mobbing or predator inspection, revealing the ability of animals to follow tit-for-tat strategy6,7,8,9,10 However, the mechanisms motivating individuals to cooperate, for example to repulse a predator, are still far from being well understood4,11. Doubts persist whether non-primate species possess the cognitive abilities to remember the outcome of previous interactions and accordingly to select future cooperators4,12,13. Thus, a number of studies have suggested that animals’ cooperative interactions are motivated only by short-term rewards based on byproduct mutualism or by the need to retain a valuable potential partner14. Cognitive skills may also constrain the ability of animals to establish and maintain reciprocity, which could explain why finding evidence for contingent reciprocity in nonhuman animals has been difficult15,16. However, some recent evidence suggests that animals are indeed capable of acting according to their future, rather than current, needs17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25.

Reciprocity denotes a behaviour whereby an organism acts in a manner such that it risks reducing or even losing its fitness while increasing the fitness of another organism, because this other organism is likely to act similarly at a later time2,17,26. The original analysis of the conditions where reciprocity could evolve suggested that organisms should follow simple strategies such as ‘tit-for-tat’, i.e. an initial bias towards cooperation, followed by each individual copying its counterpart’s moves2,26. Previous experiments with pied flycatchers showed clear response patterns to assist "co-operating" in preference over "defecting" conspecific neighbours based on a prior event19,20,27, but there arose questions about byproduct mutualism and dominance effects because of the moderately close proximity of the nestboxes (48–54 m apart)18,24,28.

We carried out a field experiment to test whether breeding pied flycatchers can act purely reciprocally when mobbing in response to a predator. We tested this possibility against the hypothesis that the behaviour might be explicable in terms of byproduct mutualism, whereby the activities of an organism provide benefits predominantly for itself and only incidentally to others. We compared the mobbing responses of pied flycatchers for two proximity groups: those breeding either closely or distantly from one another.

Results

In the control subgroups, when an owl was presented at nestboxes #1 of the ‘close’ pairs and the ‘distant’ pairs, adult flycatchers of both subgroups mobbed the predator (Fig. 1). This happened in all 12 cases in ‘close’ pairs control birds and in all 12 cases in ‘distant’ pairs control birds (Fisher’s exact test, P = 1.00). When the owl was presented 1 hr later at nestbox #2, all of the previously assisted pairs of flycatchers from nestboxes #1 for both ‘close’ and ‘distant’ groups reciprocated the assistance and arrived to mob the predator at the boxes of their cooperating neighbours (Fisher’s exact test, P = 1.00).

Figure 1
figure 1

Number of pairs of pied flycatchers in ‘close’ and ‘distant’ groups that assisted co-operators, assisted defectors and remained at their own nest.

In the experimental subgroups, nestbox #1 birds in phase one had to mob the owl on their own and were made aware of the "decision" of nestbox #2 birds to defect. When the owl was presented 1 hr later at nestbox #2, all 12 defectors from nestbox #2 were assisted by their ‘close’ neighbours, while only 2 out of 14 defectors were assisted by their ‘distant’ neighbours (Fig. 1). The other 12 birds from the ‘distant’ experimental subgroup pairs were seen to interrupt their feeding and were heard giving alarm calls. These behavioural responses suggest that the uncooperative neighbours of the defectors were indeed aware of the situation at their ‘distant’ neighbour’s nestbox. The incidence of reciprocity in the experimental subgroup of ‘close’ neighbours differed significantly from that of the ‘distant’ group (Fisher’s exact test, P = 0.0001).

Discussion

These results suggest that joining in the mobbing of predators at the nests of near-neighbours might contribute to the protection of one’s own nest and thus be explicable in terms of byproduct mutualism. It has been shown that a predator tends to leave an area sooner, the more intensely it is harassed29,30,31,32. It has also been shown that predators avoid visiting areas where they have been previously harassed29. Thus, prey individuals may profit from a joint defence against predators because their own fitness may be enhanced as a result of the generally reduced probability of predation.

Our results demonstrate that breeding pied flycatchers transition to cooperation behaviours which resemble reciprocity as the distance to a neighbour increases. For distant neighbours, they significantly punished defectors and reliably assisted cooperators. This finding suggests that they are aware of their spatial context, are aware of current and fully capable of remembering previous social interactions with neighbours, change their responses as a result of previous experiences and choose a situation-specific strategic course of action which might promote their own (self-centered) longer-term security (fitness). This is a demonstration of tit-for-tat strategy, where only co-operating individuals will be supported by their neighbours in the next move. The choice of mobbing or defecting in the distant-neighbour group appears to be an instance of a prisoner’s dilemma. Since harassment of a predator may entail a risk of injury or even death33,34,35 defection against a non-cooperating individual, rather than co-operation, may be the best option. To mob or not to mob – that is the question and the answer for a pied flycatcher appears to be a reasonable pattern of responses which incorporates spatial, behavioural, historical, real-time and future factors.

Methods

Study site and birds

The study was conducted in May and June 2010 and 2011 near Krāslava, southeastern Latvia (55°90′N, 27°19′E). We placed nestboxes in pairs in pine forests with sparse understory, with pairs separated by at least 650 m. ‘Close’ nestboxes were located 20–24 m from one another (mean ± SD = 22 ± 0.96 m; 24 pairs of nestboxes) and ‘distant’ nestboxes 69–84 m from one another (mean ± SD = 80.02 ± 3.50 m; 26 pairs of nestboxes). ‘Close’ and ‘distant’ nestbox pairs were further assigned to either an experimental or a control subgroup. In the ‘close’ group, 12 pairs of nestboxes were assigned to the experimental and 12 to the control subgroup. In the ‘distant’ group the corresponding assignment was 14 pairs to the experimental and 12 pairs to the control subgroup.

Since parents should take higher risks while defending larger and older broods36,37, we included in the experiment only nests with ≥ 10-day old nestlings and with 6–7 chicks. The age of nestlings did not differ between the ‘close’ and ‘distant’ neighbour groups (2-tailed paired t-test, t = −0.73, df = 49, P = 0.36).

All adult flycatchers were marked with light colours of non-waterproof ink 2–4 days before the trials. The birds marked themselves by touching a piece of ink-saturated foam-rubber at the entrance of their nestboxes. We captured birds by temporarily modifying the nestbox entrance hole so that birds could enter, but not leave. Calls played back to ensure recognition of defection were recorded with a PCM-D50 Solid State Recorder connected to a parabolic microphone.

We manipulated the owl stimulus and observed and evaluated flycatcher behaviour from a hide. The owl was mounted on a pole on a small platform 1.2 m above ground. It was installed when no pied flycatcher was detected nearby and was positioned 1.0–1.5 m from the nestbox. The owl was looking towards the nest, being placed between two neighbouring nestboxes. Before presenting the owl to birds in the ‘close’ neighbour group, we kept the predator under cover. We uncovered and presented the predator only when the nest owners appeared in the vicinity, to exclude the possibility that neighbouring individuals could discover and start mobbing before mobbing was initiated by nest owners. Directly after each 15-min period of presenting the owl, the stimulus was moved into the hide.

Predator presentation

For phase one in both ‘close’ and ‘distant’ experimental subgroups, during the nestling phase we presented a stuffed tawny owl Strix aluco, a common predator of small birds in Northern Europe, that was mounted on a pole on a small platform 1.2 m above ground, It was installed when no pied flycatcher was detected nearby and was positioned 1.0–1.5 m from the nestbox. The owl was looking towards the nest, being placed between two neighbouring nestboxes19. While presenting the owl to birds in the ‘close’ neighbour group, we initially kept the predator under cover. We uncovered and presented the predator only when the nest owners appeared in the vicinity, to exclude the possibility that neighbour individuals could discover and start mobbing before mobbing was initiated by nest owners. Directly after each 15-min period of presenting the owl, the stimulus was moved into the hide.

In both experimental subgroups, just before the owl was exposed at nestbox #1, we captured both parents breeding in nestbox #2 and kept them in captivity (out of sight of the pair at nestbox #1) for the 15 min of exposure of the owl at nestbox #1. During these 15 min, we played back alarm calls of pair #2 recorded 4–6 days previously. As such, we made it clear to the nestbox #1 pair that the #2 pair was aware of a predator and had chosen to defect. Our previous results revealed that pied flycatchers generally remained near their own nests and made alarm calls when "retaliating" against neighbours which had recently defected from assisting them19. As soon as the predator was removed, we released the ‘defectors’. In the control subgroups, when the owl was presented at nestbox #1, the nestbox #2 birds were left free to assist their neighbours.

For phase two, 1 hr later, in both control and experimental subgroups, we presented the owl for 15 min at nestbox #2 and monitored the behaviour of both the resident (#2) and the neighbour pair (#1). We expected that predators placed near one of the closely located pairs of nestboxes would represent an equal threat to both pairs of birds and that therefore both experimental and control pairs in the ‘close’ groups would join in the mobbing, whether or not their neighbours had assisted them previously. This is because mobbing alongside a defector is in the individual’s interest given the proximity of the predator in the ‘close’ pairs. On the other hand, under the hypothesis of reciprocity, we predicted that flycatchers from pairs of distant nestboxes would join in mobbing only if their neighbours previously had assisted them. Thus, in the ‘distant’ group we predicted mobbing for control pairs, but no mobbing in experimental pairs.

The experiments comply with the current laws of the Republic of Latvia. All animal manipulations were approved by Latvian Council of Science (grant # 09.1186).