Introduction

The study of the medionigra form of the moth Panaxia (Callimorpha) dominula L. in the colony at Cothill, Oxfordshire, England, begun in 1939, has continued to provide controversy in interpretation (Clarke et al., 1993a,b, 1996; Cook, 1993; Jones, 1993a,b; Owen & Clarke, 1993; Owen & Goulson, 1994; Cook & Jones, 1996; Goulson & Owen, 1997; Goulson, 1999; Jones & Cook, 1999). Following Kettlewell (1943/44), Owen & Clarke (1993) predicted that temperature could affect the expression of the medionigra phenotype. Owen & Goulson (1994) and Goulson & Owen (1997) demonstrated an increase in the proportion of individuals they scored as medionigra when late larvae and pupae were raised at constant temperatures.

A reanalysis of the field data by Cook & Jones (1996) was based on the published mean temperatures for June in central England. June was chosen because most of the larvae pupate in that month. No effect of temperature on the frequency of medionigra was detectable. They commented, however, that ‘the temperature actually experienced by the insects may be controlled by their choice of pupation site’.

In a brood containing medionigra Kettlewell (1943/44) observed an increase in the number of black scales in the critical regions of the wings, particularly of the hind wings, when pupae were subjected to a ‘constant’ 21°C throughout their development, whereas pupae from the same brood raised ‘at varying out-of-doors temperature(s)’ showed no such increase. Owen & Goulson (1994) repeated these experiments using constant temperatures of 12°, 18° and 24°C on larvae and pupae from two colonies (Cothill and North Hinksey, Oxfordshire). They used the frequencies of medionigra among wild-caught adults of the same cohort (1993) from the two colonies for comparison. The 12°C temperature was thought to be ‘about the same as the mean air temperature in the wild’ (Owen & Goulson, 1994) and so could be regarded as the baseline for comparisons. Although all three treatments increased the proportion of individuals scored as medionigra over wild controls, 18°C apparently had little effect on insects from Cothill and an intermediate effect on the sample from North Hinksey. This apparent anomaly was not explained.

In a follow-up experiment (Goulson & Owen, 1997), larvae and pupae from another habitat (Axmouth, Devon) were subjected to 18°C and 24°C, but not to 12°C, and compared with the earlier experiments and with moths from a Deal, Kent, population held in the British Museum, Natural History. Again they reported an increase in the frequency of insects they scored as medionigra among the adults.

All previous discussions of temperature in the environment of P. dominula have been confined to air temperatures, often meteorological records from some distance away (Kettlewell, 1943/44; Owen & Clarke, 1993; Owen & Goulson, 1994; Clarke et al., 1996; Cook & Jones, 1996; Goulson & Owen, 1997), but the larvae pupate under the litter at ground level from late May and not in midair. Thus it is necessary to measure the actual temperatures to which late larvae and pupae are subjected in their natural habitat.

I report the results of measuring temperatures in the Cothill habitat. I also update to 1999 the annual fieldwork data from which the population size and the frequency of the medionigra allele can be estimated.

Methods

Cothill Fen (Parsonage Moor) is an alkaline fen fed by a number of small streams. There is much evidence of earlier peat cutting, but the remaining uncut areas are thick with plant debris at various stages of decomposition. Currently the dominant species surrounding the major food plant of P. dominula (Symphytum officinale L.) are Phragmites australis (Cav.) Trin. ex Steudel, Eupatorium cannabinum L. and Filipendula ulmaria (L.) Maxim., the last two also being food plants for the moth. The last three are tall plants in this habitat. It is very unlikely that any direct sunlight reaches the level of the litter in the fen after late May.

Temperatures were measured with Hobo® XT temperature loggers (the square box type, −5.0 to +37°C range, Onset Computer Corporation) attached by short cables to external thermistor probes. The covers of the thermistors happened to be the same length and colour as P. dominula pupae although somewhat narrower. The data loggers were placed in round 235 mL plastic food containers (Rubbermaid®) each with a hole in the side through which the cable was passed. All was sealed with exterior grade window sealant. Each container was protected by a 1-m long, 115-mm diameter drainage pipe pushed vertically into the soil. The thermistor and cable were passed through a small hole made in the drainpipe at ground level, below the loose litter. The thermistors were placed in plant litter under or near to the canopy of Symphytum officinale plants, up to 1 m from the crown of a plant. The larvae scatter from the food plant to pupate in nature (Kettlewell, 1942/43; Sheppard, 1951; D.A.J., pers. obs.), whereas in captivity they often have no choice but to pupate in the food plant (Porter, 1997; D.A.J., pers. obs.).

As a trial, two loggers were placed at different sites within Cothill fen in late July 1995 and the data downloaded into a computer in mid-December 1995. The cables of both probes had been damaged by rodents in November and thus the sequences of data were incomplete for that period.

The loggers were replaced in the field in late December 1995 fitted with new thermistor probes. Fine metal tubing covered the cables. A clear plastic 35 mm photographic film canister (Fuji) was attached by sealant to the end of each tube. Numerous holes were punched through the wall of the canister using a fine soldering iron. Each thermistor probe was adjusted to be approximately 15 mm beyond the end of the tube, hanging free within the canister. The tubes and canisters were covered by moss and litter typical of the area, care being taken to ensure that the thermistors and their cables would not be exposed to direct sunlight at any time of the year.

In the field, full sets of data were downloaded from the two loggers in late June 1996 (see Table 1). The data loggers were reset and returned to the field. At the end of July the loggers were collected and replaced by new ones. During a management activity in the fen, there had been extensive clearance of shrubs and trees in the spring of 1996. As a result a new site (North-east) was chosen for the second data logger (Table 1). Furthermore, the clearance work had reopened parts of the fen to the moth and an additional site for collecting temperature data was later chosen in the North-west of the fen (Table 1).

Table 1 The mean, maximum and minimum temperatures in Cothill Fen 1996–99 in the plant litter and mosses at three sites where Panaxia dominula larvae pupate and two other sites 0.4 m above the litter below which the larvae pupate. Each entry lists the mean (standard error; number of records) minimum/maximum temperature during the month

The calibration of each logger and thermistor probe was subsequently checked in the laboratory by standard methods. It was also necessary to justify that the Fuji film canisters were not distorting the readings. Pairs of Hobos were set up as described above and placed in constant temperature (18°C) and fluctuating temperature (7–22°C) incubators. The probe of one of each pair was exposed, and the other was contained within a Fuji canister. After a few days the canister was transferred to the other probe of each pair. The two sets of readings from each incubator matched almost exactly.

In April 1997 it was possible to begin measuring air temperatures. A Stevenson Screen or similar device could not be used, because security was likely to be a problem. Hobo XT loggers with a temperature recording range of −37 to +46°C were chosen. They were sealed into round plastic food containers as before. The containers were mounted on marine grade plywood boards and protected both by plywood screens and by chicken wire. The thermistors were positioned 0.4 m above ground level in such a way that they would not be exposed to direct sunlight or to rain. Two devices were placed in the fen with regard both to their security and to the appropriateness of the data they would collect, because their data would be the controls for comparison with those obtained from the litter-level Hobos (Table 1).

Sets of data were collected at the end of June and again at the end of July. A cycle of setting up loggers and downloading data was repeated so that relevant data were retrieved in July of 1998 and 1999. The performance of each Hobo and thermistor probe was checked both before being placed in the field and immediately after the data had been downloaded.

The initial processing of the data used the LOGBOOK program that came with the data loggers. The data were transferred for statistical analysis by appropriate programs in Sigma Stat®. Because the temperature data were not normally distributed they were analysed by nonparametric methods.

The moths were sampled in July in 1996 to 1999 (inclusive) using the traditional technique for this work of mark–release–recapture and catch-per-unit-effort originating in Fisher & Ford (1947). The results of subsequent sampling can be found in Sheppard (1951), Sheppard & Cook (1962), Ford & Sheppard (1969), Lees (1970), Ford (1975, 1981) and Jones (1993b).

Results

The monthly maximum, minimum and mean temperatures over the relevant time periods are given in Table 1. Tables 2 and 3 contain the latest mark–release–recapture data presented in the same format as earlier (Jones, 1993b; Cook & Jones, 1996). Figure 1(a,b) shows the patterns of temperature over the critical period from late May to early July 1998 at the South-west site and at one of the air temperature sites. Figure 2(a-d) shows the daily mean temperatures for June in 1996–99.

Table 2 Forewing median spots and hind wing spots in Panaxia dominula at Cothill 1988–99. Columns h and i are classical medionigra, e and f are yellow spot medionigra and are included as medionigra in Table 3. Moths from column g were scored as medionigra. See Jones (1993b) for an explanation for this scoring
Table 3 The numbers of Panaxia dominula captured and recaptured, together with estimates of mean daily survival rate, population size and the frequency of the medionigra allele at Cothill in July 1988–99
Figure 1
figure 1

(a) Pattern of diurnal temperature from 21 May to 10 July 1998 at the South-west site. Temperature was measured under the surface litter in precisely the places where Panaxia dominula larvae pupate. (b) Pattern of diurnal temperature from 21 May through 10 July 1998 at 0.4 m above the litter below which P. dominula larvae pupate.

Figure 2
figure 2

Daily mean temperatures at the South-west site during June: (a) 1996; (b) 1997; (c) 1998; (d) 1999.

Of the original captures of P. dominula in 1996, 1997 and 1998, none could be classified as ‘classical’ medionigra (columns g, h and i in Table 2; see Jones, 1993b for the rationale for this statement), but 10 were scored as ‘yellow spot’ medionigra in 1996 (columns e and f in Table 2), 17 in 1997, and five in 1998. In 1999, however, three individuals were scored as classical medionigra, with a further eight being yellow spot medionigra. Thus, the overall frequencies of medionigra for the years 1996–99 were 3.01, 3.74, 1.21% and 2.63%, respectively (Table 3). These frequencies were lower than for 1993, the year that Owen & Goulson (1994) used as the control for their constant temperature experiments.

Discussion

The temperature measurements

Apart from the damage caused by rodents in November 1995 (the Hobos recorded the date and time!) there has been no evidence of tampering with any of the equipment over a four-year period. At different times, four of the loggers failed to provide full sets of data (hence some of the gaps in Table 1), but this was essentially the result of user error and not a fault of the Hobos themselves.

Unfortunately, there are no data for air temperatures at Cothill during the summer of 1996, but the patterns of the pairs of litter and air temperature data for each of the years 1997, 1998 and 1999 appear to be comparable. For example, the traces for 1998 are shown in Fig. 1(a,b). During a 24-h period we should expect the maximum and minimum temperatures in the air to be more extreme than in the litter layer, essentially because the litter will dampen both heat loss to the air and heat gain from it. The data in Fig. 1(a,b) show that this is generally true for the minimum temperature, but not for the maximum. This suggests that there is either heat retention or heat generation in the litter.

There are two independent pieces of evidence to suggest that the litter layer is being heated from a source other than from the air or direct sunlight. First, in 21 of the 22 possible comparisons (Table 1) the mean temperatures for May, June and July in the litter sites are higher than the corresponding mean air temperatures. The exception is at the South-west site in July 1997. Secondly, when the maximum air temperature on a given day is near the monthly mean the litter temperature is very similar. This is emphasized by the data in Table 4. There were five days in June 1997 and two in 1998 (none in 1999) when the maximum air temperature was lower than the monthly average air temperature. On all seven occasions the corresponding maximum day temperature in the litter was higher than the air temperature. It appears, therefore, that the temperature within the litter is higher than should be expected on a cold day in early summer. Because the vegetation is already sufficiently tall to prevent radiant energy from reaching the litter directly, the only possible source of the extra heat is from the composting of decaying vegetation below the litter. The temperature at the pupation sites may be higher therefore than it would be if there were no decomposing litter.

Table 4 Maximum temperature (°C) at the South-west site for days when the maximum air temperature was less than the monthly mean air temperature

Raised and lowered constant temperature and the medionigra phenotype

Raised temperature

The original objective in Kettlewell (1943/44) was to speed up the life cycle of P. dominula by raising a number of broods at 21°C, a higher temperature than the insects would experience in the wild. This objective was achieved, but he reported accentuated expression of medionigra in the one brood that contained the medionigra allele and darkening of the wings of insects in another brood that did not include the medionigra allele. With the rest of the broods (he did not say how many broods were tested) the only noted effects of temperature on the moths were with the apical spots. These spots are not important in determining the medionigra phenotype. He concluded that the absence of temperatures below 21°C might have been responsible for the darkening effects. Except for July 1998 and May and June 1999, the maximum temperatures at Cothill in May, June and July of 1996–99 were all in excess of 21°C. Thus the larvae and pupae do experience temperatures higher than those used in Kettlewell’s experiments.

Lowered temperature

Owen & Goulson (1994) wrote ‘interestingly, the experiments responded to the 12°C treatment, even though this is about the same as the mean air temperature in the wild.’ Presumably 12°C is what they had regarded as the baseline temperature that would produce the average phenotype. The observations in Table 1 show mean air temperatures in June higher than 12°C by 1.5°C, 1.34°C (mean) and by 1.1°C in 1997/98/99, respectively. Although these are not large temperature differences, the average differences in pupating temperature were 3.7°C, 2.6°C, 2.1°C and 2.0°C, respectively, in the four years 1996–99. To a human with a fever such small temperature changes can make the difference between life and death. Perhaps even small changes in average temperatures above 12°C or below 24°C can reduce the darkening effect. Jones & Cook (1999) have argued, on the contrary, that the wing pattern development of medionigra could be disrupted at either end of the temperature range, but is relatively stable inbetween. They suggested that this is exactly what we should expect following the evolution of recessivity of medionigra.

Constant raised temperatures

Owen & Goulson (1994) suggest that a medionigra phenotype would be induced by exposure either to a high temperature or to a stable temperature regime. It is puzzling that neither Owen & Goulson (1994) nor Goulson & Owen (1997) reported the results of experiments at fluctuating temperatures or commented beyond the quotation above about the role of 12°C in determining the phenotype.

The experiments by Kettlewell and by Owen and Goulson were based on constant temperatures. Stable temperature regimes do not exist in the natural habitat of the moth (Fig. 1 and 2; Table 1), although the composting effect does buffer the temperatures at the low end. Table 1 shows that the temperatures experienced by pupae diurnally at Cothill are sometimes in excess of 24°C. For better comparisons between years, the daily mean temperatures in June at the South-west site are presented, on the same scale, in Fig. 2(a-d)) and the number of days during June in which the temperature exceeded 18, 21 or 24°C is shown in Table 5. Clearly the patterns of temperature vary between years. In 1996 the daily means fluctuated violently and 23 days had temperatures over 21°C. 1997 shows a steady decline during the month, with only one day with a temperature over 21°C and yet that year showed the highest frequency of medionigra of the four years under discussion. Only two days in June 1998 had temperatures over 21°C. There are some days hotter and some colder in 1998 than in either 1997 or 1999, but not as extreme as in 1996. However, 1998 saw the lowest medionigra frequency of all four years. The pattern of the mean temperature in June 1999 is the most stable observed to date. Only two days had temperatures exceeding 18°C. Furthermore, the difference between the maximum and minimum temperatures at any time during the month was less than 11°C, whereas the ranges in 1996–98 were 33.0, 12.2 and 16.8°C, respectively. The closest natural situation to the laboratory experimental conditions occurred therefore in 1999; three individuals scored as classical medionigra were caught, the first since 1995, but the overall frequency of medionigra did not exceed that observed in ‘unfavourable’ years. Thus, the relatively stable temperature regime in June 1999 does not appear to have increased the frequency of medionigra in the natural population.

Table 5 Number of days and hours per day when the temperature at the South-west site exceeded 18, 21 and 24°C, respectively

Although the patterns of temperature in June of the four years were very variable, the patterns of emergence of the adults were not (Table 6). At the beginning of the flying season more males are captured than females. At peak emergence the numbers are equal. This is followed by an increase in the proportion of females in the daily captures. Table 6 shows that the peak emergence was the same date in July in three of the four years and two days earlier in 1999. By comparison, the equivalent dates for earlier years (Table 3) show a range from the 10th to 20th July. In two extreme years the relevant dates were 30th July (1958 and 1972). The numbers of medionigra captured before the peak emergence date in 1996 through 1999 are very similar to numbers after that date. Thus in spite of the great differences in the patterns of temperatures between the four years there appears to have been no effect on the time of emergence of medionigra.

Table 6 Numbers of medionigra scored before and after the date on which the sex ratio changed from excess of males to excess of females

We have to conclude, therefore, that the effect of constant elevated temperatures in the laboratory, although revealing the potential with respect to phenotypic expression, remains a laboratory phenomenon and of little or no relevance to the situation with P. dominula at Cothill.

Inevitably some individual larvae and pupae would be subjected to different temperature regimes than others, even though the litter layer acts as an effective blanket. Clearly, if temperature were an important influence on the phenotypic expression of medionigra at Cothill, we would expect any such effect on the frequency of medionigra be more marked in a year with high June temperatures than in an average year. The insects would mature earlier, there should be an increase in the frequency of medionigra and there should be a bias towards medionigra in the first half of the season. There is no evidence of any such effects in 1996.

The maximum and minimum daily temperatures and the monthly average temperatures have been measured at the time and site of pupation and metamorphosis of P. dominula. These field observations show that the frequency of medionigra and the temperatures measured both at the sites of pupation and in air near the ground in Cothill Fen are unrelated. Thus the laboratory experiments have been misleading and do not help to explain the decline and annual fluctuations in the frequency of medionigra in the Cothill population of P. dominula.