The 50-50-90 rule: Any time you have a 50-50 chance of getting something right, there's a 90% probability you'll get it wrong. 1

Have you thought that after spending considerable effort searching for the best evidence on a specific treatment or diagnosis how much will be lost in translation between yourself and the patient? The words ‘guarded’ or ‘fair’ come up a lot in communication with patients, mostly in relation to diagnosis or treatment outcomes, and I would like to explore this observation by asking; What does ‘guarded’ mean? If we type the word into Google we get ‘adjective: guarded – careful not to give too much information or show how you really feel: a guarded response’.2 I therefore undertook a little study with colleagues and patients in my practice to see how often the word came up, what did the clinicians mean when they used it, and did it match what patients understood. Is there any evidence in the medical/dental research to support my observations? Finally, I would like to discuss how other professions have dealt with these ‘words of estimative probability’ (WEPS) and are there any lessons to be learned.

The Practice-Based Study

To see how often dentists use the word ‘guarded’ I performed a small study with six experienced clinicians writing out hypothetical restorative dental treatment plans without any prompting as to what I was looking for. Eleven clinical cases were presented, and the clinicians were asked to draft out a letter assessing current dental health and future treatment options for each case. The results were collected and produced twenty-six descriptive terms relating to prognosis/risk and outcome. Chart 1. shows a truncated list of the most common WEPS used:

Chart 1
figure 1

Chart 1. Words of Estimative Probability

In these results, ‘guarded’ was the third most common term used (18%), and in more specific conversations with the same team of clinicians we agreed that the key words (excellent (95% +/− 5%), good (75% +/− 15%), fair (50% +/− 10%), guarded (25% +/− 15%) or poor (5% +/−5%)) related to a quite specific scale of success probabilities.

The next question was; ‘What do our patients think some of these WEPS mean?’ To find out I did a short randomised questionnaire duplicating some of the methods used by de Bruin3 asking sixty consecutive general practice patients what they thought an (excellent, good, fair, guarded or poor) chance of treatment success meant. The results are charted in the box-plot below (Chart 2.):

Chart 2
figure 2

Chart 2. Patients unguided perception of success

The chart shows quite clearly that the patients had a higher expectation of a successful outcome than the professionals and in relative terms this disconnect increased almost exponentially as the professional prognosis worsened (Table 1.)

Table 1 Relative difference in patient/professional expectations

Medical/Dental Evidence

What can we conclude from these preliminary results and is there any evidence out there to support this observation? I would like to first focus on the tendency of the patients to overestimate benefits of treatment and consequently underestimate harms. There is very little specific literature, but I did find two qualitative dental studies and one medical systematic review that illustrates this bias:

  • Hof et al.4 found that out of 150 consecutive patients seeking a dental implant, 59% expected it to last a lifetime and 31% at least ten years, and 9% shorter. Patients' estimation of the 10-year implant success rate was 84%.

  • A qualitative study5 on public perceptions of dental implants concluded the participants ‘expected dental implants to restore the patients’ appearance, functions, and quality of life to absolute normality. They regarded dental implants as a panacea for all cases of missing teeth, overestimated their functions and longevity, and underestimated the expertise needed to carry out the clinical procedures.

  • In 2015 a medical systematic review6 was undertaken with the objective to ‘quantitatively assess patients’ expectations of the benefits and/or harms of any treatment, test or screening test'. After reviewing 36 studies, 65% of patients overestimated a treatment benefit and 50% underestimated a possible harm from a treatment.

Evidence from other professions

The Intelligence and National Security community have been working on this precise problem since 19647 in an attempt to improve intelligence briefings to non-specialists following the Bay of Pigs disaster in 1961. Sherman Kent proposed that numerical odds were added to the descriptive words to add clarity between analysts and the decision makers. Even though the logic of his argument was accepted it was never adopted until after the 9/11 terrorist attacks and the Middle Eastern Crisis. The concern was that the numerical probability would be taken as a fact rather than as a probability and the forecaster could be accused of being wrong if the event did not occur.8 This concern is what Philip Tetlock in his book ‘Superforecasting’ calls ‘the wrong-side-of-maybe’ fallacy, if the weather forecast says there is a 60% chance of rain and it does not rain the forecaster is judged as being wrong.9

All the results show professionals may unconsciously or deliberately use words that can be interpreted elastically; ‘guarded’ in our case being interpreted by the patients as meaning anything between 30-100 percent success. These elastic words were called Doublespeak in George Orwell's 1984 where the same word represents two contradictory ideas, or Weasel Words10 where the word appears to say one thing but in reality, it says the opposite or nothing at all.

What if we add a number and a time frame?

Taking into consideration the comments Barnes highlighted with intelligence estimates and the concern over precision I modified the presentation of the estimate by firstly adding a five-star/circle () rating scale. This is similar to the infographic used to create online reputation systems that collect, distribute and aggregate feedback about participants for online retailers.11 A time frame was also added to a survival (time to event) estimate to create a more meaningful prediction, similar to a weather forecast. This type of presentation may not be necessary for simple diagnosis or treatment but becomes more important as cases increase in complexity.

A dental example could be a large periodontally involved five-unit bridge which previously we might describe as having a guarded survival. We could translate this into, ‘the five-year survival is guarded ()’. We repeated the patient-based randomised questionnaire with new patients using the WEP/time/infographic and created a new box-plot (Chart 3.) This now showed an improved separation of the categories and the relative differences are now a lot closer to 1.0, but there were still wide interquartile ranges around the words ‘guarded’ and ‘poor’.

Chart 3
figure 3

Chart 3. Patients guided perception of success

Discussion

Taking the results from my small in-house questionnaire and comparing it with the results from Hoffman's systematic review we can see that optimism bias (individuals see themselves at less risk of an even happening to them than to their peers) is a substantial problem in risk communication with patients.12 This deep-seated overestimation of benefits and consequently underestimation of harm combined with inaccurate online information sources13, 14 is likely to become more problematic for patients, especially as treatments increase in complexity and cost, and patients present with multiple co-morbidities.

Conclusion

Following collecting the best evidence clinicians must try where possible to focus on simple and direct means of communicating treatment benefits and harms to their patients and avoid institutionalised ambiguous language. This will always be confounded by the requirement of our patients needing us to not only ‘provide detailed prognoses, they also want their physicians to give them good news and to be optimistic about their illnesses.15