Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

Ecology

Avoidance of disease by social lobsters

These gregarious animals shun lobsters that carry a lethal virus, even when they still seem to be healthy.

Abstract

Transmissible pathogens are the bane of social animals1, so they have evolved behaviours to decrease the probability of infection2,3. There is no record, however, of social animals avoiding diseased individuals of their own species in the wild. Here we show how healthy, normally gregarious Caribbean spiny lobsters (Panulirus argus) avoid conspecifics that are infected with a lethal virus. Early detection and avoidance of infected, though not yet infectious, individuals by healthy lobsters confers a selective advantage and highlights the importance of host behaviour in disease transmission among natural populations.

Main

Panulirus argus virus 1 (PaV1) is a lethal pathogenic virus that infects juvenile spiny lobsters4. It is transmitted by physical contact and, among the smallest juveniles, through sea water. Spiny lobsters are social and share communal dens, so these modes of viral transmission could have devastating consequences in the absence of a mechanism to check its spread.

During underwater surveys of juvenile lobsters, we observed that infected lobsters rarely shared shelters with conspecifics (less than 7% shared dens and more than 93% were solitary), even though healthy lobsters generally preferred to live together (more than 56% shared dens and less than 44% were solitary).

To test whether this could be explained by healthy lobsters avoiding diseased individuals, we set up a laboratory experiment (for methods, see supplementary information) in which healthy and diseased lobsters were given a choice between an empty den and one containing either a healthy or a diseased individual. We found that the healthy lobsters avoided dens containing diseased conspecifics and preferred to share dens with other healthy lobsters. But diseased lobsters did not discriminate between dens, irrespective of whether the animal inside was sick or well (Fig. 1a). These results confirm our observations in the wild.

Figure 1: Laboratory results showing avoidance by lobsters of diseased individuals and its relation to infectivity.
figure1

a, Den selection by healthy (left panel) and infected (right panel) lobsters, showing the aversion of healthy lobsters for diseased lobsters. All lobsters were either given a choice of sheltering alone or with a healthy conspecific (blue bars), or given a choice of sheltering alone or with an infected (red bars) conspecific that was tethered in the den. b, Increase of infectivity over time for lobsters inoculated with PaV1 virus: green line shows the increasing percentage of healthy lobsters infected by inoculated lobsters, and purple line indicates the increasing percentage of inoculated lobsters that became visibly diseased. c, Cohabitation time series indicating that healthy lobsters started to avoid inoculated (but non-infectious) lobsters four weeks after inoculation and completely avoided them after six weeks, which is before they became infectious.

For an avoidance strategy to reduce the transmission of infection effectively, healthy lobsters must also start to avoid infected individuals before they actually become infectious. We therefore investigated whether there was any link between the timing of infectivity and the avoidance of diseased individuals by healthy lobsters; we found that these were remarkably coincident (Fig. 1b). Lobsters inoculated with PaV1 developed symptoms of disease after six weeks and became infectious after eight weeks (Fig. 1b). Most of the healthy lobsters avoided these inoculated lobsters from four weeks after their inoculation and all of them avoided inoculated individuals at six to eight weeks after inoculation (Fig. 1c).

Other evidence (our unpublished results) indicates that avoidance of diseased conspecifics may act powerfully against the spread of disease in the wild. Laboratory confinement of healthy juvenile lobsters with lobsters infected with PaV1 results in more than 60% of them succumbing within 80 days. However, field surveys conducted since 1999 indicate that the prevalence of PaV1 in Florida's juvenile lobster population remains at less than 7% and does not correlate with lobster density (our unpublished results). The high transmissibility under laboratory conditions and the much lower prevalence in the wild, even at high density, can be reconciled if healthy, normally social lobsters thwart transmission of PaV1 by avoiding diseased lobsters.

Although infection may alter the subtle visual displays used by lobsters to establish aggregations, avoidance of diseased lobsters is likely to be chemically mediated, given that olfaction mediates dominance hierarchies, mate choice, foraging and aggregation5,6. Lobsters do not have an adaptive immune system, so disease may be signified by broad-spectrum defensin compounds or by degradatory products that are associated with poor health.

Pathogens can alter host dynamics and modify communities by disrupting species interactions7,8,9, but their effect on the behaviour of uninfected hosts and on disease dynamics has been largely unexplored. Epidemiological models have not generally included avoidance strategies10,11, despite the possible fitness advantages conferred by such behaviour12,13. Our findings indicate that lobsters can identify infected individuals before they become infectious and that by avoiding them they may limit disease transmission in the wild.

References

  1. 1

    Cote, I. M. & Poulin, R. Behav. Ecol. 6, 159–165 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. 2

    Minchella, D. J. Parasitology 90, 205–216 (1985).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. 3

    Loehle, C. Ecology 76, 326–335 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. 4

    Shields, J. D. & Behringer, D. C. Dis. Aquat. Org. 59, 109–118 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. 5

    Penn, D. & Potts, W. K. Trends Ecol. Evol. 13, 391–396 (1998).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. 6

    Derby, C. D. Physiol. Behav. 69, 203–209 (2000).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. 7

    Bushman, P. J. & Atema, J. J. Chem. Ecol. 26, 883–899 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. 8

    Washburn, J. O. et al. Science 253, 185–188 (1991).

    ADS  CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. 9

    Hudson, P. & Greenman, J. Trends Ecol. Evol. 13, 387–390 (1998).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. 10

    Anderson, R. M. & May, R. M. Population Biology of Infectious Diseases (Springer, Düsseldorf, 1982).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  11. 11

    McCallum, H., Barlow, N. & Hone, J. Trends Ecol. Evol. 16, 295–300 (2001).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. 12

    Nevitt, G., Pentcheff, N. D., Lohman, K. J. & Zimmer, R. K. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 203, 225–231 (2000).

    ADS  Article  Google Scholar 

  13. 13

    Rosenquist, R. & Johansson, P. Anim. Behav. 49, 1039–1045 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mark J. Butler.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information

Supplementary methods

Supplementary methods (DOC 25 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Behringer, D., Butler, M. & Shields, J. Avoidance of disease by social lobsters. Nature 441, 421 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1038/441421a

Download citation

Further reading

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing