Main

Calls of túngara frogs consist of a whine followed by up to seven chucks. The whine is a long-frequency sweep, whereas the chuck is a shorter sound whose fundamental frequency is half that of the whine (for details, see supplementary information). Females prefer a whine–chuck call to a whine alone (probability of 0.856; 3,135 versus 527; exact binomial, P < 0.000001).

In P. pustulosus and some close relatives, the vocal folds support a fibrous mass that extends into the bronchus (see supplementary information). The size of this mass correlates with the presence or absence of chuck-like call components among species within the genus1, within the P. pustulosus species group9 and between populations of the túngara frog's sister species, P. petersi10.

To investigate whether the fibrous mass could be responsible for production of the chuck sound, we recorded calls of male túngara frogs in the field (‘pre-treatment’ condition). We then anaesthetized the frogs, surgically removed the posterior elongation of the fibrous mass, and recorded their calls after they had recovered (‘post-treatment’ condition). A sham control replicated all aspects of the surgery apart from ablation of the mass. An additional control omitted surgical manipulation but replicated the feeding, housing and handling used in the other treatments.

As the fundamental frequency of the chuck is half that of the whine, its odd harmonics do not coincide with the whine's harmonics, although its even harmonics do (see supplementary information). We predicted that ablation of the fibrous mass would reduce the amount of energy in odd relative to even harmonics of the chuck, but would not influence the spectral features of the whine or the increased amplitude at the call's end.

The relative energy in odd versus even harmonics of the chuck was significantly reduced after surgical treatment (F2,17 = 21.77, P < 0.001; Fig. 1a, b). The harmonic-energy ratio was significantly lower than in either of the two controls (Tukey post-hoc test; all comparisons, P < 0.001); the two controls did not differ from one another (Tukey test, P = 0.538). There was no treatment effect in the same analysis of the whine (F2,17 = 0.891, P = 0.429).

Figure 1: Effect of different treatments on call harmonics of the túngara frog, Physalaemus pustulosus.
figure 1

a, Relative amplitude of odd (red bars) to even (blue) harmonics in each surgical-treatment group for ‘chuck’ (left panel) and ‘whine’ (right panel) call components; means and standard errors are shown. Surgery, n = 8; sham, n = 6; none, n = 6. b–d, Representative calls (top, relative amplitude of waveforms; bottom, spectrogram) by an individual male in pre- (left panels) and post-treatment (right panels) conditions after b, ablation of the ‘chuck’-associated fibrous mass; c, sham surgery; and d, no surgery.

Although the chuck is diagnosed by its fundamental frequency relative to that of the whine, it also increases the amplitude of the end of the call. An increase in amplitude is therefore evidence that the male is attempting to produce a chuck. As predicted, there was no treatment effect on the relative amplitude at the end of the call (F2,21 = 0.287, P = 0.754; Fig. 1c, d). Males without the posterior elongation of the fibrous mass therefore still increase call amplitude at the end of the whine, as they would when producing a chuck, but the spectral structure of the chuck is missing. Simply put, males without an elongated fibrous mass on their vocal folds try to produce chuck sounds but are unable to do so.