News & Views | Published:

Human behaviour

Brain trust

Nature volume 435, pages 571572 (02 June 2005) | Download Citation

Subjects

As is the case with other social interactions, financial transactions depend on trust. That fact is behind ingenious experiments that explore the neurobiological underpinnings of human behaviour.

Image: S. CHENN/CORBIS

Michael Kosfeld and his colleagues got students in Zurich to play a serious game. The game involved real monetary exchanges between two people playing the anonymous roles of ‘investor’ and ‘trustee’; beforehand, each subject had received either the neuropeptide oxytocin or an inert placebo, via nasal spray.

As a group, the investors who received oxytocin exhibited more trust in the anonymous trustee than did the investors who received the placebo. Because intranasally administered oxytocin crosses the blood–brain barrier into the central nervous system, Kosfeld et al. (page 673 of this issue)1 conclude that the central action of oxytocin increases trusting behaviour; and because the oxytocin spray did not change the behaviour of the trustees, it seems that oxytocin only increases trust, not the reliability of the trustee. This is a remarkable finding, and to explain its significance we must first say a word about trust and about oxytocin itself.

Given the polarities of reward and punishment that pervade biology at various levels, trust is essential for the normal operation of human societies. Remove trust and you compromise love, friendship, trade and leadership. Little is known about the neurobiology of trust, although the phenomenon is beginning to attract attention2.

As for oxytocin, it is a small peptide, consisting of nine amino acids, that is produced mostly in the hypothalamus, the brain's master controller of biological regulation, including emotion. Oxytocin acts both on certain targets of the body (it is best known for inducing labour and lactation) and on brain regions whose function is associated with emotional and social behaviours (the amygdala and nucleus accumbens, for example) — that is, it works both as a hormone and as a neuromodulator, a kind of neurotransmitter. In animals, oxytocin contributes to social attachments, including male and female bonding after mating, mother and infant bonding after childbirth, and assorted sexual behaviours3,4. Besides triggering complex and specific action-programmes, oxytocin may well work part of its charm by selectively lowering the natural resistance that animals have to the proximity of others, thus facilitating what is known as ‘approach behaviour’.

Given this background, Kosfeld et al.1 hypothesized, reasonably and perceptively, that oxytocin might be involved in trusting behaviour in humans. After all, trust and approach behaviour are indelibly linked. We commonly describe the child who approaches others with ease as ‘trusting’, and we use comparable descriptions for animals in similar situations. Kosfeld and colleagues' finding supports their hypothesis and opens the way to a richer understanding of perhaps the most complex tier of human social interactions. I once likened5 oxytocin to a love potion, the magic elixir that makes Tristan fall for Isolde: add trust to the mix, for there is no love without trust.

Kosfeld et al. provide an engaging discussion of the possible mechanisms behind their finding. They reject the possibility that oxytocin has a nonspecific positive effect on social behaviour, because of its different influence on investors and trustees. Approach and trust possibly dominate the behaviour of investors, and that is where oxytocin works, whereas trustee behaviour is dominated by a principle of reciprocity, for which oxytocin seems irrelevant. Kosfeld et al. also reject the possibility that oxytocin merely reduces the sensitivity to risk, because in a control experiment in which the investors knew the trustee was a computer, they did not take any extra risks. The authors finally settle for an attractive pair of factors: that oxytocin overcomes the aversion to betrayal (which applies only to the investors), and that this is combined with the effects of reward that result from enhanced approach behaviour.

The significance of the study lies in what it can tell us about non-experimental circumstances, when the equivalent of an investor is not sniffing oxytocin. What might be happening then? First, perceiving certain social configurations probably leads to oxytocin release in selected brain regions — that is, the cognitive appraisal of a situation, based on an individual's genetic make-up and past experience, triggers a chain of neural events that includes (but is not limited to) the release of oxytocin. Second, oxytocin modulates the activity of cognitive neural networks, resulting in enhanced trusting behaviour. Whether this result is achieved via a mostly unconscious bias (by altering the competition among ensembles of neurons that represent varied choice options), or a conscious deliberative process, remains to be established — although the evidence seems to favour the former possibility in the current experiment. However, the input, along the cognitive chain, of neural events arising in brain areas associated with social and emotional responses is a requisite part of the explanation. The finding points to the crucial involvement of emotional phenomena in the processes leading from cognition to behaviour.

The authors' results open up possibilities for investigating conditions in which trust is either diminished, as in autism, or augmented. For example, patients with bilateral damage to the amygdala approach strangers with unusual ease, and fail to recognize untrustworthy individuals whom normal people would resolutely avoid6. In this case, damage to the amygdala may prevent the detection of the potential threat evoked by certain stimuli. And children with Williams syndrome, a rare genetic disorder, approach strangers fearlessly and indiscriminately7. Might their high level of trust be due to excessive oxytocin release?

Some may worry about the prospect that political operators will generously spray the crowd with oxytocin at rallies of their candidates. The scenario may be rather too close to reality for comfort, but those with such fears should note that current marketing techniques — for political and other products — may well exert their effects through the natural release of molecules such as oxytocin in response to well-crafted stimuli. Civic alarm at the prospect of such abuses should have started long before this study, and the authors cannot be blamed for raising it. Whatever the beneficial biomedical applications, or the abuses, may turn out to be, Kosfeld et al. have made a valuable contribution to our understanding of the role of neuromodulators in human behaviour that involves choice.

References

  1. 1.

    , , , & Nature 435, 673–676 (2005).

  2. 2.

    et al. Science 308, 78–83 (2005).

  3. 3.

    Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 779–818 (1998).

  4. 4.

    & Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 89, 5981–5985 (1992).

  5. 5.

    Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain (Penguin, New York, 1994).

  6. 6.

    & Nature 393, 470–474 (1998).

  7. 7.

    et al. Am. J. Med. Genet. 124A, 263–273 (2004).

Download references

Author information

Affiliations

  1. Antonio Damasio is in the Department of Neurology, University of Iowa College of Medicine, 200 Hawkins Drive, Iowa City, Iowa 52242, USA.  antonio-damasio@uiowa.edu

    • Antonio Damasio

Authors

  1. Search for Antonio Damasio in:

About this article

Publication history

Published

DOI

https://doi.org/10.1038/435571a

Further reading

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Newsletter Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing