Abstract
Sexual mimicry among animals is widespread1,2, but does it impart a fertilization advantage in the widely accepted ‘sneak–guard’ model3 of sperm competition? Here we describe field results in which a dramatic facultative switch in sexual phenotype by sneaker-male cuttlefish leads to immediate fertilization success, even in the presence of the consort male. These results are surprising, given the high rate at which females reject copulation attempts by males, the strong mate-guarding behaviour of consort males, and the high level of sperm competition in this complex mating system4,5.
Main
The giant Australian cuttlefish, Sepia apama, is a solitary cephalopod mollusc that forms large aggregations to mate and lay eggs4. Females lay one large egg at a time (5–39 per day) and mate up to 17 times with 2–8 males daily. The operational sex ratio averages four males to one female, but ranges up to eleven males to one female. Females reject 70% of mating attempts and the competition between males for mates is intense; mate guarding is almost continuous. Consort males obtain 64% of matings, the remainder being by small, unpaired or extrapair males. Small males (with male coloration and of similar size to females) obtain extrapair copulations by ‘open’ stealth (approaching a guarded female as the consort is repelling other males), by ‘hidden’ stealth (meeting females under rocks) or — the subject of this investigation — by mimicking the appearance and behaviour of females4,6.
Visual deception is achieved when small males suddenly hide their sexually dimorphic fourth arms, acquire the mottled skin patterning typical of females, and shape their arms to mimic the posture of egg-laying females, who are not receptive to mating4 (Fig. 1; for video, see supplementary information). This facultative change in appearance is instantaneous and occurs at a rate of about 10 changes per 15 min during intense behavioural interactions (demonstrated in 12 video subsamples (data not shown); mimic duration, 10–184 s). We found that female mimickers could successfully deceive the consort male and that they were able to position themselves near the female in 30 out of 62 attempts. Other males attempted to mate with the mimics 41 times (25 attempts by large males and 16 by small males, two of which were by other female mimics).
a, An unpaired male (m) assumes female coloration and posture and approaches the paired female (f) while the consort (c) displays to an approaching large male (top right); note the conspicuous, large white arms, a sexually dimorphic male character. b, The female accepts a mating attempt by the female mimic as the consort continues to display to the other large male. c, The consort male allows the mimic to finish mating without interruption, even when he is not distracted (for video, see supplementary information); the other small ‘sneaker’ male has swiftly transformed into a female mimic as well.
We observed five initiations of mating by mimics. One mimic was rejected, one was interrupted by the consort male, and three resulted in successful spermatophore transfer. Consorts did not mate with females immediately after being cuckolded by the mimic. We tested the paternity of the next laid egg by using five microsatellite DNA fingerprints5 on tissue samples from the females and any male that mated with them during the focal samples (up to four in 30 min; females had up to 5 male genotypes in their sperm sources, which included recently attached spermatangia and stored sperm7). Two of the three successful inseminations by mimics resulted in fertilization. Both guarders and sneaks are able to father subsequent eggs5, so the matings by mimics may have fertilized later eggs, but we could not test this possibility because SCUBA-diving constraints curtailed our sampling efforts.
Cuttlefish have keen vision but poor social recognition8, which favours visual sexual mimicry. In contrast to taxa in which alternative mating tactics are either fixed or vary ontogenetically1,9,10, cuttlefish use neural control to change their skin patterning, posture and tactics instantly. To our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of immediate fertilization success in an animal using facultative mimicry as part of a conditional mating strategy11. These field results, combined with those on bluegill sunfish10 (Lepomis macrochirus), which are ‘broadcast’ spawners, lend genetic and behavioural support to Parker's predictions that some sneaks can win sperm competition and steal3 fertilization from guarders.
References
Andersson, M. Sexual Selection (Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, New Jersey, 1994).
Saetre, G. P. & Slagsvold, T. Am. Nat. 147, 981–995 (1996).
Parker, G. A. in Sperm Competition and Sexual Selection (eds Birkhead, T. R. & Møller, A. P.) 3–54 (Academic, London, 1998).
Hall, K. C. & Hanlon, R. T. Mar. Biol. 140, 533–545 (2002).
Naud, M. J., Hanlon, R. T., Hall, K. C., Shaw, P. W. & Havenhand, J. N. Anim. Behav. 67, 1043–1050 (2004).
Norman, M. D., Finn, J. & Tregenza, T. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266, 1347–1349 (1999).
Naud, M. J., Shaw, P. W., Hanlon, R. T. & Havenhand, J. N. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B (in the press).
Boal, J. G. Anim. Behav. 52, 529–537 (1996).
Shuster, S. M. Evolution 43, 1683–1698 (1989).
Fu, P., Neff, B. D. & Gross, M. R. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268, 1105–1112 (2001).
Gross, M. R. Trends Ecol. Evol. 11, 92–98 (1996).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Supplementary information
Supplementary movie
This movie shows a successful extrapair copulation by a small male cuttlefish in the presence of a large consort male that is fighting off another large rival male. The female mimic is in the bottom of the scene as it opens. Another mimic appears about halfway through the short movie. (MOV 1051 kb)
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Hanlon, R., Naud, MJ., Shaw, P. et al. Transient sexual mimicry leads to fertilization. Nature 433, 212 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1038/433212a
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/433212a
This article is cited by
-
Distraction sinking and fossilized coleoid predatory behaviour from the German Early Jurassic
Swiss Journal of Palaeontology (2021)
-
The overlapping reproductive traits of the two male mating types of the oval squid Sepioteuthis lessoniana
Fisheries Science (2019)
-
Cuttlefish perform multiple agonistic displays to communicate a hierarchy of threats
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (2016)
-
Conservation of a phenomenon: rapid, reversible colour change in both sexes of one of the world’s most threatened damselflies
Journal of Insect Conservation (2016)
-
Complex adaptive traits between mating behaviour and post-copulatory sperm behaviour in squids
Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries (2016)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.