Main

The ability to learn language, whether spoken or signed, declines with age3,4,5,6. How the onset and type of the initial language experience contributes to this critical-period phenomenon is unclear. This question cannot be investigated by studying hearing individuals only, because the factors of age and experience are inseparable in these individuals — all hearing babies experience language from birth. But the question can be investigated by studying individuals who were born deaf, because they often do not experience any language until they are enrolled in special programmes7,8. We therefore compared the language-learning capacities of deaf and hearing individuals as a function of early language experience.

We first investigated whether early experience of a spoken language could facilitate subsequent learning of a signed language. We tested two groups of adults who had learned American Sign Language (ASL) at school between the matched ages of 9 and 15 years and who had used it for over two decades. One group (n = 9) was born hearing, had experienced spoken English in early life, and had later learned ASL after becoming profoundly deaf (≥ 90 decibels) as a result of viral infection; the second group (n = 9) was born profoundly deaf and had had little experience of language before being exposed to ASL in school (auditory speech-perception abilities were at chance levels even with hearing aids). Deaf adults who had little experience of language in early life showed low levels of ASL performance; in contrast, late-deafened adults showed high levels of ASL performance (Fig. 1; paired t = 4.17; d.f., 8; P < 0.001).

Figure 1: Effects of early experience on later language learning.
figure 1

a, American Sign Language (ASL) performance of deaf adults who had experienced no language in early life, and of deaf adults who had experienced spoken language in early life. Subjects were tested using a task requiring recall of complex ASL sentences. b, English performance of deaf adults who had had no experience of language in early life, of deaf adults who had experienced ASL in infancy, and of hearing adults who had experienced a spoken language other than English in infancy. Subjects were tested using a task requiring judgements of whether complex English sentences given in print were grammatically correct; chance performance is 50%. Further details are available from the authors.

We next investigated whether early experience of a signed language facilitates subsequent learning of a spoken language. We tested three groups of adults who had learned English in school at comparable ages between 4 and 13 years and who had used it for over 12 years. One group (n = 14) was born profoundly deaf and had had little language experience before being exposed to ASL in school; the second group (n = 13) was born profoundly deaf and had experienced ASL in infancy; the third group (n = 13) was born hearing and had experienced various spoken languages in infancy (Urdu, French, German, Italian or Greek). Deaf and hearing adults who had experienced either a signed or a spoken language in early life showed similarly high levels of performance on the later learned language, English, whereas deaf adults who had little experience of language in early life showed low levels of performance (Fig. 1; F2,37 = 11.32, P < 0.0001).

Our results show that the ability to learn language arises from a synergy between early brain development and language experience, and is seriously compromised when language is not experienced during early life. This is consistent with current knowledge about how experience affects visual development in animals9 and humans10, and about learning and brain development in animals11,12. The timing of the initial language experience during human development strongly influences the capacity to learn language throughout life, regardless of the sensorimotor form of the early experience.