Main

In animal communication, colouring is a compromise between being conspicuous to conspecifics and being poorly visible to predators or prey1. Female crab-spiders adapt their entire body colour to that of the flowers on which they are trying to hide, a behaviour that is presumed to conceal them from predators and from the visiting pollinators that constitute their main prey2.

To appear cryptic to both predators and prey, these spiders must precisely match the flower colour in their respective ranges of colour vision: four cone types, corresponding to ultraviolet (UV)–blue–green–red, for birds, and three (UV–blue–green) for insects3,4. However, these visual systems differ markedly in their range of sensitivity and number of photoreceptors, making a precise colour match for both unlikely.

We used spectroradiometry to measure the degree of matching (crypsis) of T. onustus collected on the corollae of two flower species (Senecio jacobea and Mentha spicata) growing around Tours, France. To analyse spider crypsis with respect to potential predators and prey on each flower, we computed and compared colour distances in the respective colour spaces of birds and Hymenoptera (see supplementary information).

For birds, spiders matched pink Mentha corollae when viewed through the four-cone colour-vision system (Fig. 1; χ2=9, d.f.=1, P<0.01). Likewise, on Senecio, each spider matched the individual yellow corolla on which it was waiting for prey (χ2=7.5, d.f.=1, P<0.01). But on the Senecio UV–yellow petal background, spiders produced a strong colour contrast that birds were likely to detect (χ2=2.7, d.f.=1, P>0.05). For Hymenoptera, spiders matched the blue-green colour of Mentha corollae in the three-cone colour-vision system (χ2=4, d.f.=1, P<0.05). They also effectively mimicked the individual blue-green colour of Senecio corollae (χ2=7.5, d.f.=1, P<0.01), but contrasted on UV–green petals when seen by Hymenoptera (χ2=2.7, d.f.=1, P>0.05).

Figure 1: Colour contrast of spiders (mean euclidean distances ± s.e.m.) against Mentha corollae (pink bars), Senecio corollae (yellow bars) and Senecio petals (white bars) when viewed by birds and Hymenoptera.
figure 1

Dashed lines indicate thresholds for colour-contrast detection calculated for birds and Hymenoptera. For details of modelling calculations, see supplementary information.

To detect small targets, birds and bees can use achromatic vision instead of colour contrast5,6. Spiders were significantly brighter than corollae of Mentha (repeated-measures ANOVA, birds: F1,17=8.5, P<0.001; Hymenoptera: F1,17=8.6, P<0.001) and Senecio (birds: F1,14=35.7, P<0.001; Hymenoptera: F1,14=46.2, P<0.001), but were darker than Senecio petals (birds: F1,14=133.7, P<0.001; Hymenoptera: F1,14=157.3, P<0.001).

Our results indicate that crab-spiders' colour mimicry works successfully on the visual systems of both predator and prey, achromatic vision being potentially more efficient under particular viewing conditions. This aggressive mimicry may vary from species to species, as shown by Misumena vatia7, a crab-spider that reduces its chromatic contrast to bees on white flowers (as in Fig. 2), as does T. onustus, but is also able to reduce its achromatic contrast on yellow flowers.

Figure 2
figure 2

Master of disguise: a crab-spider (left), concealed against a flower's white petals, preys on an unsuspecting bee.

Competing financial interests: declared none.